EEG-Rhythm Dynamics during a 2-back Working Memory Task and Performance

Similar documents
Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

A Brain Computer Interface System For Auto Piloting Wheelchair

Do children with ADHD and/or PDD-NOS differ in reactivity of alpha/theta ERD/ERS to manipulations of cognitive load and stimulus relevance?

Novel single trial movement classification based on temporal dynamics of EEG

Simultaneous Real-Time Detection of Motor Imagery and Error-Related Potentials for Improved BCI Accuracy

Prestimulus Alpha as a Precursor to Errors in a UAV Target Orientation Detection Task

The EEG Analysis of Auditory Emotional Stimuli Perception in TBI Patients with Different SCG Score

Attentional Blink Paradigm

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

WAVELET ENERGY DISTRIBUTIONS OF P300 EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS FOR WORKING MEMORY PERFORMANCE IN CHILDREN

Brain Computer Interface. Mina Mikhail

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

BIOPAC Systems, Inc BIOPAC Inspiring people and enabling discovery about life

An Analysis of Improving Memory Performance Based on EEG Alpha and Theta Oscillations

Brain oscillatory 4 30 Hz responses during a visual n-back memory task with varying memory load

REHEARSAL PROCESSES IN WORKING MEMORY AND SYNCHRONIZATION OF BRAIN AREAS

Human Brain Institute Russia-Switzerland-USA

Brain wave synchronization and entrainment to periodic acoustic stimuli

Event Related Potentials: Significant Lobe Areas and Wave Forms for Picture Visual Stimulus

Synchronous cortical gamma-band activity in task-relevant cognition

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

Processed by HBI: Russia/Switzerland/USA

Physiology Unit 2 CONSCIOUSNESS, THE BRAIN AND BEHAVIOR

From Single-trial EEG to Brain Area Dynamics

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Discrimination of EEG-Based Motor Imagery Tasks by Means of a Simple Phase Information Method

Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

CHAPTER 6 INTERFERENCE CANCELLATION IN EEG SIGNAL

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

Neurophysiologically Driven Image Triage: A Pilot Study

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

Modifying the Classic Peak Picking Technique Using a Fuzzy Multi Agent to Have an Accurate P300-based BCI

TEMPORAL CHARACTERISTICS OF THE MEMORY CODE

AUXILIARIES AND NEUROPLASTICITY

Northeast Center for Special Care Grant Avenue Lake Katrine, NY

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION 2. ARTIFACT REJECTION ON RAW DATA

Top-Down versus Bottom-up Processing in the Human Brain: Distinct Directional Influences Revealed by Integrating SOBI and Granger Causality

EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition timing hypothesis

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves

Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information

Neuro Q no.2 = Neuro Quotient

Decisions Have Consequences

In what way does the parietal ERP old new effect index recollection?

EEG Event-Related Desynchronization of patients with stroke during motor imagery of hand movement


Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

EEG changes accompanying learned regulation of 12-Hz EEG activity

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

To link to this article: PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Transcranial direct current stimulation modulates shifts in global/local attention

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neurosci Lett. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 March 13.

Working Memory Impairments Limitations of Normal Children s in Visual Stimuli using Event-Related Potentials

Functional Connectivity and the Neurophysics of EEG. Ramesh Srinivasan Department of Cognitive Sciences University of California, Irvine

Seizure onset can be difficult to asses in scalp EEG. However, some tools can be used to increase the seizure onset activity over the EEG background:

The role of phase synchronization in memory processes

The Sonification of Human EEG and other Biomedical Data. Part 3

Electroencephalography

Abnormal late visual responses and alpha oscillations in neurofibromatosis type 1: a link to visual and attention deficits

Lesson 5 EEG 1 Electroencephalography: Brain Rhythms

Physiological and Physical Basis of Functional Brain Imaging 6. EEG/MEG. Kâmil Uludağ, 20. November 2007

Emotion Detection Using Physiological Signals. M.A.Sc. Thesis Proposal Haiyan Xu Supervisor: Prof. K.N. Plataniotis

Spectral Analysis of EEG Patterns in Normal Adults

Support Vector Machine Classification and Psychophysiological Evaluation of Mental Workload and Engagement of Intuition- and Analysis-Inducing Tasks

The role of theta oscillations in memory and decision making

Classification of EEG signals in an Object Recognition task

Multiscale Evidence of Multiscale Brain Communication

The Effects of a Single Session of Upper Alpha Neurofeedback for Cognitive Enhancement: A Sham-Controlled Study

