Running head: REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 1. whether they should be considered separate or unique disorders in the next DSM. withdrawn types.

Similar documents
REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER. Critical Analysis and Case Presentation Ashley Woods Community Health of Central Washington March 2, 2018

Annual Research Review: Attachment disorders in early childhood clinical presentation, causes, correlates, and treatment

Chapter 1. General introduction

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER: A REVIEW FOR DSM-V. Charles H. Zeanah, M.D. Mary Margaret Gleason, M.D. Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences

Descriptions of clinical syndromes in very NEW RESEARCH

Reactive attachment disorder (RAD) was first introduced as

Chapter 5. General Discussion and Summary

Review Article Maltreatment-Associated Psychiatric Problems: An Example of Environmentally Triggered ESSENCE?

Incidence and Comorbidity of Reactive Attachment Disorder: Based on National Health Insurance Claims Data, in Korea

Index. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

Research Article Reactive Attachment Disorder in the General Population: A Hidden ESSENCE Disorder

Predictors of Disruptive Behavior, Developmental Delays, Anxiety, and Affective Symptomatology Among Institutionally Reared Romanian Children

McLaughlin, K. A., Fox, N. A., Zeanah, C. H., Sheridan, M. A., Marshall, P., & Nelson, C. A. (2010). Delayed maturation in brain electrical activity

Stine Lehmann 1,2,5 Odd E. Havik 1

Disinhibited Attachment Disorder in UK Adopted Children During Middle Childhood: Prevalence, Validity and Possible Developmental Origin

Maltreatment and mental health

A copy can be downloaded for personal non-commercial research or study, without prior permission or charge

Attachment disorders versus more common problems in looked after and adopted children: comparing community and expert assessments

INSTITUTIONALIZED AUTISM AND THE ADOPTED CHILD CHRISTINE PETERSON. Integrated Studies Final Project Essay (MAIS 700) submitted to Dr.

Foster care promotes adaptive functioning in early adolescence among children who experienced severe, early deprivation

CRITICAL PERIODS IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF ATTACHMENT: LESSONS FROM THE BUCHAREST EARLY INTERVENTION PROJECT

(2015) : ISSN

Effects of early psychosocial deprivation on the development of memory and executive function

NEUROIMAGING AND BEHAVIORAL DYSFUNCTIONS IN INSTITUTIONALIZED CHILDREN

Inattention/Overactivity Following Early Severe Institutional Deprivation: Presentation and Associations in Early Adolescence

An exploratory study of the association between reactive attachment disorder and attachment narratives in early school-age children

3/9/2017. Diagnostic Classification of Mental Health and Developmental Disorders of Infancy and Early Childhood: An Overview of DC:0-5

EARLY CHILDHOOD PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Biological and Experimental Psychology School of Biological and Chemical Sciences Individual Differences in Environmental Sensitivity

Residential Childcare WorkersÕ Knowledge of Reactive Attachment Disorder

DIFFERENTIAL SUSCEPTIBILITY TO ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES: From Longitudinal Research to Intervention Part B. Jay Belsky

Preventing ACEs Finding our gems and polishing them

EMOTION REGULATION & EMOTION DYSREGULATION + CHILD-CAREGIVER ATTACHMENT & REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER. Diane Benoit, MD, FRCPC

Deviations from the Expectable Environment in Early Childhood and Emerging Psychopathology

Mental disorders in foster children: a study of prevalence, comorbidity and risk factors

Early Childhood Mental Health

Psychological consequences of early global deprivation: An overview of findings from the English & Romanian Adoptees Study

Does the social environment have a role to play in ADHD? Edmund Sonuga-Barke King s College London

Trauma and Child Attachment Representation The Manchester Child Attachment Story Task (MCAST)

Current Directions in the Study of Risk And Adversity in Early Childhood

Physiology & Behavior

A. The Broad Continuum of Attention Problems

9/7/2016. DC: 0-3R Revision. Diagnostic and Classification Revision Task Force

Factors related to neuropsychological deficits in ADHD children

Recognition and Assessment of Anxiety & Depression in Early Childhood

Smith ScholarWorks. Carla Gerber-Weintraub. Theses, Dissertations, and Projects

Something Has To Change

Welcome. Rogers treats children, adolescents and adults with: Anxiety disorders Eating disorders Mood disorders Substance use disorders

