Cognitive Impairment in Depression Is Not Associated with Neuropathologic Evidence of Increased Vascular or Alzheimer-Type Pathology

Similar documents
STRUCTURAL ABNORMALITY ON BRAIN MAGNETIC RESONANCE IMAGING IN LATE-ONSET MAJOR DEPRESSIVE DISORDER

NACC Vascular Consortium. NACC Vascular Consortium. NACC Vascular Consortium

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 January 1.

Neuropathology of Neurodegenerative Disorders Prof. Jillian Kril

Synaptic changes in dementia: links to cognition and behaviour

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI)

A prospective study of dementia with Lewy bodies

Lewy body disease (LBD) is the second most common

V ascular dementia is recognised as one of the most common

Imaging of Alzheimer s Disease: State of the Art

Alzheimer disease (AD) is a devastating neurodegenerative

FDG-PET e parkinsonismi

Anxiety, Depression, and Dementia/Alzheimer Disease: What are the Links?

WHAT IS DEMENTIA? An acquired syndrome of decline in memory and other cognitive functions sufficient to affect daily life in an alert patient

Psychology, 3 Department of Anatomy, Histology and Embryology,

Visual Rating Scale Reference Material. Lorna Harper Dementia Research Centre University College London

Hallucinations and signs of parkinsonism help distinguish patients with dementia and cortical

Neuropathological Substrates of Psychiatric Symptoms in Prospectively Studied Patients With Autopsy- Confirmed Dementia With Lewy Bodies

Anosognosia, or loss of insight into one s cognitive

Diffusion Tensor Imaging in Psychiatry

OLD AGE PSYCHIATRY. Dementia definition TYPES OF DEMENTIA. Other causes. Psychiatric disorders of the elderly. Dementia.

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Regional Distribution of Neuritic Plaques in the Nondemented Elderly and Subjects With Very Mild Alzheimer Disease

Disease Modification in Schizophrenia: Overview of the Issues. ISCTM February 18 th 2014 Ravi Anand, MD Switzerland

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

Neuroimaging for Diagnosis of Psychiatric Disorders

Gross morphological brain changes with chronic, heavy cannabis use

Neuropsychiatric Manifestations in Vascular Cognitive Impairment Patients with and without Dementia

Role of TDP-43 in Non-Alzheimer s and Alzheimer s Neurodegenerative Diseases

Clinicopathologic and genetic aspects of hippocampal sclerosis. Dennis W. Dickson, MD Mayo Clinic, Jacksonville, Florida USA

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Neurofibrillary Tangles in Nondemented Elderly Subjects and Mild Alzheimer Disease

Brain imaging for the diagnosis of people with suspected dementia

Cerebral amyloid angiopathy in dementia and old age

New life Collage of nursing Karachi

Amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) is a progressive neurodegenerative

Yin-Hui Siow MD, FRCPC Director of Nuclear Medicine Southlake Regional Health Centre

Pentagon copying is more impaired in dementia with Lewy bodies than in Alzheimer s disease

W hite matter lesions (WML) in elderly people result

Age, Neuropathology, and Dementia

ALLEN Human Brain Atlas

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Diagnostic Accuracy of Dementia With Lewy Bodies. to be the second

Dementia. Stephen S. Flitman, MD Medical Director 21st Century Neurology

Subject Index. Band of Giacomini 22 Benton Visual Retention Test 66 68

Vascular Dementia. Laura Pedelty, PhD MD The University of Illinois at Chicago and Jesse Brown VA Medical Center

Late-onset major depression: clinical and treatment-response variability

DEMENTIA 101: WHAT IS HAPPENING IN THE BRAIN? Philip L. Rambo, PhD

DISCLOSURES. Objectives. THE EPIDEMIC of 21 st Century. Clinical Assessment of Cognition: New & Emerging Tools for Diagnosing Dementia NONE TO REPORT

Caring Sheet #11: Alzheimer s Disease:

Dementia and Healthy Ageing : is the pathology any different?

Piano playing skills in a patient with frontotemporal dementia: A longitudinal case study

ALZHEIMER S DISEASE. Mary-Letitia Timiras M.D. Overlook Hospital Summit, New Jersey

Dementia and Alzheimer s disease

Cognitive Decline in Patients with Alzheimer's Disease, Vascular Dementia and Senile Dementia of Lewy Body Type

Vascular risk factors in late onset mania

A Dynamic Model of Care for Late Onset Cognitive Impairment. Linda CW Lam Department of Psychiatry The Chinese University of Hong Kong

Dementia. Aetiology, pathophysiology and the role of neuropsychological testing. Dr Sheng Ling Low Geriatrician