Neurophysiologically Driven Image Triage: A Pilot Study

ISSN: (Online) Volume 3, Issue 7, July 2015 International Journal of Advance Research in Computer Science and Management Studies

Beyond the Basics in EEG Interpretation: Throughout the Life Stages

Power-Based Connectivity. JL Sanguinetti

Visual Selection and Attention

From single-trial EEG to brain area dynamics

Practicalities of EEG Measurement and Experiment Design YATING LIU

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

NeuroReport 2013, 24: a Section on Child and Family Research, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

Event-related alpha and theta responses in a visuo-spatial working memory task

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG

ANALYSIS OF EEG FOR MOTOR IMAGERY BASED CLASSIFICATION OF HAND ACTIVITIES

Normal brain rhythms and the transition to epileptic activity

EEG in the ICU: Part I

Electrophysiological, behavioral, and subjective indexes of workload when performing multiple tasks: manipulations of task difficulty and training

The role of selective attention in visual awareness of stimulus features: Electrophysiological studies

Keywords: EEG, adaptive classification, single-trial spectral EEG analysis, spectral patterns, memory task, pre-/post-stimulus intervals, ERD/ERS

EEG/ERP Control Process Separation

The error-related potential and BCIs

Threat modulates neural responses to looming visual stimuli

EEG History. Where and why is EEG used? 8/2/2010

Brain and Cognition. Cognitive Neuroscience. If the brain were simple enough to understand, we would be too stupid to understand it

Matrix Energetics Research Brainwaves and Heart waves Research on Matrix Energetics in Action

Working with EEG/ERP data. Sara Bögels Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics

BCIA NEUROFEEDBACK CERTIFICATION PROGRAM

Transcription:

EEG-Rhythm Dynamics during a 2-back Working Memory Task and Performance Tsvetomira Tsoneva, Davide Baldo, Victor Lema and Gary Garcia-Molina Abstract Working memory is an essential component of human cognition and determines to a large extent an individual s intellectual ability. In this paper, the human brain oscillatory response system associated with working memory performance is evaluated in an experimental and analysis setting involving volunteers performing a visual 2-back task. Event-related dynamics in three bands: theta (3. - 7 Hz), alpha (7. - 12 Hz) and upper beta (17-29 Hz) at 32 locations distributed over the scalp are examined analyzing the event-related desynchronization (ERD)/synchronization (ERS) in these bands. Both global dynamics as well as trial- and subject-specific trends were considered. The overall across participants trend shows that the theta level synchronizes during working memory engagement, whereas beta and alpha desynchronizes. While common features seem to emerge, different subjects exhibit equally significant but opposite in direction correlation between reaction time and power dynamics. I. INTRODUCTION Working memory (WM) is the brain system involved in the temporary storage and manipulation of information and coordination of resources needed for various cognitive processes such as learning, reasoning and language comprehension [1]. It has been suggested that various selectively distributed oscillatory systems control the integrative brain functions at all sensory and cognitive levels, playing an important role in functional communication in the brain [2]. Scalp recorded electroencephalogram (EEG) can reveal the functional role and the interaction between the different oscillatory systems involved in various mental states and processes. Frequency specific changes of the ongoing EEG activity reflect the decrease or increase in synchrony of the underlying neuronal populations and manifest as decreases or increases of power in given EEG frequency bands [3]. When those changes are time-locked to a specific event they are called the eventrelated desynchronization (ERD) or synchronization (ERS). The frequency band most often linked to working memory and mental effort in general is the theta band (4-8 Hz) [4]. Increases in theta band power have been related to episodic memory demands []. Increases in theta oscillatory responses also reflect different memory load [6], [7] and task demands [8]. Correlations between theta band synchronization and performance have also been reported [9]. Alpha rhythms (8-12 Hz) often behave in the opposite way. Increasing task demands are associated with decrease in alpha power [], Philips Research Europe, High Tech Campus 34, 66AE Eindhoven, The Netherlands tsvetomira.tsoneva@philips.com [7]. High tonic alpha band power levels are also significantly correlated with increased performance [9]. Beta oscillations (12-3 Hz) initially related to motor processes, lately have also been found in association with cognitive processing. Early appearing beta ERD has been observed, becoming longer with increased memory load []. A majority of the studies studying event-related dynamics rely on trial averaging, which assumes that most trials express single mode of activity time-locked to an event of interest plus some background activity. As [4] suggests, however, different modes of activity, due to task-unrelated mental effort or specific performance strategies for example, may occur in different trials. Therefore, the trial variability may be reflecting other factors accompanying the task execution. Thus, it is important to study the event-related brain dynamics on a per trial basis and try to identify possible features that would explain the inter-trial variability. Especially interesting is the question how differences in performance are manifested in the brain dynamics modulations. The goal of this study is to evaluate the human brain oscillatory response system associated with working memory performance. Considering the rhythmic nature of the signals, we go beyond the studies that use event-related potentials (ERPs) and investigate the event-related power changes in three bands corresponding to the classical theta, alpha, and upper beta frequency bands both as global and trialspecific dynamics. We also study the link between the neurophysiological responses and their behavioral correlates, performance in particular. This paper is organized as follows. First, we introduce our methods in Section II. Section III explains the data analysis and Section IV presents the results and discussion. We conclude the paper in Section V. A. Participants II. METHODS Ten healthy volunteers ( males and females) in their twenties (average age 24 years) with normal or corrected to normal vision participated in the study. To minimize the effect of hemispheric biases, only right handed subjects were considered for participation. Half of the participants were asked to perform the experiment in the morning and half in the afternoon. All participants signed an informed consent and were rewarded for their participation at the end of the study.

