Running head: EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 1

Similar documents
Hope: A predictor of successful goal attainment. A Senior Honors Thesis

Investigating Motivation for Physical Activity among Minority College Females Using the BREQ-2

Running head: PREDICTING SOCIAL INTIMACY 1. Predicting Social Intimacy: Exploring Contribution of Romantic Relationships and Interpersonal

Calculate a 95% confidence interval for the mean time spent studying statistics by students in this class.

SPSS Correlation/Regression

David O Malley, Ph.D., LISW Case Western Reserve University Cleveland, Ohio

CHAPTER 10 Educational Psychology: Motivating Students to Learn

Understanding Psychological Distress, Job Satisfaction and Academic Performance in College Students

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

Why Try? Achievement Motivation and Perceived Academic Climate among Latino Youth

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

Cognitive Theories. of motivation. Please take 5 minutes to read over your Unit 5 Case Study. Then Nathan will present.

The Effect of Being a Student-Athlete on Academic Performance

Module 1 CLINICAL PRECEPTOR PRINCIPLES & CLINICAL EDUCATION MODEL

Introduction. Lecture 1. What is Statistics?

The Effects of Societal Versus Professor Stereotype Threats on Female Math Performance

THE MOTlVA'blQNS AND SELF-ESTEEM OF INTRAMURAL SPORTS WWTlClPANBS

Academic Achievement and Emotional Intelligence: Predicting the Successful Transition from High School to University*

Running head: GOALS, ACADEMICS, AND SELF-EFFICACY 1. Self-Efficacy and Goal Orientation and their Association with Academic Achievement

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students

The Effects of Task Difficulty and Reward Opportunity on Motivation

Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and

F2: ORS Scores for a single session sessions

Types of Group Comparison Research. Stephen E. Brock, Ph.D., NCSP EDS 250. Causal-Comparative Research 1

SAMPLE 100 DAY JOURNEY GLOBAL BASELINE REPORT

Personality traits and locus of control as predictors of work motivation Sandhya Puthanpurayil Rajan

The Relation Between Happiness and Number of Siblings. Michael W. Passer. University of Washington

Chapter 1 Chapter 1. Chapter 1 Chapter 1. Chapter 1 Chapter 1. Chapter 1 Chapter 1. Chapter 1 Chapter 1

Perceived Stress Factors and Academic Performance of the Sophomore IT Students of QSU Cabarroguis Campus

Procrastination, Motivation, & Flow

MODEL FOR IMPROVING INTERPERSONAL SKILLS

Dominican University

Teacher satisfaction: some practical implications for teacher professional development models

What Causes Stress in Malaysian Students and it Effect on Academic Performance: A case Revisited

Evaluation of Academic Performance, Academic Motivation, Hope for the Future and Life Satisfaction of Pharmacy Students of a Medical School

Winston-Salem State University

Strengths Use and Self-concordance. in difficult situations. A pilot study. by Sophie Francis

Scientist-Practitioner Interest Changes and Course Outcomes in a Senior Research Psychology Course

Data Collection Worksheet

Connecting Suicidal College Students to Care

Running head: PREDICTABILITY OF DEPRESSION AND ANXIETY 1

Use the above variables and any you might need to construct to specify the MODEL A/C comparisons you would use to ask the following questions.

iafor The International Academic Forum

THE EFFECTS OF WIN:LOSS [W:L] ON INDIVIDUAL MOTIVATION AND PERFORMANCE

The Effect of Procrastination and Stress on Low Effort and High Effort Tasks

ADHD in the Classroom

The Woman Scientist: Communal Goals as Predictors for Women s Interest in STEM

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

Achievement: Approach versus Avoidance Motivation

Anxiety Studies Division Annual Newsletter

The Nature of Intrinsic Motivation and How to Support It

SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS

Title: Validation of the Comprehensive Feeding Practices Questionnaire with parents of 10-to-12-year-olds

International Journal of Management (IJM), ISSN (Print), ISSN (Online), Volume 5, Issue 1, January (2014), IAEME

Hearing, Deaf, and Hard-of-Hearing Students Satisfaction with On-Line Learning

Minnesota s Dental Therapist Workforce, 2016 HIGHLIGHTS FROM THE 2016 DENTAL THERAPIST SURVEY

Do misery and happiness both love company? The emotional consequences of listening to experiences shared by others.