Leukoencephalopathy in Normal and PathologiC Aging: 1. CT of Brain Lucencies

Neuro degenerative PET image from FDG, amyloid to Tau

Introduction, use of imaging and current guidelines. John O Brien Professor of Old Age Psychiatry University of Cambridge

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Early and Widespread Cholinergic Losses Differentiate Dementia With Lewy Bodies From Alzheimer Disease

Supplementary Online Content

Neurotransmitter: dopamine. Physiology of additive drugs. Dopamine and reward. Neurotransmitter: dopamine

A lzheimer s disease has many neuropsychiatric manifestations

Neuro-Imaging in dementia: using MRI in routine work-up Prof. Philip Scheltens

Non Alzheimer Dementias

Pathogenesis of Degenerative Diseases and Dementias. D r. Ali Eltayb ( U. of Omdurman. I ). M. Path (U. of Alexandria)

Dementia. Assessing Brain Damage. Mental Status Examination

Comparison of clock drawing with Mini Mental State Examination as a screening test in elderly acute hospital admissions

The ABCs of Dementia Diagnosis

EXECUTIVE dysfunction, including

Quantitative analysis for a cube copying test

Delirium & Dementia. Nicholas J. Silvestri, MD

NACC Neuropathology (NP) Diagnosis Coding Guidebook

Vascular Cognitive Impairment-- NEUROPATHOLOGIC ISSUES. VCI vs. IVD/DEMENTIA with VASCULAR DISEASE (IVD) advanced pathology

Assessing Cognition. Andreana Haley, Ph.D. Department of Psychology University of Texas at Austin

Southwest Brain Bank

DOWNLOAD PDF THE EFFECT OF AEROBIC EXERCISE ON INFORMATION PROCESSING IN OLDER ADULTS

Erin Cullnan Research Assistant, University of Illinois at Chicago

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

ACENTRAL TENET OF ALZHEIMER

PSYC& 100: Biological Psychology (Lilienfeld Chap 3) 1

biological psychology, p. 40 The study of the nervous system, especially the brain. neuroscience, p. 40

Parkinson e decadimento cognitivo. Stelvio Sestini

T he concept of vascular cognitive covers a

White matter lesions on magnetic resonance imaging in dementia with Lewy bodies, Alzheimer s disease, vascular dementia, and normal aging

Parkinson' s disease: comparisons with Alzheimer' s

Chronic Traumatic Encephalopathy Provider and Parent Essentials

ORIGINAL CONTRIBUTION. Attention and Fluctuating Attention in Patients With Dementia With Lewy Bodies and Alzheimer Disease

Probable Early-onset Alzheimer s Disease Diagnosed by Comprehensive Evaluation and Neuroimage Studies

Cognitive Impairment and Magnetic Resonance Changes in Multiple Sclerosis. Background

The Neurobiology of Mood Disorders

CEREBRUM. Dr. Jamila EL Medany

PRESERVE: How intensively should we treat blood pressure in established cerebral small vessel disease? Guide to assessing MRI scans

TGF-ß1 pathway as a new pharmacological target for neuroprotection in AD. Filippo Caraci


The Nervous System. Biological School. Neuroanatomy. How does a Neuron fire? Acetylcholine (ACH) TYPES OF NEUROTRANSMITTERS

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston

Depression: An Important Risk Factor for Cognitive Decline

Name: Period: Chapter 2 Reading Guide The Biology of Mind

Transcription:

