Supplemental Material

Similar documents
Visual processing is usually subdivided into an early preattentive

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

Supplemental Information. Gamma and the Coordination of Spiking. Activity in Early Visual Cortex. Supplemental Information Inventory:

Texture Segregation Causes Early Figure Enhancement and Later Ground Suppression in Areas V1 and V4 of Visual Cortex

Repetition Suppression Accompanying Behavioral Priming. in Macaque Inferotemporal Cortex. David B.T. McMahon* and Carl R. Olson

Extrastriate Visual Areas February 27, 2003 A. Roe

How does the ventral pathway contribute to spatial attention and the planning of eye movements?

Model-free detection of synchrony in neuronal spike trains, with an application to primate somatosensory cortex

1 Introduction Synchronous ring of action potentials amongst multiple neurons is a phenomenon that has been observed in a wide range of neural systems

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4.

Oscillations: From Neuron to MEG

To Accompany: Thalamic Synchrony and the Adaptive Gating of Information Flow to Cortex Wang, Webber, & Stanley

Models of Attention. Models of Attention

Perceptual Grouping in a Self-Organizing Map of Spiking Neurons

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey

From feedforward vision to natural vision: The impact of free viewing and clutter on monkey inferior temporal object representations

Beyond bumps: Spiking networks that store sets of functions

Report. Attention Narrows Position Tuning of Population Responses in V1

Attention March 4, 2009

2 INSERM U280, Lyon, France

The detection of visual signals by macaque frontal eye field during masking

Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons

Nov versus Fam. Fam 1 versus. Fam 2. Supplementary figure 1

Analysis of in-vivo extracellular recordings. Ryan Morrill Bootcamp 9/10/2014

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Effects of similarity and history on neural mechanisms of visual selection

A Single Mechanism Can Explain the Speed Tuning Properties of MT and V1 Complex Neurons

Limits to the Use of Iconic Memory

Response Plasticity in Primary Visual Cortex and its Role in Vision and Visuomotor Behaviour: Bottom-up and Top-down Influences

Synchrony and the attentional state

Correlation at the neuron and population levels. Correlation at the neuron and population levels. Recording from LGN/V1.

Physiological Correlates of Perceptual Learning in Monkey V1 and V2

Contextual Influences in Visual Processing

Supplementary Note Psychophysics:

Signal detection theory. Direct comparison of perception and neural activity. ? Choice probability. Visual stimulus. Behavioral judgement

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more

Attention and normalization circuits in macaque V1

On the tracking of dynamic functional relations in monkey cerebral cortex

Attention improves performance primarily by reducing interneuronal correlations

Relationships between Spike Train Spatiotemporal Structure and Cortical Synchronization

Report. A Causal Role for V5/MT Neurons Coding Motion-Disparity Conjunctions in Resolving Perceptual Ambiguity

Priming in Macaque Frontal Cortex during Popout Visual Search: Feature-Based Facilitation and Location-Based Inhibition of Return

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Article. A Growth-Cone Model for the Spread of Object-Based Attention during Contour Grouping

Context-Dependent Changes in Functional Circuitry in Visual Area MT

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences

Each neuron in the primary visual cortex (V1) is activated by

Lecture overview. What hypothesis to test in the fly? Quantitative data collection Visual physiology conventions ( Methods )

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

Commentary on Moran and Desimone's 'spotlight in V4

Self-Organization and Segmentation with Laterally Connected Spiking Neurons

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Processing of the incomplete representation of the visual world

Supporting Information

Cortical Algorithms for Perceptual Grouping

Neural response time integration subserves. perceptual decisions - K-F Wong and X-J Wang s. reduced model

Expansion of MT Neurons Excitatory Receptive Fields during Covert Attentive Tracking

Competitive Mechanisms Subserve Attention in Macaque Areas V2 and V4

Time [ms]

Tai Sing Lee 1,2, Cindy F. Yang 1, Richard D. Romero 1,2 and David Mumford 3

Quantifying the signals contained in heterogeneous neural responses and determining their relationships with task performance

Toward a noninvasive automatic seizure control system with transcranial focal stimulations via tripolar concentric ring electrodes

Areas of the visual cortex are arranged hierarchically, with

The Role of Working Memory Representations in the Control of Attention

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher

Supplementary Motor Area exerts Proactive and Reactive Control of Arm Movements

Object-based vision and attention in primates Carl R Olson

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Evidence of Stereoscopic Surface Disambiguation in the Responses of V1 Neurons