Correspondence should be addressed to Krisztian Jozsa;

for Music Therapy Supervision

Research for proposing the subcategories of the volitional element for the ARCS-V model

Physics Department Student Climate Survey Report

Analysis and Interpretation of Data Part 1

The role of intrinsic motivation in the academic pursuits of nontraditional students

Study in psychology provides multiple perspectives

Highlights of the Research Consortium 2002 Non-Clinical Sample Study

Leisure Motivation of Participants and Nonparticipants in Campus Recreational Sports Programs

PSYCHOLOGY (PSYC) Courses. Psychology (PSYC) 1

MY SIMPLE VIEW OF LIFE

Psychology Department Assessment

Psychological Science

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

GERONTOLOGY MINOR & GERONTOLOGY CERTIFICATE

Perceived Emotional Aptitude of Clinical Laboratory Sciences Students Compared to Students in Other Healthcare Profession Majors

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA

Improving the Performance of Failing Students by Overcoming Their Self-Serving Attributional Biases

POLICY. Institutional Research Projects/Data Requests #7220

The Potential of Survey-Effort Measures to Proxy for Relevant Character Skills

Annex A: Estimating the number of people in problem debt while being treated for a mental health crisis

Hunter College of CUNY. Undergraduate Psychology New Curriculum Structure

Motivation for Treatment and Therapeutic Alliance in Residential Care

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

CHAPTER LEARNING OUTCOMES

Motivational correlates of need for cognition

Running head: RELIGIOUS STRENGTH AND PTG 1

ACADEMIC MOTIVATION AND ADJUSTMENT OF SECONDARY SCHOOL STUDENTS AS RELATED TO SELF-CONFIDENCE AND CERTAIN DEMOGRAPHIC VARIABLES

AlcoholEdu for College

Measuring Perceived Social Support in Mexican American Youth: Psychometric Properties of the Multidimensional Scale of Perceived Social Support

Minnesota s Dental Therapist Workforce, 2016 HIGHLIGHTS FROM THE 2016 DENTAL THERAPIST SURVEY

Title: The Relationship between Locus of Control and Academic Level and Sex of Secondary School Students

Job Choice and Post Decision Dissonance1

The Grateful Disposition: Links to Patterns of Attribution for Positive Events. Sharon L. Brion. Michael E. McCullough. Southern Methodist University

International Journal of Educational Advancement (2011) 10, doi: /ijea

Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers

Massachusetts Alzheimer s Disease & Other Dementias Online Training Program 2017

College Student Self-Assessment Survey (CSSAS)

An Examination of Goal Orientation between Genders An Exploratory Study

RUNNING HEAD: RAPID EVALUATION OF BIG FIVE 1

REPLICATION: GOING GREEN TO BE SEEN 1. Replication of: Griskevicius, Tybur, & Van den Bergh, (2010), Going Green to be seen: Status,

Factors Related to High Risk Drinking and Subsequent Alcohol-Related Consequences Among College Students

Transcription:

Running head: EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 1 The relationship between gender, study habits, plans after college and Generalized Expectancy for Success and Frequency of Self-Reinforcement Lauren Shepherd PSYC 450 Project University of Nebraska-Lincoln

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 2 Abstract The Frequency of Self-Reinforcement Questionnaire (FSRQ) and the Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale (GESS) are shown to be linked to depression and overall life satisfaction. This study looked at the relationship between gender, amount of time spent studying per week, and plans after college as they relate to both the FSRQ and GESS. Data was collected in the form of five surveys from 391 undergraduate students at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln. A factorial analysis revealed significant three-way interactions between gender, study time, and plans after college for both the GESS and FSRQ.