Cognitive Impairment in Depression Is Not Associated with Neuropathologic Evidence of Increased Vascular or Alzheimer-Type Pathology John O Brien, Alan Thomas, Clive Ballard, Andrew Brown, Nicol Ferrier, Evelyn Jaros, and Robert Perry Background: Cognitive impairment is common in depression, but underlying mechanisms remain unknown. We examined whether increases in Alzheimer-type or vascular pathology are associated with cognitive impairments in elderly depressed subjects. Methods: Eleven subjects who had died during a welldocumented episode of DSM-IV major depression were included. Neuropathologic assessments, blind to group membership, included standardized assessment of neuritic plaques, neurofibrillary tangles, and Lewy Bodies in frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices. Braak staging of Alzheimer pathology was also performed. Cerebral microvascular disease was scored according to a previously validated scale, and a score for cerebral and systemic atheroma of large and medium sized arteries was obtained. Results: No subject had Lewy bodies. Plaque and tangle counts for all subjects were well within published norms for age-matched control subjects. There were no significant differences in plaque or tangle counts between subjects who were cognitively impaired (n 5) and those who were nonimpaired (n 6) during their depressive illness. Similarly, neither total microvascular pathology nor deep frontal microvascular pathology differed between the two groups. Conclusions: Our results indicate that the liability for some patients to develop cognitive impairment during a depressive episode is not related to an increase in Alzheimer-type or vascular neuropathologic change. This indicates that other mechanisms must underlie both the cognitive impairment associated with depression and the observation that depression is a risk factor for dementia. Biol Psychiatry 2001 Society of Biological Psychiatry From the Institute for the Health of the Elderly (JO, AT, CB, AB, EJ, RP) and the Department of Psychiatry (NF), University of Newcastle upon Tyne, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom. Address reprint requests to John O Brien, D.M., Newcastle General Hospital, Wolfson Research Centre, Institute for the Health of the Elderly, Westgate Road, Newcastle upon Tyne NE4 6BE, United Kingdom. Received January 3, 2000; revised May 19, 2000; accepted May 26, 2000. Key Words: Depression, affective disorder, neuropathology, cognitive impairment, postmortem, elderly Introduction It is well known that depression, particularly in elderly subjects, is associated with a number of deficits in cognitive function (Abas et al 1990; Austin et al 1999; Beats et al 1996). These deficits, when severe, can resemble those seen in an organic dementia such as Alzheimer s disease, giving rise to the clinical syndrome of pseudo dementia and the emphasis placed on trying to obtain an accurate diagnosis in such patients (Bulbena and Berrios 1986; McAllister 1985). It is now clear that the relationship between cognitive impairments occurring as part of a depressive disorder and those seen in organic conditions is complex. Cognitive impairments occurring during depression do not always remit on recovery from illness. For example, Abas et al (1990) found that whereas 70% of depressed subjects were impaired on a learning task during illness, 30% remained impaired despite clinical recovery. The cause of such enduring cognitive deficits is unknown, though they may carry an adverse prognosis with regard to long-term outcome. Kral and Emery (1989) followed up 44 elderly patients who had previously suffered from what was thought to be depressive pseudodementia on the grounds that cognitive function reverted to premorbid levels following treatment. After an average follow-up of 8 years, 89% were felt to have developed Alzheimer s disease. Epidemiologic studies have shown that depression is an independent risk factor for dementia (Jorm et al 1991). These lines of evidence suggest that cognitive impairment occurring during a depressive illness may indicate an increased liability to subsequently develop dementia and, in particular, Alzheimer s disease. If so, neuropathologic study of such cases might be expected to show a greater burden of Alzheimer type pathology in those depressed subjects who develop significant cognitive dysfunction compared to those who do not. 2001 Society of Biological Psychiatry 0006-3223/01/$20.00 PII S0006-3223(00)00944-6