B. Experimental paradigm In recent years the n-back task has been employed in many studies to investigate working memory processing [11]. During the n-back task the subjects are presented with a series of items appearing on a screen one at a time. They are asked to decide whether each item in the sequence matches the one that was presented n-items ago. In our study we used the 2-back letter version of this task where the participants had to decide whether a letter currently presented on the screen is the same as the one presented two letters earlier (see Figure 1). The used test set consisted of 8 letters, namely B, F, K, H, M, Q, R and X. Vowels were not included in order to prevent semantic associations. The selected letters did not have common shape features to avoid errors due to shape confusions (e.g. V and W). Each letter was presented on the computer screen for milliseconds. A fixation cross appeared on the screen between trials (a letter presentation) for a random duration between and 3 milliseconds. When a trial was a target (the letter was the same as the one 2 letters ago), the participants had to press the button 1. In all other cases, the participants had to press the button 2. Fig. 1. Experimental paradigm: 2-back task. The task was administered using the E-Prime application suite [12]. The participants had to complete 3 sessions of 3 trials ( targets) each. Before starting they had a chance to practice the task. Behavioral performance (reaction time and accuracy) was recorded. The total duration of the experiment was around 4 minutes, including 1 minutes for initial set up of the equipment, introduction of the subjects and informed consent signing, followed by two baseline EEG measurements of two minutes with eyes open and eyes closed. C. Signal acquisition Brain activity was recorded with the BioSemi ActiveTwo signal acquisition system [13] using 32 electrodes mounted on an elastic cap at a sampling frequency of 48 Hz. The electrodes were positioned according to the international / standard and were uniformly distributed over the scalp. The signal from a photo diode placed on the computer screen rendering the task, was jointly recorded with the EEG to annotate the letter presentation events. III. DATA ANALYSIS The signals were subsampled to 24 Hz, the DC component was removed using a 2 Hz high-pass FIR filter, and the power line interference was attenuated applying a 1 Hzwide band-stop FIR filter centered at Hz. In order to minimize the effect of background neuronal activity in the area of interest and to localize the neural responses we rereferenced each channel to the average of all channels. This is known as common average re-referencing (CAR) [14], which was subtracted from each channel. Eye blink artifacts were corrected using Independent Component Analysis (ICA) [1]. ICA is a technique used to separate linearly mixed, statistically independent sources, based on the assumption that the cerebral sources, generating the signals recorded on the scalp, are non-gaussian and temporally independent. An eye-blink component has been identified for each subject and then the signals have been reconstructed excluding this component. No obvious distortions to the original underlying brain signals were observed. To examine the event related dynamics of working memory processing we estimated the power of the signal in three frequency bands: 3. - 7 Hz, 7. - 12 Hz and 17-29 Hz by band-pass filtering the signal in each band, squaring the result, and applying a 1-ms long running average filter. The choice of the bands was made by taking into account the largest differences between pre and post letter presentation. Theses bands do also coincide with the classical EEG ranges theta, alpha, and upper beta. For convenience hereafter we refer to them as theta, alpha, and beta. The original non-filtered (after eye-blink correction) and band-pass filtered signals were then segmented into 3 millisecond-long epochs starting at milliseconds before a stimulus onset until milliseconds after a stimulus onset resulting in one epoch for each trial. The non-filtered epochs were screened one more time for extreme high amplitude values, high frequency muscle noise and other irregular artifacts generated by non-cerebral activity. We used an automatic procedure taking the average of the two extremes (the minimum and the maximum) across all epoch plus twice their standard deviation as rejection thresholds. IV. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION The summary of the behavior results is shown in Table I. The average reaction time () for targets was slightly faster than that of non-targets. The percentage of correct responses was 86.89% on average, 81% for targets and 89.83% for nontargets. This results are comparable to that of other studies using a similar experimental paradigm. Only trials with correct responses were considered for further analysis. This resulted in 23 target and 1 non-target trials on average per participant. TABLE I BEHAVIOR RESULTS Targets Non-targets Total Mean (SD) [ms] 69 (223) 81 (228) 776 (2) Hits (SD) % 81. (9.6) 89.83 (7.22) 86.89 (6.74)