Oscillatory Neuronal Synchronization in Primary Visual Cortex as a Correlate of Stimulus Selection

Dynamics of Response to Perceptual Pop-Out Stimuli in Macaque V1

Reading Neuronal Synchrony with Depressing Synapses

Investigating neural correlates of conscious perception by frequency-tagged neuromagnetic responses

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection

Supplemental Information: Adaptation can explain evidence for encoding of probabilistic. information in macaque inferior temporal cortex

Dendritic compartmentalization could underlie competition and attentional biasing of simultaneous visual stimuli

Introduction to Electrophysiology

Attention activates winner-take-all competition among visual filters

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions

Attention decouples action potentials from the phase of local field potentials in macaque visual cortical area MT

Neuronal responses to plaids

NEURAL CONTROL OF MOVEMENT: ENGINEERING THE RHYTHMS OF THE BRAIN

Consciousness The final frontier!

Responses to Natural Scenes in Cat V1

Attentional Changes in Either Criterion or Sensitivity Are Associated with Robust Modulations in Lateral Prefrontal Cortex

Strength of Gamma Rhythm Depends on Normalization

Supporting Online Material for

Is contrast just another feature for visual selective attention?

CROSSMODAL PLASTICITY IN SPECIFIC AUDITORY CORTICES UNDERLIES VISUAL COMPENSATIONS IN THE DEAF "

A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal

Supplementary Data for "Single-trial learning of novel stimuli by individual. neurons of the human hippocampus-amygdala complex"

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

Neuronal representations of cognitive state: reward or attention?

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex

Object detection in natural scenes by feedback

Transcription:

Supplemental Material Recording technique Multi-unit activity (MUA) was recorded from electrodes that were chronically implanted (Teflon-coated platinum-iridium wires) in the primary visual cortex representing the lower visual field, and positioned 1-2 mm below the cortical surface (see Roelfsema et al., 2004). Supplemental Figure 4a illustrates the method used to record multi-unit activity. Signals from the chronically implanted electrodes were first amplified and then filtered between 750 (18 db/octave) and 5000 Hz (12 db/octave) (Filt1 in Fig. 4a,b). In a number of cases it was possible to record single unit activity (SUA) from the chronically implanted electrodes by using a Schmidt trigger. Generally, however, we recorded multiunit activity. MUA was recorded by a full-wave rectification of the Filt1-signal (taking the absolute value of the voltage) followed by a low-pass filtering step at 500 Hz. MUA reflects the envelope of the Filt1 signal, and it thereby provides an instantaneous measure of the number and size of action potentials of neurons in the vicinity of the electrode tip (Legatt et al., 1980; Brosch et al., 1995) (Fig. 4b). Note that this signal does not include local field potentials (<500 Hz), since these are filtered out in the first step (Filt1). We recently compared MUA to single-unit activity in area V1 during attentiondemanding tasks, and found that MUA provides a reliable estimate of the average singleunit responses (Supèr and Roelfsema, 2005). This is expected, since MUA pools the responses of a number of single neurons in the vicinity of the recording electrode and population responses obtained with this method are therefore the same as population responses obtained with single-unit recordings. Pooling improves the signal-to-noise ratio, which is advantageous if the aim is to measure the average timing of attentional effects. Recordings with a sufficient signal-to-noise ratio were obtained from about 50% of the wires. Before the experiments, the receptive field (RF) borders were measured by determining the onset and offset of the visual response to a slowly moving light bar, for each of 8 movement directions (the method is described in detail in Supèr and Roelfsema, 2005). Median RF size was 0.98º (range 0.45º-1.9º), and average eccentricity was 3.7º