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 3 The relationship between gender, study habits, plans after college and Generalized Expectancy for Success and Frequency of Self-Reinforcement The purpose of the current study is to evaluate if gender, plans after college, and amount of time spent studying attribute to the likelihood of a person scoring higher on both the Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale and the Frequency of Self-Reinforcement Questionnaire. Story et al (2009) found that high intrinsic motivation was positively related to frequency of self-reinforcement. Intrinsic motivation, in this study, meant that students were more likely to focus on mastering material that leads to more persistence and classroom involvement, while extrinsically motivated students worry about how their performance compares to others. Additionally, the study found if an individual has high intrinsic or extrinsic motivation, they will have a relatively high score on the Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale. This study found that intrinsically motivated students scored higher the FSRQ and GESS (Story, 2009). Snyder et at (2002) states that scores on the Hope scale are positively correlated with scores for the GESS. Their study found that those students who had higher GPA (broken into three levels) had a positive correlation with scores on the Hope Scale. Those students who scored higher on the Hope Scale were also more likely to graduate than to be academically dismissed. This study did not find any significant gender differences. Heiby (1983) completed two experiments in order to determine the reliability and validity of the most commonly used frequency of self-reinforcement questionnaire for two different tasks. One task included a 12-item analogies problem and the second was an anagram test comprised of seven different anagrams. Her results supported her research hypothesis and found

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 4 that, responses to the FST questionnaire are reflective of a person s self-praise behavior on an experimental task. Schill (1991) found that men who were more self-defeating used less self-reinforcement and were more depressed than their regular counterpoints. He used his own scale to test 51 male psychology students to assess the scores for both the Self-defeating Personality Scale and the Frequency of self-reinforcement scale (Schill, 1991). These results support previous works, such as Beck (1959), which found a significant relationship between self-defeating patterns with depression. These five studies show the significance and validity of both the GESS and FSRQ. They also tell how both GESS and FSRQ can influence factors such as depression, life satisfaction, GPA, and intrinsic motivation. All of these factors could have implications with amount of time spent studying and plans after college. The current study sought to find any interactions between these variables and both the GESS and FSRQ, as well as converge upon the aforementioned research. The first hypothesis was that gender, time spent studying, and plans after college would influence scores on the Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale. The second hypothesis stated that gender, time spent studying, and plans after college would influence scores on the Frequency of Self-Reinforcement Questionnaire. Next, it was hypothesized that those who spent more time studying and planned to go to graduate school would have the highest score on the FSRQ, with those planning to go to grad school having higher scores overall than those who planned to work after college. This hypothesis would expand upon the research done by Heiby (1983) and Schill (1991). We predicted those individuals with lower self-defeating scores would be more likely to go on to graduate school, and have higher scores on the FSRQ. The fourth hypothesis stated that

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 5 for each increased level of time spent studying, there would be an increase in the score on the Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale. This hypothesis seeks to expand on the research done by Story (2009), who found that students that were intrinsically motivated sought to master school-related tasks and scored higher on the GESS. Therefore, we would like to find out if those who spent more time seeking to master courses by studying more would score higher on the GESS.

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 6 Method Participants Survey data was collected from 391 undergraduate students at the University of Nebraska-Lincoln. Most of the population included friends and associates of students enrolled in an introductory stats course. The analysis only included the data from those who reported a European-American background (91.6% of population, 358). There was relatively equal distribution of gender, including 56.5% females and 43.5% males (221 and 170, respectively). Materials All of the participants completed a self-report questionnaire in a natural setting. This questionnaire included demographic information such as age, gender, ethnicity, plans after college, and average time spent studying per week. They also completed another set of five scales that included the Frequency of Self Reinforcement Questionnaire (FSRQ) and the Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale (GESS). Only the surveys listed were included in the analysis, though the participants completed all five. Procedures All of the investigators completed one survey themselves before surveying participants. Our investigators sought out other undergraduate students to complete 5 surveys for each researcher. Age was not a factor when seeking students. The researchers scored and collated surveys, entered the data into a database that consisted of multiple sections of data, formulated hypotheses, and completed the appropriate analysis, which included two between-groups 3-way ANOVAs. The amount of time spent studying was broken into three categories: 9 hours per week or less (low), 9.1-14 hours per week (medium), and more than 14 hours per week (high).