Neuropathology of Cognitive Impairment BIOL PSYCHIATRY 131 There are, however, other reasons why elderly depressed patients may develop cognitive impairments. It is now clear that neuropsychological deficits affect most domains tested and include problems with attention, learning and memory, frontal and executive tasks, as well as motivation (Abas et al 1990; Austin et al 1999; Beats et al 1996; Elliot 1998; Salloway et al 1996). It has also been suggested that some impairments, particularly those in learning and memory, may be associated with hippocampal damage due to the raised levels of corticosteroids known to be associated with depressive illness, particularly in the elderly (O Brien 1997). The recent demonstration of reduced hippocampal volume in depressed subjects that correlated with lifetime duration of depression (and therefore presumably exposure to corticosteroids) supports this view (Sheline et al 1999). Another possible explanation for depressed subjects developing cognitive impairment is the presence of cerebrovascular pathology. One of the most consistently observed abnormalities in elderly depressed subjects is of an increased frequency of hyperintensities in the white matter and subcortical grey matter visualized on magnetic resonance imaging (Coffey et al 1990; Figiel et al 1991; Greenwald et al 1996, 1998; O Brien et al 1996). These lesions are more common in depressed subjects with onset in late life (Figiel et al 1991; O Brien et al 1996; Salloway et al 1996) and have been associated with cognitive impairments both during the depressive episode (Salloway et al 1996) and at follow-up (Hickie et al 1997). Partly on the basis of these lesions and the link between depression and cardiovascular disorder, the notion of vascular depression has been proposed (Alexopoulos et al 1997; Hickie and Scott 1998; Krishnan and McDonald 1995). Because microvascular change is a cause of white matter hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (Chimowitz et al 1992) and such lesions are associated with cognitive impairments in depression, it is possible that cognitive impairments occurring during depressive episode may be related to an increase in microvascular pathology. There have been no previous studies of the neuropathologic basis of the cognitive impairments that occur during depression. We wished to test the hypothesis that such impairments would be associated with an increased burden of Alzheimer-type and/or microvascular pathology in cortical areas. Methods and Materials Subjects Cases were obtained from records of the Neuropathology Department and the Newcastle Brain Tissue Bank. All depressed cases were from referrals to a secondary care unit, the Brighton Clinic. All were inpatients except one who was attending the day hospital. Full ethical approval has been granted for this postmortem study and consent for postmortem examination obtained from the next of kin. Clinical Assessment Subjects were included if they were 60 years or over at death and had died during a well-documented episode of DSM-IV Major Depression (American Psychiatric Association 1994). They had all undergone extensive assessments including a full history, mental state examination, physical examination, screening blood tests and, in some cases, computed tomography (CT) or MRI scans. Subjects were excluded if they had committed suicide by violent means or had ever met DSM-IV criteria for dementia, schizophrenia, or other psychotic disorders, manic or hypomanic episode or a mood disorder due to a general medical condition. Subjects were divided into a cognitively impaired group who were significantly impaired when depressed, and a nonimpaired group who showed no evidence of impairment even when depressed. Neuropathologic Assessment All subjects received a full postmortem examination (except one whose body was taken by the undertakers after the brain was removed) and the delay to postmortem was recorded. The right hemisphere was fixed in 10% formalin and the brains were dissected in a standard manner (Perry et al 1990a). Blocks were taken from the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices, the hippocampus, the cingulate, the basal ganglia, the cerebellum, the upper and lower midbrain, the pons and the medulla. Five m or20 m sections were cut, and sections were prepared on glass slides. These were stained with haematoxylin and eosin (H&E), luxol fast blue (LFB) and/or Loyez (myelin), cresyl fast violet (CFV) or Nissl, methanamine silver and/or von Braunmuhl (plaques), Palmgren and/or tau-2 (tangles) and ubiquitin (Lewy bodies). These were then examined by experienced neuropathologists (RP and EJ) Subjects were excluded if they met neuropathologic criteria for any neurodegenerative disease such as Alzheimer s disease or dementia with Lewy bodies. Assessment of Alzheimer and Lewy Body Pathology Histopathologic analysis was carried out to quantify the number of neuritic plaques and neurofibrillary tangles in the four neocortical lobes (Perry et al 1990a, 1996). Both neuritic and diffuse plaques were counted in five fields (including gyral crests and sulcal depths and including all cortical layers) from each neocortical lobe (frontal, temporal, parietal, occipital). A mean score was obtained and converted to numbers per mm 2. Neocortical tangles were also counted in a similar manner, except the count used was the mean of the fields across all the cortical layers. Alzheimer-type pathology was staged according to the procedure of Braak and Braak (1991), assessing neurofibrillary tangles and neuropil threads in different parts of the hippocampus (stages 1 4) and neocortex (stages 5 and 6). Reliability was assessed by the single operator responsible for the study rating 10 subjects