Scientists, educators, and clinicians have long been interested in the effects

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

A Longitudinal Study of the Achievements Progress and Attitudes of Severely Inattentive, Hyperactive and Impulsive Young Children

Parent-child interactions and children with autism: A novel perspective

(2007) ISSN

Minnesota DC:0-3R Crosswalk to ICD codes

COMORBIDITIES ASSOCIATED WITH ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER IN PRESCHOOLERS

Supporting Families to Build Secure Attachment Relationships : Comments on Benoit, Dozier, and Egeland

Diagnosis Advancements. Licensee OAPL (UK) Creative Commons Attribution License (CC-BY) Research study

Average length/number of sessions: 50

The Diagnosis and Treatment of Trauma and Stressor Related Disorders. Eileen E. Joy & Cynthia L. Turk. Washburn University.

Creating and Sustaining a Trauma Informed Approach. Re n e e D i e t c h m a n L e s l i e W i s s

Background Paper: Shy Children. Briana Jackson. University of Pittsburgh. December 2011

AMBIANCE Training Workshops

ISTSS Guidelines Position Paper on Complex PTSD in Children and Adolescents

References to Relevant Papers. ADOS Standardisation / Psychometrics. BeginningwithA

CHARACTERISTICS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DISORDERS IN THE CLINICAL POPULATION OF A MENTAL HEALTH CENTER

Autism Diagnosis as a Social Process

No conflicts of interest

The Effects of Early Institutionalization on the Discrimination of Facial Expressions of Emotion in Young Children

Abstract. Author. Costanza Colombi. Keywords: Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD), Early Intervention, Challenges

Aspects of clinical symptomatology of preschool abused children. Applications of a program that aims to improve the children s socio-emotional skills.

Researchers probe genetic overlap between ADHD, autism

Guidelines for Making Changes to DSM-V Revised 10/21/09 Kenneth Kendler, David Kupfer, William Narrow, Katharine Phillips, Jan Fawcett,

Citation for published version (APA): Parigger, E. M. (2012). Language and executive functioning in children with ADHD Den Bosch: Boxpress

Closing Date for Comments: 23 November 2007, 5pm AEST

Autism rates in the United States explained

Introducing a New Classification of Early Childhood Disorders: DC:0 5

THE EFFECTS OF ADVERSITY ON BRAIN AND BEHAVIORAL DEVELOPMENT: LESSONS FROM THE BUCHAREST EARLY INTERVENTION PROJECT

ADHD and Comorbid Conditions A Conceptual Model

FROM BIOLOGY TO BEHAVIOR TO THE LAW: POLICY IMPLICATIONS OF THE NEUROBIOLOGY OF EARLY ADVERSE EXPERIENCES

QUT Digital Repository:

DSM Comparison Chart DSM-5 (Revisions in bold)

Autism Spectrum Conditions

Below are five evaluative statements about the Romanian orphan research. Which two statements are correct?

Early Adverse Experiences and the Neurobiology of Facial Emotion Processing

Autism Diagnosis as a Social Process

Autism/Pervasive Developmental Disorders Update. Kimberly Macferran, MD Pediatric Subspecialty for the Primary Care Provider December 2, 2011

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

Editorial: DSM-5 and autism spectrum disorders two decades of perspectives from the JCPP

CONDUCT DISORDER. 1. Introduction. 2. DSM-IV Criteria. 3. Treating conduct disorder

Childhood ADHD is a risk factor for some Psychiatric Disorders and co-morbidities

Introduction. SPPAHI or also known as Skala Penilaian Perilaku. Anak Hiperaktif is an instrument to evaluate if there is

Strategic Application of Human Development Applying Positive Psychology. The Principles of Developmental Psychology

INFORMATION PAPER: INTRODUCING THE NEW DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

ENHANCING POSITIVE CHANGE FOR THE DEVELOPING BRAIN

The Impact of Adverse Childhood Experiences on Psychopathology and Suicidal Behaviour in the Northern Ireland Population

NATIONAL INSTITUTE FOR HEALTH AND CLINICAL EXCELLENCE SCOPE. Personality Disorder: the clinical management of borderline personality disorder