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 7 Results Refer to Table 1 for univariate statistics and Chart 1 for the three-way interaction. The first hypothesis stated that gender, time spent studying, and plans after college would influence scores on the Generalized Expectancy for Success scale. There was an effect between GESS and gender, time spent studying, and plans after college, F(2,342)=5.607, MSe=525.061, p=.004), which fully supports the research hypothesis. However, this effect is misleading for males and females that plan to find work and study the least or medium amount (105.03=108.70; 107.14=102.97). It is descriptive for the most amount of time spent studying, with females scoring higher than males (90.03<105.30). For those that plan to go to graduate school, there was no difference between males and females who studied the least (103.5=105.83) and those who studied the most (105.4=106.67). There is a difference for those who study a medium amount such that females scored higher than males (91.60<112.09). There was also a main effect of gender, F(1,342)=5.597, MSe=525.061, p=.019. The two category main effect of gender is fully descriptive. That result is contrary to the results found in Snyder s (2002) study. Although there was a main effect of gender, there was not a significant main effect of plans after college (F(1,342)=.102, MSe= 525.061, p >.10) or study time (F(2,342)=.620, MSe= 525.061, p >.10) as they relate to GESS. An analysis of the cell means shows the insignificant effect is descriptive for all categories of the two effects. Refer to Chart 2 for the three-way interaction. The second hypothesis stated that gender, time spent studying, and plans after college would influence scores on the Frequency of Self- Reinforcement Questionnaire such that females who studied the most and planned to go onto further schooling would score the highest. There was an interaction between FSRQ and gender, time spent studying, and plans after college, F(2,342)=3.109, MSe=161.54, p=.046). This effect

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 8 is misleading for all of the folks who plan to find work, as there are no significant differences for each increased level of studying. It is also misleading for those who plan to go to graduate school and study the most (61.40=59.40) and the least (56.08=59.58). There was a significant difference such that females scored higher on the FSRQ than did males for the medium amount of study time (51.56<59.63). The third hypothesis stated that those who spent more time studying and planned to go to graduate school would have the highest score on the FSRQ, with those planning to go to grad school having higher scores overall. There was a relationship between plans after college and time spent studying, F(2,342)=5.472, MSe=161.54, p=.005. This interaction is misleading, as those planning to attend graduate school who studied the most did have higher FSRQ scores than those who studied a medium amount (55.59 < 60.40), but there was not a significant difference between those who studied the least and the most (57.83=60.4). Those who planned to find work had higher scores for the medium amount of time spent studying, and there was no significant difference between the least amount and the medium amount of study time (60.74=61.77). There was a difference between the least amount and greatest amount, but the folks in the greatest amount of time spent studying had a lower FSRQ score than did the folks in the least amount (60.74 > 56.18). The hypothesis that those who spent more time studying and planned to go to graduate school would have higher scores on the FSRQ is partially supported, while the second part stating that those planning to go to graduate school having higher scores overall is not supported. However, there were no significant main effects between plans (F(1,342)=1.187, MSe= 161.541, p >.10), study time (F(2,342)=.143, MSe= 161.541, p >.10), or gender (F(1,342)=2.100, MSe= 161.541, p=.15) and frequency of self-reinforcement. There was also