132 BIOL PSYCHIATRY J. O Brien et al selected at random on 5 occasions for plaque and tangle counts. Co-efficients of variation (SD/mean) were 0% for tangle counts and 5.3% for plaque counts. Assessment of Large and Medium-Sized Arteries The extent of atheromatous degeneration in the coronary arteries, cerebral carotid and vertebrobasilar systems, and aorta was rated on a semiquantitative 0 3 scale (RP and AT). 0 none; 1 mild: atheroma only mild and patchy; 2 moderate: more extensive atheroma with less than 50% luminal stenosis; 3 severe: atheroma widespread with greater than 50% luminal stenosis in at least one vessel. An overall atheroma score was obtained for each subject by adding together the scores from each site. Assessment of Small Arteries and Arterioles For assessing microvascular disease the sections stained with H&E, LFB, and/or Loyez and Nissl from the frontal, temporal, parietal, and occipital cortices were used. In addition, large full-face coronal blocks were taken from the frontal and occipital cortices to include all the deep white matter in these two areas, coronal levels 4/5 and 27 (Perry 1993). Ten- m or 20- m sections were prepared on large (3 2 ) slides and stained with H&E, LFB, and/or Loyez (myelin) and Nissl. Microvascular disease was then scored blind to diagnosis by two raters (RP and AT) in each neocortical area in accordance with a scale previously used in vascular dementia (Esiri et al 1997). Briefly, this is a 0 3 scale in which 0 normal; 1 dilatation of perivascular spaces or hyaline thickening or a few perivascular macrophages; 2 dilatation of perivascular spaces or hyaline thickening plus mild to moderate perivascular pallor of myelin staining or loosening of the nerve fibres or nerve cells with gliosis; 3 Binswanger s disease (2 plus more widespread myelin pallor, nerve fibre or nerve cell disruption and gliosis). An overall microvascular score was calculated for each subject by adding together the scores for each neocortical area. A separate score was obtained by assessment of the frontal deep white matter. Statistical Analysis The two groups were compared statistically using standard software. Minitab (version 12; Minitab, State College, PA) was used to carry out Mann Whitney tests for the quantitative data (age, post-mortem interval, plaques, tangles, Lewy bodies) and StatXact (version 4, Cytel Software, Cambridge, MA) was used to carry out Fisher Freeman Halton test (more appropriate than 2 because of the low cell frequency) for the categorical data (sex, Braak, atheroma, and microvascular scores). Results Eleven subjects were studied. Eight suffered from recurrent unipolar depression, and all but one were depressed at death. All except one had received past treatment with electroconvulsive therapy. Five subjects were cognitively impaired when depressed. Of these, two had such marked psychomotor retardation that meaningful standardized cognitive testing was not possible, though both were clearly clinically very impaired, as for a time genuine doubt existed as to whether or not they had an organic dementia or a depressive pseudodementia. Testing of the other three showed scores in the impaired range on either the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE, maximum score 30; Folstein et al 1975) or the Mental Test Score (MTS, maximum score 37; Blessed et al 1968). One subject had an MMSE score of 22; the others had MTS scores of 22 and 25. Six subjects showed no clinical evidence of cognitive impairment. Two were recorded in casenotes as having undergone cognitive testing and being completely oriented, with normal concentration and memory, although neither the MMSE nor MTS was completed. Tests on the other four revealed MMSE scores of 28 and 29 and MTS scores of 36 and 37. Cause of death for the cognitively impaired patients was carcinoma (n 2) and bronchopneumonia (n 3). Cause of death for the nonimpaired patients was cardiac arrest (n 3), suicide (n 1), pulmonary embolus (n 1) and congestive cardiac failure (n 1). Four of the five cognitively impaired patients had failed to respond to adequate courses of two different antidepressants. Of unimpaired subjects, two had failed to respond to adequate courses of two different antidepressants, two had shown some response but were still depressed, and two died without apparent response to an antidepressant. Subject characteristics and results are shown in Table 1, whereas Table 2 contains a detailed breakdown of neuropathologic scores for each subject. For comparison, scores are also presented in Table 2 from published data on control and demented subjects of very similar age obtained using the same methodology (Perry et al 1996). There were no significant differences in age (t 36, p.31) or postmortem delay (t 22, p.31), but there was a trend toward a difference in gender (Fisher statistic 4.8, p.06), because all five cognitively impaired depressed subjects were female. No statistical differences were found between the groups on Alzheimer-type pathology (p.41) or Lewy body pathology (p 1.00, no Lewy bodies were seen in any case). The total atheroma and total microvascular scores were very similar (Fisher statistic 1.42, p.70 and Fisher statistic 4.31, p.51, respectively) and, although the frontal microvascular scores were slightly higher in the nonimpaired group, this did not approach significance statistically (Fisher statistic 6.9, sig. 0.23). As seen in Table 2, levels of Alzheimer type pathology is comparable to that of similarly aged normal control subjects and much less than found in Alzheimer s disease.

Neuropathology of Cognitive Impairment BIOL PSYCHIATRY 133 Table 1. Comparison of Cognitively Impaired and Nonimpaired Groups Impaired Nonimpaired Test statistic Significance Age (years) a 79.0 (4.69) 73.0 (8.81) 36.31 Gender (male/female) b 0/5 4/2 4.8.06 Postmortem delay (hours) a 47.5 (28.62) 27.8 (6.34) 22.31 Total atheroma score b 3 3 1.42 1.00 Total microvascular score b 3 5 4.31.84 Deep white matter frontal 0 2 0.55 1.00 microvascular b Neocortical plaques (per mm 2 ) a 1.4 (1.27) 0.98 (1.41) 34.52 Neocortical tangles (per mm 2 ) a 0.2 (0.39) 0 (0) 34.41 Braak stage b 1 1 2.59.65 Lewy bodies (per cm 2 ) a 0 (0) 0 (0) Not applicable 1.00 a Values are mean (SD) and comparisons are made using the Mann Whitney test. b Values are medians and comparisons are made using the Fisher Freeman Halton test. Discussion This is the first study to investigate neuropathologic changes in depressed subjects with and without cognitive impairment. We found no evidence of increased neuropathologic abnormalities in the form of Alzheimer-type pathology or microvascular change in depressed subjects who developed cognitive impairment during depression compared to those who did not. A relationship with Alzheimer pathology might have been expected because of the similarity of some aspects of cognitive dysfunction in depression to those seen in Alzheimer s disease (Abas et al 1990), the high proportion of cognitively impaired depressed patients who subsequently develop the disorder (Kral and Emery 1989), and the fact that depression is a risk factor for Alzheimer s disease (Jorm et al 1991). The original study of Blessed et al (1968) which found a correlation, now replicated by several groups, between plaque count and cognitive test score in dementia included some cases with functional illness, seven of whom were depressed; however, our more detailed study (which does not include any of the same cases used by Blessed et al) shows that this well-recognized relationship in dementia does not apply to cognitive impairment in depression. Minimal Alzheimer-type pathology was seen in the depressed cases, of the same order as would be expected in age-matched control subjects (Perry et al 1990b). Microvascular pathology was also not extensive using currently available methods of assessment (Esiri et al 1997). Although the link between vascular disease and late life depression is well recognised (Hickie and Scott 1998), such pathology did not explain the occurrence of signifi- Table 2. Neuropathologic Scores in Two Depressed Groups Cognitive impairment Total atheroma Total microvascular Deep frontal microvascular Plaques per mm 2 Tangles per mm 2 Braak stage Lewy bodies Impaired 4 4 2 0.9 0.1 1 0 Impaired 3 3 1 0.9 0.0 2 0 Impaired 0 2 0 0.4 0.0 0 0 Impaired 1 8 2 3.6 0.0 0 0 Impaired 3 2 1 1.1 0.9 2 0 Nonimpaired 0 3 1 1.0 0.0 1 0 Nonimpaired NA 4 0 1.0 0.0 1 0 Nonimpaired 3 6 2 0.2 0.0 0 0 Nonimpaired 3 8 2 3.7 0.0 3 0 Nonimpaired 4 1 0 0.0 0.0 2 0 Nonimpaired NA 8 1 0.0 0.0 1 0 Control subjects 2.0 (3.3) 0.07 (0.58) 2.6 (1.1) (n 138) a Alzheimer s disease 19.6 (13.4) 23.3 (17.3) (n 113) a Senile dementia of Lewy body type (n 61) a 110 (134) NA, not available. a Data taken for comparison from Perry et al (1990b, 1996) using the same criteria. Values are mean (SD).