The Resilient Revolution takes on

Child-Parent Psychotherapy Research Fact Sheet OVERVIEW

Timing of Intervention Affects Brain Electrical Activity in Children Exposed to Severe Psychosocial Neglect

Transcription:

Running head: REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 1 Critically evaluate the characteristics of two overlapping clinical conditions justifying whether they should be considered separate or unique disorders in the next DSM. Reactive Attachment Disorder: Indiscriminately social disinhibited and emotionally withdrawn types. Word Count: 1998

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 2 Abstract Reactive Attachment Disorder (RAD) was classified as a single disorder with two subtypes in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, up until the release of the fifth edition of the manual (DSM 5, 2013). With the release of this edition, RAD: emotionally withdrawn type and RAD: disinhibited type became two separate disorders. They are now known as RAD (formally RAD: withdrawn type) and Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder (DSED; formally RAD: disinhibited type). This review evaluates the decision to separate these two disorders, concluding that the majority of evidence is behind the change. Problems which remain in the DSM-5 criteria for these disorders are discussed, and improvements for subsequent editions of the DSM are proposed. Key Words: Attachment Disorders; Child Development; Child Neglect; Diagnostic Criteria; Institutionalised Children

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 3 Reactive Attachment Disorder: Indiscriminately social disinhibited and emotionally withdrawn types. Diagnostic criteria for Reactive Attachment Disorder (RAD) include: emotionally withdrawn behaviour; social emotional disturbance; and exposure to extremes of insufficient care. Diagnostic criteria for Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder (DSED) include: reduced/absent reticence when interacting with unfamiliar adults; socially disinhibited behaviours; and exposure to extremes of insufficient care (Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5), American Psychiatric Association (APA), 2013). Despite the very different symptoms, these two disorders were considered to be subtypes of a single construct; RAD: emotionally withdrawn type (now RAD), and RAD: disinhibited type (now DSED) (DSM III R, APA, 1987; DSM-IV, APA, 1994; DSM-IV-R, APA, 2000) until very recently. With the release of the DSM-5 in 2013 (APA), they now sit as separate disorders, bringing the DSM-V in line with both: the ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioural Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines (ICD-10; World Health Organization, 1992); and a vast body of research evidence (Zeanah & Gleason, 2010). The reasons for this decision, and why it should be maintained in the next DSM, will be discussed along with the relevant research evidence. Some key issues with the current diagnostic criteria will also be highlighted, in order to allow for further improvements to the next DSM. There is much research support for the greater separation of RAD and DSED in the DSM-5. For example, using factor analysis methods on attachment questionnaire items, Minnis et al (2007) found a distinct factor corresponding to each disorder, suggesting the two are in fact separate constructs. Similarly, O Connor, Bredenkamp & Rutter, (1999) found that on analysis of questionnaire responses,

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 4 disinhibited items were more closely associated with the other disinhibited items than they were with the inhibited item. This again supports a distinction between the two. There are a number of key differences between RAD and DSED; one of these is the prevalence following adoption. Whilst disinhibited social behaviour persists after adoption and can still be seen in children aged 11 (Rutter et al, 2007), this is not the case for the inhibited type. In the Bucharest Study (Nelson et al, 2007) prior to fostering 24 out of 56 children showed marked inhibited RAD; this figure reduced to just 1 case after fostering. Further, whilst the relationship between caregiving and RAD is well documented, the same cannot be said for DSED. Zeanah et al (2005) found moderate associations between caregiving and signs of RAD, but no association between caregiving and signs of DSED. Similarly, RAD responds well to enhanced caregiving, with symptoms reducing once children are placed in a more remedial environment; in contrast, enhanced caregiving is not as effective at reducing DSED (Rutter et al, 2009). These findings have implications for treatment; because DSED is very persistent and unresponsive to enhanced caregiving, it is difficult to provide intervention to these children. RAD shares some features with autistic spectrum disorders (Zeanah & Smyke, 2008), and also shows moderate convergence with depressed mood (Gleason et al, 2011), suggesting it might be described as an internalising disorder. In contrast, DSED is more similar to externalising disorders such as Attention Deficit Hyperactive Disorder (ADHD), as it shows moderate associations with measures of inattention and hyperactivity (Roy, Rutter & Pickles, 2004); this highlights a further distinction between the disorders. Finally, one of the most crucial differences between RAD and DSED is their relationship to attachment behaviour, as measured by the strange situation (SS) paradigm. RAD occurs when no selective attachment has been formed