To examine stimulus- and response-induced global trends in the EEG, we have computed the grand average power across all subject in each of the bands identified above and for each electrode location. The proportional power change with respect to the baseline reference interval (set between and milliseconds before stimulus onset) in each channel was then averaged for every -millisecond long interval starting from 4 milliseconds before the letter presentation until milliseconds after the letter presentation. The result is visualized as a topographic map in Figure 2. In the first milliseconds after stimulus onset, the theta band shows increase in occipital sites, most probably related to the initial processing of the visual stimulus. We can also observe theta synchronization localized over frontocentral sites, which diminishes around 6 milliseconds. At the same time over parietal and occipital sites theta desynchronizes. Alpha band shows desynchronization in the first 6 milliseconds, primarily over frontal and occipital sites. Around the average reaction time (776 milliseconds) we can observe the focal ERD/surround ERS phenomena [16], which reflects sensorimotor activation and deactivation. Since all our participants were right handed, the ERD in the alpha band is, as expected, lateralized to the left hemisphere in central electrode positions. Beta band shows stable desynchronization appearing first over central, parietal and occipital sites, and later around 4 milliseconds after the stimulus onset also propagating over frontal electrode locations. After the average reaction time across participants we can observe another sensorimotor effect, a beta ERS, over parietal and occipital sites. Figure 2 confirms the relevance of frontal sites but also shows the potential involvement of central and parietal sites. The grand average of the instantaneous power (power dynamics) for theta, alpha, and beta along the midline, which includes frontal (Fz), central (Cz) and parietal (Pz) sites, is shown in Figure 3. We distinguish the power dynamics of target and non-target stimuli. Similarly to Figure 2, the power at each time point was normalized and related in percentage to the power in the reference interval. The average power changes in the interval from to 6 milliseconds after stimulus presentation was compared to a baseline interval from to 4 milliseconds before stimulus presentation using the non-parametric Wilcoxon signed-rank test. The results show that theta and beta dynamics are statistically significant at level p <=.1 for all sites and stimulus types. Overall, an initial rise of the theta level can be observed upon presentation of the stimulus. This is more prominent at the central site Cz and for the target stimuli. For the frontal and central locations, the theta power returns to reference levels before the reaction time has elapsed. After the reaction time, the theta level increases and decreases again in the parietal location Pz, which may be indicative of the subject s expectancy of presentation of the next letter. The decrease of the alpha level on presentation of the stimulus indicates the active engagement of the corresponding cortical site in processing the stimulus and the retrieval of the answer in working memory. For both targets and nontargets, and for Fz, Cz, and Pz, the alpha level decreases after presentation of the letter. The alpha level returns to the reference level few hundred milliseconds after the reaction time and more slowly than theta does. Beta also appears to evolve in an opposite direction of theta. Beta desynchronizes after stimulus presentation and returns to its reference level faster than alpha overall and slightly faster than theta at the parietal site Pz. Chn: Fz TARGET 1 Chn: Cz TARGET 1 Chn: Pz TARGET 1 Chn: Fz NONTARGET 1 Chn: Cz NONTARGET 1 Chn: Pz NONTARGET 1 Alpha Beta Theta Fig. 3. Grand average of the power changes for all subjects in the three bands: theta, alpha and beta at midline sites Fz, Cz, and Pz for target and non-target stimuli. The second vertical line between and milliseconds correspond to the average reaction time. The statistical significance, as estimated by Wilcoxon signed-rank test, of the average power levels in theta and beta band in the time interval from to 6 milliseconds after stimulus presentation compared to the baseline interval from to 4 milliseconds before stimulus presentation was p <=.1 for all sites and stimulus types. To visualize the trial-specific dynamics of the three power bands at the frontal midline location Fz, we represent in Figure 4(a) and Figure 4(b), for two participants in the study (subjects S2 and S7 respectively) the individual trials sorted in ascending order by their corresponding reaction time. The trials are represented in the time period spanning from milliseconds before stimuli presentation up to milliseconds after stimulus presentation and are smoothed using a -trial moving average window. The thick black curve represents the reaction time and the color code is commensurate with the instantaneous level. The level is simply the ratio between the current power and the power of the reference interval. As in Figure 3, we distinguish the target and non-target stimuli. These two participants exhibit signal dynamics that are qualitatively different from each other in the sense that for