134 BIOL PSYCHIATRY J. O Brien et al cant cognitive dysfunction in our patients during their depression. Consistent with this, we found no difference between impaired and nonimpaired groups for atheromatous degeneration score that reflected cerebral and systemic large and medium vessel pathology. Although many studies have examined neurotransmitter and receptor changes during depression, there has been only very limited focus on neuropathologic changes. This is partly because affective illness has been viewed as a state-dependent, reversible condition. This is, particularly in the elderly, now known to be flawed, as elderly depressed subjects have both enduring cognitive impairments (Abas et al 1990) and demonstrable neurobiological changes, which suggest structural abnormalities. For example, depressed subjects have increased cortical atrophy and ventricular enlargement when compared to control subjects (Elkis et al 1995; Jacoby and Levy 1980; Rabins et al 1991). In an MRI study, Rabins et al (1991) was unable to differentiate those with depression and Alzheimer s disease on the basis of either cortical atrophy or the presence of white matter change. Our findings show that the causes of cognitive impairment and structural brain changes in depression lie outside those of degenerative or microvascular pathology. Results are consistent with the notion that deficits in some areas, particularly in learning and memory, may be related to excess cortisol secretion as a result of hypothalmic pituitary adrenal axis activation during the illness (O Brien 1997). Several lines of evidence support this view. First, in animal models prolonged raised corticosteroid secretion is associated with deficits in learning and memory and hippocampal cell loss (Sapolsky and Plotsky 1990; Sapolsky et al 1986). Second, the hippocampus is a major site of glucocorticoid receptors, and at a cellular level excess glucocorticoids increase calcium dependent influx, which is known to be neurotoxic (Landfield and Eldridge 1994). Third, following exogenous steroid administration or raised endogenous levels as a result of stress, cognitive deficits can be demonstrated in healthy subjects (Kirschbaum et al 1996). Fourth, in Cushing s syndrome excess cortisol production is associated with cognitive impairments and hippocampal change (Starkman et al 1992). Finally, depression has been shown to be associated with mild hippocampal atrophy (Sheline et al 1999), which correlates with the total duration of depression. To determine whether hippocampal cell loss is a cause of cognitive decline associated with depression, further neuropathologic study of cell morphometry in the hippocampus would need to be performed. Cognitive impairments during depression have also been associated with abnormalities in other brain areas, particularly the medial prefrontal cortex (Dolan et al 1992). Changes in similar brain areas have also been described on MRI (Drevets et al 1997), and a preliminary neuropathologic report suggests glial loss in the absence of neuronal loss in one part of the prefrontal cortex, at least in younger patients with depression (Drevets et al 1998). Changes in these or related areas may also underlie some of the neuropsychological impairments that occur during depression. Salloway et al (1996) found white matter lesions to be associated with impairments in attentional function. Although such lesions may be an important correlate of impairment, we could find no evidence that this was accompanied by microvascular change, at least in the cortical areas that we examined (which did include frontal cortex). The tests of cognition used in this study, however, the MMSE and the MTS, are global measures of cognitive function not particularly sensitive to executive function, processing speed and other tasks subserved by frontal subcortical areas. Because these are the regions most affected in imaging studies (Coffey et al 1993; Greenwald et al 1998), it may be that there would be correlations between microvascular pathology in these regions and more specific frontal tests. Some other limitations of our study should be considered. Our cases were not prospectively assessed, and so information regarding their depressive illness and cognitive function came from case notes; however, case notes within the service were of a very high quality, with detailed typed summaries. All cases had detailed comment about cognitive performance, and the majority had received a standardized assessment of cognitive function. Numbers of depressed subjects were small and, although the cognitive impairment documented during the depressive illness was felt to be secondary to depression, we were not able to document reversibility on recovery from the depression and so cannot be certain that this impairment was not due to another cause. Although this may have been a possible confounding explanation if we had found an increase in microvascular or Alzheimer type pathology in the group with cognitive impairment, we do not feel this can explain our negative results. Although our subjects were impaired on clinical measures of cognition, scores were in the moderate range and it may be that by studying a group with more pronounced cognitive impairments during depression correlations with neurodegenerative pathology may exist. In summary, we found no evidence of neuropathologic differences between cognitively impaired and nonimpaired depressed subjects. This supports the view that cognitive dysfunction in depression is mediated by mechanisms that are different from those related to Alzheimer s disease or vascular dementia. Hypercortisolaemia is one possible explanation. There also needs to be further study of the possible neurochemical basis of cognitive impairments in depression, as previous reports have suggested