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 5 (Gleason et al, 2011), thus when measured using the SS, children with RAD are often rated as unclassified due to not displaying any attachment behaviours (Zeanah et al, 2005). In contrast, those with DSED may or may not have selective attachments, and even those who are classified as secure on the SS can still display indiscriminate behaviour (Chisholm, 1998; O Connor et al, 2003). Further, Zeanah et al (2005) found moderate associations between RAD and SS ratings of attachment behaviour, but no relationship between DSED and these ratings. Findings such as these have led to debate about what the core deficit of DSED actually is. One interesting question raised by the relationship of DSED to ADHD is whether the two disorders form a unitary post institutional syndrome, or if they are just commonly comorbid (Zeanah & Smyke, 2008). Further, some have argued that DSED should be seen as something other than an attachment disorder (Gleason et al, 2011). In fact, the new name disinhibited social engagement disorder is intended to reflect that it is not necessarily attachment behaviours that are disinhibited, but social behaviours more generally (Zeanah & Gleason, 2010). If the core of this disorder is not attachment behaviour, DSED is even more distinct from RAD than was previously thought. Due to this uncertainty, it makes sense for the two to stand as separate disorders. In summary, evidence is completely against the view that the two disorders are part of the same construct (Rutter, Kreppner & Sonuga-Barke, 2009). Compared to this vast number of differences, the similarities between the two disorders are few and far between. One similarity is the aetiology; it is specified in the DSM criteria that both disorders are a result of pathological care. Even this is problematic, however. The disorders are rare, even among maltreated children (Zeanah & Smyke, 2008); if not all children faced with pathological care develop them, there must be additional risk factors (Hanson & Spratt, 2010). Current research

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 6 is examining the role of genetics (Minnis et al, 2007); in particular, a potential role for 5HTT (Kumsta et al, 2010; Bakermans-Kranenburg, Dobrova-Krol & van IJzendoorn, 2012). It has also been questioned how two disorders with such different characteristics can develop from the same early environment (Zeanah & Smyke, 2008). Hinshaw-Fusilier, Boris and Zeanah (1999), for example, report a case study of two dizygotic twins, who grew up in the same family home and were placed in foster care at 18 months. Despite the shared early environment, one twin displayed symptoms of RAD, the other one displayed symptoms of DSED. Zeanah and Fox (2004) suggest that temperamental dispositions (e.g. withdrawn inhibited behaviour/ negative emotionality or exuberance/impulsivity) may interact with a neglectful caregiving environment and explain the development of the different disorders. Thus, it is likely that there are different risk factors for both RAD and DSED, leaving them with only a partly shared aetiology. There is one key argument for RAD and DSED being returned to subtypes of the same construct: the potential of a mixed subtype. An alternative classification system for RAD exists; this appears in the Research Diagnostic Criteria - Preschool Age (RDC-PA; AACAP, 2002) and the Diagnostic Criteria 0-3R (DC: 0-3R; Zero to Three, 2005). These alternative criteria still consider RAD as one disorder; however, they include three separate subtypes: Inhibited, disinhibited, and a mixed type which shares features of both inhibited and disinhibited. These criteria are based on studies showing moderate intercorrelations between RAD and DSED; these same studies also used cluster analyses which suggested a mixed type of RAD (Zeanah et al, 2004). However, there are a number of reasons why including a mixed subtype in the next DSM would be a premature decision. Firstly, there has never been a case report of a child with features of both the types (Zeanah & Gleason, 2010). In addition, when the