Khayat et al. 2 (range 1.1º-6.9º). Median orientation selectivity ratio (OSR, response to bar of optimal orientation and direction by least effective) was 1.9 (mean 2.1, range 1.2 to 7). We designed the curve stimuli in such a way that the segment in the RF matched as closely as possible the neuron s preferred orientation. In our sample of recording sites, 75% of neurons were therefore stimulated optimally, with a curve segment that was within 0-30 deg of their preferred orientation, whereas 25% were stimulated suboptimally with a curve segment that was within 30-60 deg. To examine whether neuronal response modulation depends on orientation selectivity, we computed the population responses for each category. Figure 5 shows the responses during normal trials of sites that were stimulated with a curve segment oriented optimally (N=44, left panel) and suboptimally (N=15, right panel). In both cases, the response to the target curve was stronger than the response to the distractor curve (p<0.001, paired t-test), and the MI did not differ significantly between the two groups (U-test, P>0.05). Thus, the attentional response modulation also occurs for neurons that are stimulated with a suboptimal orientation. Eye position control A gold ring was implanted under the conjunctiva of one eye to record the eye position with the double magnetic induction method (Bour et al., 1984), which has a resolution better than 0.l deg. The eye position signal was digitized at a rate of more than 500 Hz. During stimulus presentation, the monkeys had to maintain steady fixation within a 1 x1 fixation window centered on the fixation point. Because RFs are relatively small in area V1, small differences in the position of gaze around the fixation point between the normal and switch condition may therefore influence the strength and latency of the response enhancement and suppression. To exclude this possibility, we first removed all trials with microsaccades during stimulus presentation. Then we applied a stratification procedure (Roelfsema et al., 1998), which is illustrated in Fig. 6. Figure 6a shows the distribution of eye positions across trials with stimulus T of the normal condition (Left) and stimulus T D of the switch condition (Center). The stratification procedure removes trials from these two conditions until the eye position distributions are identical (within bins of 0.2º x 0.2º). The stratified eye position distribution is shown in Fig. 6a (Right). Note that the number of remaining trials in the stratified distribution is smaller than the

Khayat et al. 3 number of trials with either stimulus. This is due to the removal of trials with stimulus T in some of the bins and removal of trials with stimulus T D in other bins. A similar analysis of the eye position distributions across trials with stimulus D and stimulus D T is shown in Fig. 6b. Figure 6c shows the population data (N=59) of the same recording sites that are shown in Fig. 3a, but now after the stratification procedure. The response enhancement (light grey area) yielded a modulation index of 0.38 and occurred at a latency of 138 ms after the stimulus switch (lower panel), whereas the response suppression (dark grey area) yielded a modulation index of 0.18 and occurred later, at a latency of 182 ms. We conclude that the magnitude and latency of the response enhancement and suppression are not caused by a systematic difference in gaze position between the normal and switch conditions. Analysis of the latency of the response enhancement Various methods exist to measure the latency of neuronal responses or the modulation thereof. A commonly used procedure is to take the first of a number of time bins that satisfy a significance criterion (e.g. Lennie, 1981; Chelazzi et al., 2001; Maunsell and Gibson, 1992). However, if the difference in neuronal responses between conditions builds up gradually, this procedure yields biased estimates since it is sensitive to the amount of data that was collected. Shorter latencies are obtained if there are more trials. We therefore used an alternative latency estimate that is independent of the number of trials (see below), and is derived by fitting a function f(t) to the difference between the responses evoked by the target and distractor curve (see also Thompson et al., 1996; Roelfsema, et al., 2003). The shape of f(t) was derived from the following two assumptions: [1] the onset of response modulation has a gaussian distribution across trials, and [2] a fraction of the modulation dissipates exponentially. These assumptions yield the following two differential equations: $ m t) / $ t = #" m ( t) + g( t, µ, ) (1) 1 ( 1! for the dissipating modulation, and

Khayat et al. 4 " m ( t) / " t = g( t, µ, ) (2) 2! for the non-dissipating modulation. Here, g(t,µ,σ) is a gaussian density with mean µ and standard deviation σ, and α 1 is the time constant of dissipation. Thus, the total modulation equals f ( t) = k m ( t) + k m ( t) (3) 1 1 2 2 The solution to these equations was fitted to the response difference: f t Ad d Exp t G t 2 2 2 ( ) = /( + 1) # ( µ! + 0.5 "! $! )# (, µ + "!," ) + A/( d + 1) # G( t, µ," ) (4) where the constants k 1 and k 2 of Eq (3) are defined as follows: k1 = Ad /( d + 1) (5) k2 = A/( d + 1) (6) Thus, f(t) depends on 5 parameters: µ, σ, α, A, and d; G(t,µ,σ) is a cumulative gaussian, A is the amplitude of the function, and d determines the fraction of the modulation that dissipates. The latency of the response modulation was defined as the time at which the fitted function reached 33% of its maximum value. This value is arbitrary, but qualitatively similar results were obtained with other criteria (e.g. 25%, 50%, and 75%). With a criterion of 33%, the latency of enhancement and suppression at the population level equaled 144 ms and 210 ms, respectively (see Fig.3a). With criteria of 25%, 50% and 75%, the latency of the response enhancement (suppression) equaled 130 ms (202 ms), 162 ms (221 ms) and 190 ms (238 ms), respectively. We also analyzed the latency at the individual recording sites for which the response enhancement and suppression were both