Neuropathology of Cognitive Impairment BIOL PSYCHIATRY 135 cholinergic loss may be important (Perry et al 1978). To investigate this further, in vivo study of subjects who have undergone detailed neuropsychological testing, assessment of endocrine function, and brain imaging should be combined with clinico-pathological study that includes detailed assessment of hippocampal cell morphometry, investigation of glucocorticoid receptor changes, and detailed neurochemical analysis. The authors thank the Medical Research Council and the Stanley Foundation for financial support. References Abas MA, Sahakian BJ, Levy R (1990): Neuropsychological deficits and CT scan changes in elderly depressives. Psychol Med 20:507 520. Alexopoulos GS, Meyers BS, Young RC, Campbell S, Silbersweig D, Charlson M (1997): Vascular depression hypothesis. Arch Gen Psychiatry 54:915 922. American Psychiatric Association (1994): Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Austin M-P, Mitchell P, Wilhelm K, Parker G, Hickie I, Brodaty H, et al (1999): Cognitive function in depression: A distinct pattern of frontal impairment? Psychol Med 29:73 85. Beats BC, Sahakian BJ, Levy R (1996): Cognitive performance in tests sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in the elderly depressed. Psychol Med 26:591 603. Blessed G, Tomlinson BE, Roth M (1968): The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects. Br J Psychiatry 114:797 811. Braak H, Braak E (1991): Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 82:239 59. Bulbena A, Berrios GE (1986): Pseudodementia: Facts and figures. Br J Psychiatry 148:87 94. Chimowitz MI, Estes ML, Furlan AJ, Awad IA (1992): Further observations on the pathology of subcortical lesions identified on magnetic resonance imaging. Arch Neurol 49:747 752. Coffey CE, Figiel GS, Djang WT, Weiner RD (1990): Subcortical hyperintensity on magnetic resonance imaging: a comparison of normal and depressed elderly subjects. Am J Psychiatry 147:187 189. Coffey CE, Wilkinson WE, Weiner RD, Parashos IA, Djang WT, Webb MC, et al (1993): Quantitative cerebral anatomy in depression: A controlled magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 50:7 15. Dolan RJ, Bench CJ, Brown RG, Scott LC, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS (1992): Regional cerebral blood flow abnormalities in depressed patients with cognitive impairment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 55:768 773. Drevets WC, Ongur D, Price JL (1998): Neuroimaging abnormalities in the subgenual prefrontal cortex: Implications for the pathophysiology of familial mood disorders. Mol Psychiatry 3:220 226. Drevets WC, Price JL, Simpson JR Jr, Todd RD, Reich T, Vannier M, Raichle ME (1997): Subgenual prefrontal cortex abnormalities in mood disorders. Nature 386:824 827. Elkis H, Friedman L, Wise A, Meltzer HY (1995): Meta-analyses of studies of ventricular enlargement and cortical sulcal prominence in mood disorders. Comparisons with controls or patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry 52:735 746. Elliot R (1998): The neuropsychological profile in unipolar depression. Trends Cogn Sci 2:447 454. Esiri MM, Wilcock GK, Morris JH (1997): Neuropathological assessment of the lesions of significance in vascular dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 63:749 753. Figiel GS, Krishnan KR, Doraiswamy PM, Rao VP, Nemeroff CB, Boyko OB (1991): Subcortical hyperintensities on brain magnetic resonance imaging: A comparison between late age onset and early onset elderly depressed subjects. Neurobiol Aging 12:245 247. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR (1975): Mini-mental state. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 12:189 198. Greenwald BS, Kramer-Ginsberg E, Krishnan KRR, Ashtari M, Auerbach C, Patel M (1998): Neuroanatomic localization of magnetic resonance imaging signal hyperintensities in geriatric depression. Stroke 29:613 617. Greenwald BS, Kramer-Ginsberg E, Krishnan KRR, Ashtari M, Aupperle PM, Patel M (1996): MRI signal hyperintensities in geriatric depression. Am J Psychiatry 153:1212 1215. Hickie I, Scott E (1998): Late-onset depressive disorders: A preventable variant of cerebrovascular disease? Psychol Med 28:1007 1013. Hickie I, Scott E, Wilhelm K, Brodaty H (1997): Subcortical hyperintensities on magnetic resonance imaging in patients with severe depression a longitudinal evaluation. Biol Psychiatry 42:367 374. Jacoby RJ, Levy R (1980): Computed tomography in the elderly. 3. Affective disorder. Br J Psychiatry 136:270 275. Jorm AF, Van Duijn CM, Chandra V, Fratiglioni L, Graves AB, Heyman A, et al (1991): Psychiatric history and related exposures as risk factors for Alzheimer s disease: A collaborative re-analysis of case-control studies. EURODEM. Int J Epidemiol 20:43 47. Kirschbaum C, Wolf OT, May M, Wippich W, Hellhammer DH (1996): Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Sci 58:1475 1483. Kral VA, Emery OB (1989): Long-term follow-up of depressive pseudodementia of the aged. Can J Psychiatry 34:445 446. Krishnan KR, McDonald WM (1995): Arteriosclerotic depression. Med Hypotheses 44:111 115. Landfield PW, Eldridge JC (1994): Evolving aspects of the glucocorticoid hypothesis of brain aging: hormonal modulation of neuronal calcium homeostasis. Neurobiol Aging 15: 579 588. McAllister TW (1985): Recognition of pseudodementia. Am Fam Physician 32:175 181. O Brien J, Desmond P, Ames D, Schweitzer I, Harrigan S, Tress B (1996): A magnetic resonance imaging study of white matter lesions in depression and Alzheimer s disease. Br J Psychiatry 168:477 485.