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 7 data from the studies was subjected to closer inspection, it was found that the items most used to code indiscriminate behaviour (for example, willingness to go off with a stranger, failure to check back with caregivers in unfamiliar settings) can also be applied to children with emotionally withdrawn behaviour. What is important in distinguishing between the two is the passivity of the behaviour; those with indiscriminate behaviour will seek interaction with other adults, whilst those with withdrawn behaviour may simply act in these ways due to a lack of regard for any particular caregiver (Zeanah & Gleason, 2010). Therefore, before any changes are made to the DSM based on the possibility of a mixed type, controlled studies with coding taking into account not only the types of behaviours, but also the passivity with which they are performed are needed. As of yet, there is insufficient validity data for a mixed type of RAD. Therefore, the majority of evidence suggests that the DSM 5 is an improvement on previous versions. However, there are other problems that remain to be addressed relating to the disorders. This is hardly surprising, as the DSM criteria have largely been developed without research evidence (Zeanah & Gleason, 2010); RAD was first included in the DSM III in 1980 (APA), but the first study addressing the validity of the criteria was not until 1998 (Boris, Zeanah, Larrieu, Scheeringa & Heller). This has left some areas in desperate need of research attention. One issue is the rate of diagnosis, with concerns that children are being over-diagnosed in institutions (Hanson & Spratt, 2000), and under-diagnosed in the general population (e.g. Pritchett, Pritchett, Marshall, Davidson & Minnis, 2013). Research that tried to address RAD/DSED in the general population faces a number of problems, such as poor response rate. Minnis et al, (2013) only received a 67.5% parental response rate in their study of RAD in a deprived population. Additionally, diagnosis of this

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 8 disorder is a sensitive issue. Due to the requirement of pathological care, this would be a difficult diagnosis for parents to deal with, and for psychologists to give. For these reasons, it is believed RAD/DSED often goes undetected in this population. On the other hand, researchers have expressed concern that within children with a history of maltreatment (e.g. neglect; sexual abuse) the disorder is over-diagnosed. In these cases, it is assumed that any behavioural problems observed are a result of the maladaptive relationships with their caregivers, with other possible causes not being considered, leading to a diagnosis of RAD/DSED (Hanson & Spratt, 2000). A further issue is that little is known about the developmental course. The current cut off age of 5 is arbitrary (Hanson & Spratt, 2000). This age is not based on any form of research evidence, and due to this, cases of RAD/DSED which are not identified until after the age of 5 could miss out on a diagnosis, affecting their chances of treatment. To conclude, it is clear that despite having some degree of shared cause, these two disorders are very different, and should remain separate in the next DSM. The vast number of differences between the disorders, shown in the majority of research, completely nullifies the idea they belong to the same construct. The only reason for this decision to be reconsidered in the future would be if evidence for a mixed subtype was found. This evidence would need to be methodologically sound and replicable in order to counter the existing support for the separation of the disorders. The main problem with RAD at present is not the issue of whether the two types are distinct; this issue has received plenty of research attention, whilst issues with the diagnostic criteria in general have received comparatively little. It is important that we consider how to identify and treat hidden cases of RAD/DSED in the general population, without causing more cases of over-diagnosis in institutionalised children. Due to the rarity of the disorders, and the criteria changing often, some clinicians may not be

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 9 very familiar with them (Hanson & Spratt, 2010); therefore raising awareness of RAD and DSED could be a solution to the problems with diagnosis. Further, the validity of the current age of onset requirement of 5 years needs to be looked into. In summary, the RAD criteria are still a work in progress, but they are improving with each edition of the DSM and have come a long way since their first appearance in 1980. The decision to separate them in the DSM 5 has only improved the criteria further. Hopefully by the next edition of the DSM there will be more research on the areas outlined, allowing for further improvements.

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 10 References AACAP Task Force on Research Diagnostic Criteria: Infancy and Preschool Age (2003). Research diagnostic criteria for infants and preschool children: the process and empirical support. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 1504-1512. American Psychiatric Association (1980), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition (DSM-III). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association (1987). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition--Revised (DSM-III-R). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition, (DSM-5). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing. Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Dobrova-Krol, N., & van IJzendoorn, M. (2012). Impact of institutional care on attachment disorganization and insecurity of Ukrainian preschoolers Protective effect of the long variant of the serotonin transporter gene (5HTT). International Journal of Behavioral Development, 36(1), 11-18. Boris, N. W., Zeanah, C. H., Larrieu, J. A., Scheeringa, M. S., & Heller, S. S. (1998). Attachment disorders in infancy and early childhood: A preliminary