Khayat et al. 5 significant (p<0.05, N=21; Fig. 3c). The median latency of the response enhancement (suppression) equaled 130 ms (187 ms), 134ms (201ms), 158 ms (223 ms) and 188 ms (250 ms) at criteria of 25%, 33%, 50% and 75%, respectively. A two-sided sign-test revealed that the latency of enhancement was significantly shorter than that of suppression, at each criterion (p<0.01). We investigated how our method depends on the amount of available data. Supplementary Figure 7 compares the latency of enhancement (or suppression) that is computed by fitting a curve onto the difference in responses averaged across 100% of the trials and across 50% of the trials. The analyses include all sites where the response enhancement (i.e. difference in responses between D and D T trials) and the response suppression (i.e. difference in responses between T and T D trials) were both significant (p<0.05, N=21). The latency measure for the enhancement (in red) and suppression (in black) at most recording sites is similar irrespective of the number of trials included in the analyses (p>0.4, two-sided sign-test). The median latency of enhancement equaled 134 and 138 ms as measured on 100% and 50% of trials, respectively. The median latency of suppression equaled 201 and 195 ms as measured on 100% and 50% of trials, respectively. These results show that fitting a function to the response difference indeed provides a reliable estimate of the latency that does not depend strongly on the amount of data. Computation of the confidence interval for the difference in latency To compute a 95% confidence interval for differences in the latency of attentional modulation between enhancement and suppression, a Monte-Carlo procedure was used (Press et al., 1986). This Monte-Carlo (bootstrapping) procedure simulated 10.000 data sets, by randomly selecting 59 recording sites with replacement from the population of all recording sites (N=59). In each data set, the latency of the enhancement and suppression were determined by using the curve-fitting method. This analysis resulted in a distribution of 10.000 latency differences and this distribution was used to compute the 95% confidence interval, which was equal to [39.8ms, 79.7ms].

Khayat et al. 6 References Bour LJ, van Gisbergen JA, Bruijns J, Ottes FP (1984) The double magnetic induction method for measuring eye movements: Results in monkeys and man. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 31:419-427. Brosch M, Bauer R, Eckhorn R (1995) Synchronous high-frequency oscillations in cat area 18. Eur J Neurosci 7:86-95. Chelazzi L, Miller EK, Duncan J, Desimone R (2001) Responses of neurons in macaque area V4 during memory-guided visual search. Cereb. Cortex 11:761-772. Legatt AD, Arezzo J, Vaughan HG (1980) Averaged multiple unit activity as an estimate of phasic changes in local neuronal activity: effects of volume-conducted potentials. J Neurosci Meth 2:203-217. Lennie P (1981) The physiological basis of variations in visual latency. Vision Res 21:815-24. Maunsell JHR, Gibson JR (1992) Visual response latencies in striate cortex of the macaque monkey. J Neurophysiol 68:1332-1344. Press WH, Flannery BP, Teukolsky SA, Vetterling WT (1986) Numerical recipes (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge). Roelfsema PR, Lamme VAF, Spekreijse H (1998) Object-based attention in the primary visual cortex of the macaque monkey. Nature 395:376-381. Roelfsema PR, Khayat PS, Spekreijse H (2003) Subtask sequencing in the primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 100:5467-5472. Roelfsema PR, Lamme VAF, Spekreijse H (2004) Synchrony and covariation of firing rates in the primary visual cortex during contour grouping. Nat Neurosci 7:982-991. Supèr H, Roelfsema PR (2005) Chronic multiunit recordings in behaving animals: advantages and limitations. Prog. Brain Res 147:263-282 Thompson KG, Hanes DP, Bichot NP, Schall JD (1996) Perceptual and motor processing stages identified in the activity of macaque frontal eye field neurons during visual search. J Neurophysiol 76:4040-4055.