136 BIOL PSYCHIATRY J. O Brien et al O Brien JT (1997): The glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis in man. Br J Psychiatry 170:199 201. Perry EK, Tomlinson BE, Blessed G, Bergmann K, Gibson PH, Perry RH (1978): Correlation of cholinergic abnormalities with senile plaques and mental test scores in senile dementia. Br Med J 2:1457 1459. Perry R, Jaros E, Irving D, Scoones D, Brown A, McMeekin W, et al (1996): What is the neuropathological basis of dementia associated with Lewy bodies? In: RH Perry, IG McKeith, EK Perry editors. Dementia with Lewy Bodies, Vol 1. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 212 223. Perry RH (1993): Coronal Maps of Brodmann Areas in the Human Brain. London: Wolfe. Perry RH, Irving D, Blessed G, Fairbairn A, Perry EK (1990a): Senile dementia of Lewy body type. A clinically and neuropathologically distinct form of Lewy body dementia in the elderly. J Neurol Sci 95:119 139. Perry RH, Irving D, Tomlinson BE (1990b): Lewy body prevalence in the aging brain: Relationship to neuropsychiatric disorders, Alzheimer-type pathology and catecholaminergic nuclei. J Neurol Sci 100:223 33. Rabins PV, Pearlson GD, Aylward E, Kumar AJ, Dowell K (1991): Cortical magnetic resonance imaging changes in elderly inpatients with major depression. Am J Psychiatry 148:617 620. Salloway S, Malloy P, Kohn R, Gillard E, Duffy J, Rogg J, et al (1996): MRI and neuropsychological differences in early- and late-life-onset geriatric depression. Neurology 46:1567 1574. Sapolsky RM, Krey LC, McEwen BS (1986): The neuroendocrinology of stress and aging: The glucocorticoid cascade hypothesis. Endocr Rev 7:284 301. Sapolsky RM, Plotsky PM (1990): Hypercortisolism and its possible neural bases. Biol Psychiatry 27:937 952. Sheline YI, Sanghavi M, Mintun MA, Gado MH (1999): Depression duration but not age predicts hippocampal volume loss in medically healthy women with recurrent major depression. J Neurosci 19:5034 5043. Starkman MN, Gebarski SS, Berent S, Schteingart DE (1992): Hippocampal formation volume, memory dysfunction, and cortisol levels in patients with Cushing s syndrome. Biol Psychiatry 32:756 765.