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 11 investigation of diagnostic criteria. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(2), 295-297. Chisholm, K. (1998). A Three Year Follow up of Attachment and Indiscriminate Friendliness in Children Adopted from Romanian Orphanages. Child Development, 69(4), 1092-1106. Gleason, M. M., Fox, N. A., Drury, S., Smyke, A., Egger, H. L., Nelson III, C. A., Gregas, M.C., & Zeanah, C. H. (2011). Validity of evidence-derived criteria for reactive attachment disorder: Indiscriminately social/disinhibited and emotionally withdrawn/inhibited types. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 50(3), 216-231. Hinshaw-Fuselier, S., Boris, N. W., & Zeanah, C. H. (1999). Reactive attachment disorder in maltreated twins. Infant Mental Health Journal, 20(1), 42-59. Kumsta, R., Stevens, S., Brookes, K., Schlotz, W., Castle, J., Beckett, C., Kreppner, J., Rutter, M., & Sonuga- Barke, E. (2010). 5HTT genotype moderates the influence of early institutional deprivation on emotional problems in adolescence: evidence from the English and Romanian Adoptee (ERA) study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(7), 755-762. Minnis, H., Macmillan, S., Pritchett, R., Young, D., Wallace, B., Butcher, J., Sim, F., Baynham, K., Davidson, C., & Gillberg, C. (2013). Prevalence of reactive attachment disorder in a deprived population. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 202(5), 342-346. Minnis, H., Reekie, J., Young, D., O Connor, T., Ronald, A., Gray, A., et al. (2007). Genetic, environmental and gender influences on attachment disorder behaviours. British Journal of Psychiatry, 190,490-495.

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 12 Nelson, C. A., Zeanah, C. H., Fox, N. A., Marshall, P. J., Smyke, A. T., & Guthrie, D. (2007). Cognitive recovery in socially deprived young children: The Bucharest Early Intervention Project. Science, 318(5858), 1937-1940. O Connor T.G., Bredenkamp, D. & Rutter, M. (1999), Attachment disturbances and disorders in children exposed to early severe deprivation. Infant Mental Health Journal, 20, 10-29. O Connor, T.G., Marvin, R.S., Rutter, M., Olrick, J.T., Britner, P.A. (2003), Childparent attachment following early institutional deprivation. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 19-38. Pritchett, R., Pritchett, J., Marshall, E., Davidson, C., & Minnis, H. (2013). Reactive attachment disorder in the general population: a hidden ESSENCE disorder. The Scientific World Journal, 2013. Roy, P., Rutter, M., & Pickles, A. (2004). Institutional care: associations between overactivity and lack of selectivity in social relationships. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 866-873. Rutter, M., Colvert, E., Kreppner, J., Beckett, C., Castle, J., Groothues, C., Hawkins, A, O Connor, T.G., Stevens, S.E., & Sonuga Barke, E. J. (2007). Early adolescent outcomes for institutionally deprived and non deprived adoptees. I: Disinhibited attachment. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 48(1), 17-30. Rutter, M., Kreppner, J., & Sonuga Barke, E. (2009). Emanuel Miller Lecture: Attachment insecurity, disinhibited attachment, and attachment disorders: where do research findings leave the concepts? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(5), 529-543. Tizard B. & Rees J (1975). The effect of early institutional rearing on the behaviour

REACTIVE ATTACHMENT DISORDER 13 problems and affectional relationships of four-year-old children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 16, 61-73. World Health Organization (1992). The ICD-10 Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders: Clinical Descriptions and Diagnostic Guidelines. Geneva, World Health Organization. Zeanah, C. H., & Gleason, M. M. (2010). Reactive attachment disorder: A review for DSM-V. Report presented to the American Psychiatric Association. Zeanah, C.H., Scheeringa, M.S., Boris, N.W., Heller, S.S., Smyke, A.T. Trapani, J. (2004). Reactive attachment disorder in maltreated toddlers. Child Abuse and Neglect: The International Journal, 28, 877-888. Zeanah, C.H. & Smyke, A.T. (2008). Attachment disorders in relation to deprivation. In M. Rutter, D. Bishop, D. Pine, S. Scott, J. Stevenson, E. Taylor, & A. Thapar (Eds.), Rutter s child and adolescent psychiatry, pp. 906-915. Oxford, Blackwell. Zeanah, C. H., Smyke, A. T., Koga, S. F. M., & Carlson, E. the BEIP Core Group. (2005). Attachment in institutionalized and community children in Romania. Child Development, 76(5), 1015-1028. Zero To Three (2005). Diagnostic classification of mental health and developmental disorders of infancy and early childhood: Revised edition (DC: 0--3R). Washington, D.C., Zero To Three Press.