Theta Alpha Beta -6-4 - 4 6 8 Average : 776 ms Fig. 2. Topographic representation of the grand average of the power changes for all subjects in the three bands: theta, alpha and beta. The values represent an average percentage change from a baseline [- - ms] in intervals of ms. For ease of visualization the color map has been adjusted for each band separately. subject S2, fast reaction times for both target and nontarget stimuli are characterized by a more pronounced theta synchronization while the opposite holds for subject S7. S2 behavior was common to five participants in the study. The correlation between reaction time and the average theta, alpha, and beta levels in the interval spanning from to 6 milliseconds after stimulus presentation for non-targets is reported in Figure for subjects S2 and S7. For this analysis we consider non-target trials due to the higher confidence resulting from the higher number of trials. The Pearson s correlation coefficient and it s statistical significance level for each location and band are also reported in the figure. The estimated trends confirm the fundamental difference in the brain dynamics of these two participants while performing the 2-back task. Individual specificities in brain functioning have been extensively documented in several studies and clearly manifests in our results. V. CONCLUSION Working memory is an essential component of human cognition that can be studied through the n-back task. In this study we asked participants to engage in a 2-back memory task using a set of (consonant) letters. Instead of considering ERP based features as generally done in literature, we focus on patterns of brain activity in three frequency bands, namely theta, alpha, and beta at 32 locations distributed over the scalp. In particular, we identified significant correlations between performance indicators and single trial dynamics. Our results show a pronounced relevance of location Fz and the theta dynamics to exhibit changes related to the execution of the working memory task. For both targets and non-targets the theta level raises immediately after stimulus presentation. The magnitude of such a raise appears to correlate with the speed to which subjects react to a stimulus presentation. Alpha exhibits overall desynchronization immediately after stimulus presentation and returns to pre-stimulus levels few hundred milliseconds after reaction time. From the overall alpha desynchronization, it seems that intake of the information to encode in the participant s working memory and the retrieval to compare with the item that was presented two trails ago engages several cortical sites. Traditional ensemble averaging across subjects and trials, however, does not fully model the actual brain dynamics. Individual specificities in brain activity manifest in our results as illustrated by the fact that different trends of dynamics correlate with reaction time in different subjects. Therefore, identifying relevant factors accounting for the variability in the signals and studying trial and subject specific dynamics could prove valuable. Additional insights about the link between brain dynamics and performance could be found by varying the task difficulty and looking into the accuracy of responses. Further research needs to be granted in order to correctly model the relation between working memory and brain rhythm dynamics. VI. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors gratefully acknowledge the contribution of Alina Weffers and Mark Jaeger for their relevant suggestions