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 9 not an interaction between plans after college and gender (F(1,342)=1.187, MSe= 161.541, p >.10) or study time and gender (F(1,342)=1.187, MSe= 161.541, p >.10) on scores on the FSRQ. Further examination of the estimated marginal means revealed that these insignificant interactions were unfortunately descriptive for all levels in each interaction, in that there was not an interaction. The fourth hypothesis stated that for each increased level of time spent studying, there would be an increase in the score on the Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale. This hypothesis is not supported, F(1,342)=.105, MSe=525.06, p=.749. However, the interaction between plans after college and study time was marginally significant as it relates to GESS (F(2,342)=2.427, MSe= 525.061, p=.09) Discussion The results provided support for two of the research hypotheses. The first hypothesis that gender, study time, and plans after college would have an interaction with scores on the GESS was partially supported, as the results only supported those that planned to find work and studied the most, as well as those who planned to go to graduate school and studied a medium amount had significant differences between genders, with females studying more in both conditions. The effect between gender, plans, study time as they relate to GESS is descriptive for most amount of time spent studying for those who plan to find work, as females scored higher than males. It is also descriptive for those who plan to go to graduate school and study a medium amount, such that females scored higher than males. Previous research did not find any main effects of gender, while the current did. These results are interesting in that they found a significant effect for gender on two different success scales. Further investigation into gender

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 10 would be beneficial, and consideration of other gender categories may also be beneficial to the field. There was a marginally significant result between plans after college and study time as they relate to scores on the GESS. When gender was added to the comparison, a significant interaction was found. This result is interesting since previous research found no gender difference. Per these results, gender seems to be influencing scores on the GESS. The interaction between FSRQ and gender, study time, and plans after college was misleading for all participants who plan to find work after college, as seen in the graph. The effect is only descriptive for those who plan to find graduate school and study a medium amount, as females scored higher than males. These effects were not what we had hypothesized. Since previous research had found a relationship between scores on the FSRQ and overall life satisfaction, further studies including a life satisfaction scale may be beneficial. The influence between life satisfaction and both the FSRQ and GESS would help solidify and enhance the current study. Plans after college and study time did have a significant interaction, though it was misleading. Contrary to the hypothesis, those interested in graduate school studied the least and the most did not score differently on the FSRQ. Previous research found that those with less selfdefeating tendencies had higher scores on the FSRQ. This did not influence the participant s plans after college, however. In addition, Story et al (2009) found that those who sought to master school-related tasks scored higher on the FSRQ. Further research could expand on those findings to investigate if there is a difference in mastery on school vs. work mastery, and if either or both influence scores on the FSRQ.

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 11 In regard to increased study time leading to increased scores on the GESS, we found no interaction. This is interesting as other studies did find an interaction between higher GPA and GESS, and one would believe that studying more would influence GPA. Additional studies on GPA and FSRQ could also further the reach of these findings. Overall, a further investigation into how study time, gender, and plans after college affect self-reinforcement and expectancy for success would be beneficial. Further studies could study interactions for other ethnicities, as this study only dealt with those who reported a European- American background, as well as gender. The increased demographic information, as well as more information on the effects of study time on GPA would further the results stated above.

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 12 Chart 1:

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 13 Chart 2:

EXPECTANCY FOR SUCCESS AND SELF-REINFORCEMENT 14 Bibliography Beck, A. T. (1967) Depression: clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects. New York: Harper & Row. Heiby, E. M. (1983). Assessment of frequency of self-reinforcement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(6), 1304. Fibel, B., & Hale, W. D. (1978). The Generalized Expectancy for Success Scale: A new measure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 46(5), 924. Schill, T., & Kramer, J. (1991). Self-defeating personality, self-reinforcement, and depression. Psychological Reports, 69(1), 137-138. doi:10.2466/pr0.1991.68.1.137 Snyder, C. R., Shorey, H. S., Cheavens, J., Pulvers, K. M., Adams III, V. H., & Wiklund, C. (2002). Hope and academic success in college. Journal of educational psychology, 94(4), 820. Story, P. A., Hart, J. W., Stasson, M. F., & Mahoney, J. M. (2009). Using a two-factor theory of achievement motivation to examine performance-based outcomes and self-regulatory processes.personality and Individual Differences, 46(4), 391-395. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2008.10.023