1 S2 Theta TARGET Chn: Fz - S2 Alpha TARGET Chn: Fz 1 - S2 Beta TARGET Chn: Fz 1-1 S7 Theta TARGET Chn: Fz - S7 Alpha TARGET Chn: Fz 1 - S7 Beta TARGET Chn: Fz 1-4.9 1. 3-2 -.4 S2 Theta NONTARGET Chn: Fz -8.9 - S2 Alpha NONTARGET Chn: Fz 4.9 1.4 3-2.2 -.7-9.2 - S2 Beta NONTARGET Chn: Fz 3.7.9 3-1.9-4.6-7.4 - (a) Subject S2, Channel Fz.4 4 1.7 3-2.1 -.8 S7 Theta NONTARGET Chn: Fz -9.6 - S7 Alpha NONTARGET Chn: Fz 4.4 4.7 3-3 -6.7 -.4 - S7 Beta NONTARGET Chn: Fz 3 4.7 3-1. -3.7 -.9 - (b) Subject S7, Channel Fz Fig. 4. Theta, alpha and beta dynamics for each trial at frontal midline site Fz smoothed and sorted by reaction time. Trials are represented horizontally. The level estimate results from taking the logarithm of the ratio between the instantaneous power in the band and the power in the reference period. Red values show ERS, blue values show ERD. The black curve represent the for each trial. to improve this paper and our participants for their kind collaboration. REFERENCES [1] A. Baddeley, Working memory, Science, vol. 2, no. 44, pp. 6 9, 1992. [2] E. Basar, C. Basar-Eroglu, S. Karakas, and M. Schurmann, Gamma, alpha, delta, and theta oscillations govern cognitive processes, Int J Psychophysiol, vol. 39, pp. 241 248, Jan 1. [3] G. Pfurtscheller and F. H. L. da Silva, Event-related EEG/MEG synchronization and desynchronization: basic principles, Clinical Neurophysiology, vol. 1, no. 11, pp. 1842 187, 1999. [4] J. Onton, A. Delorme, and S. Makeig, Frontal midline EEG dynamics during working memory, Neuroimage, vol. 27, pp. 341 36, Aug. [] W. Klimesch, Memory processes, brain oscillations and EEG synchronization, International Journal of Psychophysiology, vol. 24, no. 1-2, pp. 61, 1996, new Advances in EEG and cognition.. 1.9-1.7 -.4-9.3 1.9-1.4-4.8-8.2 4 1.2-1.6-4. -7.3 6.3 2.6-1.1-4.8-8.6.3 1.6-2 -.7-9.4 2.8. -1.7-4 -6.2 1 S2 Theta Chn:Fz NONTARGET corr=.73 pval=6.67e 6 1 S2 Alpha Chn:Fz NONTARGET 1 corr=.82 pval=4.9e 8 1 S2 Beta Chn:Fz NONTARGET 1 corr=.88 pval=2.77e 2 1 8 7 6 S7 Theta Chn:Fz NONTARGET corr=.6 pval=4.8e 4 1 2 S7 Alpha Chn:Fz NONTARGET 8 7 6 corr=.71 pval=3.4e 6 4 1 S7 Beta Chn:Fz NONTARGET 8 7 6 corr=.71 pval=3.3e 6 4 14 1 8 6 4 Fig.. versus average alpha, theta, and beta levels in the time interval from to 6 milliseconds after stimulus presentation. The level estimate results from taking the logarithm of the ratio between the instantaneous power in the band and the power in the reference period. [6] O. Jensen and C. D. Tesche, Frontal theta activity in humans increases with memory load in a working memory task, European Journal of Neuroscience, vol. 1, no. 8. [7] C. M. Krause, L. Sillanmki, M. Koivisto, C. Saarela, A. Hggqvist, M. Laine, and H. Hmlinen, The effects of memory load on eventrelated EEG desynchronization and synchronization, Clinical Neurophysiology, vol. 111, no. 11, pp. 71 78,. [8] A. Gevins, M. E. Smith, L. McEvoy, and D. Yu, High-resolution EEG mapping of cortical activation related to working memory: effects of task difficulty, type of processing, and practice. vol. 7, no. 4, pp. 374 38, 1997. [9] W. Klimesch, EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis, Brain Research Reviews, vol. 29, no. 2-3, pp. 169 19, 1999. [] M. Pesonen, H. Hamalainen, and C. M. Krause, Brain oscillatory 4-3 Hz responses during a visual n-back memory task with varying memory load, Brain Res., vol. 1138, pp. 171 177, Mar 7. [11] A. M. Owen, K. M. McMillan, A. R. Laird, and E. Bullmore, N-back working memory paradigm: a meta-analysis of normative functional neuroimaging studies, Hum Brain Mapp, vol. 2, pp. 46 9, May. [12] P. S. T. Inc. E-prime, Sharpsburg, USA. [Online]. Available: http://www.pstnet.com/eprime.cfm [13] BioSemi. BioSemi, Amsterdam, The Netherlands. [Online]. Available: http://www.biosemi.com/ [14] P. L. Nunez and R. Srinivasan, Electric Fields of the Brain: The Neurophysics of EEG. Oxford University Press, 6. [1] A. Hyvarinen and E. Oja, Independent component analysis: Algorithms and applications, Neural Networks, vol. 13, no. 4-, pp. 411 43,. [16] C. Neuper, M. Wortz, and G. Pfurtscheller, ERD/ERS patterns reflecting sensorimotor activation and deactivation, Prog. Brain Res., vol. 19, pp. 211 222, 6.