Evaluation of the. Learn2Live Road Safety Intervention for Young People. Plymouth Pavilions event. 25 th November 2011.

Similar documents
AN EVALUATION OF A PREDRIVER TRAINING PROGRAM USING THE THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOR (TPB)

Intention to consent to living organ donation: an exploratory study. Christina Browne B.A. and Deirdre M. Desmond PhD

Crash Risk Analysis of Distracted Driving Behavior: Influence of Secondary Task Engagement and Driver Characteristics

THE POTENTIAL IMPACTS OF LUAS ON TRAVEL BEHAVIOUR

Risk Factors Associated with Speeding Offences Among Young Western Australian Drivers

The Effectiveness of Pre-Driver Training

Inhibitory Control and Peer Passengers Predict Risky Driving in Young Novice Drivers - A Simulator Study

Attitude = Belief + Evaluation. TRA/TPB and HBM. Theory of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior. TRA: Constructs TRA/TPB

Measuring Road Safety Culture in Relation to Speed Susan Cambridge and Tony Francis Francis and Cambridge Ltd, Christchurch

EVALUATING CHANGES IN THE DRIVING BEHAVIOR OF YOUNG DRIVERS A FEW YEARS AFTER LICENSURE USING IN-VEHICLE DATA RECORDERS

YOUNG PEOPLE, DRINKING HABITS, TRANSPORTATION AND PEER RELATIONS. A QUESTIONNAIRE STUDY

Keywords: Subjective Norms, Referent, Motivation to Comply, Intention, Travel Mode Choice, Public Transport, Green Sustainable Transportation.

COMPARISON OF SELF-REPORTED AND COMPUTER-BASED HAZARD PERCEPTION SKILLS AMONG NOVICE AND EXPERIENCED DRIVERS

EXPERIMENTAL EFFECTS OF PRE-DRIVE AROUSAL ON TEENAGE SIMULATED DRIVING PERFORMANCE IN THE PRESENCE OF A TEENAGE PASSENGER.

Sampling Methodology

COGNITIVE AND PSYCHOMOTOR CORRELATES OF HAZARD PERCEPTION ABILITY AND RISKY DRIVING

Understanding Tourist Environmental Behavior An Application of the Theories on Reasoned Action Approach

Australasian Road Safety Research, Policing and Education Conference October 2012, Wellington, New Zealand

Understanding binge drinking among young people: an application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour

Students Responses to the RACQ Docudrama Program. Abstract

The role of attitudes in transportation studies

RESEARCH ARTICLE. GENDER DIFFERENCES IN RISK TAKING: EVIDENCE FROM A MANAGEMENT INSTITUTE Amit Kapoor 1

Reactions of teenagers and parents to a zero alcohol tolerance law

Identifying beliefs underlying pre-drivers intentions to take risks: an application of the theory of planned behaviour

Measurement of Constructs in Psychosocial Models of Health Behavior. March 26, 2012 Neil Steers, Ph.D.

Motivating behaviour change among young drivers: Recent findings on developmental issues and the role of parents

Determinants of Oral Health Behavior in different cultures

THE INFLUENCE OF RISK PERCEPTION AND SELF-ASSESSED DRIVING ABILITIES ON THE BEHAVIOR OF YOUNG DRIVERS

Impact of Social Role (Actor-Observer) On Attribution

Determinants of Adolescent Alcohol Abuse What role do older siblings and parents play?

Perceptions of Risk Factors for Road Traffic Accidents

Effects of passenger and alcohol on the driving behaviour of young males and females

EPILEPSY AND DRIVING- A POSITION PAPER OF EPILEPSY SOUTH AFRICA DEVELOPED IN MARCH 2016

Analyzing Determinants of Educational Methods in Environmental Education by using the Theory of Planned Behavior.

Relationships between young drivers personality. characteristics, risk perceptions, and driving behaviour

Accident Analysis and Prevention

Impact of Information, Goal Setting, Feedback & Rewards on Energy Use Behaviours: A Review & Meta-Regression Analysis of Field Experiments

The effect of training approaches and feedback for young and novice drivers on the. relationship between the actual and perceived speed

Subject: The Case for a Provincial 0.00% BAC Limit for All Drivers Under the Age of 21

Comparing 3 Means- ANOVA

8. Attitudes and Beliefs

EPHE 575. Exercise Adherence. To Do. 8am Tuesday Presentations

Impact of mailed feedback on speeding behaviours of convicted male drivers: A brief intervention.

ATTITUDES, BELIEFS, AND TRANSPORTATION BEHAVIOR

The Association Between Risk-taking Behavior and Helmet Use Among Motorcyclist

A critical analysis of an applied psychology journal about the effects of driving fatigue: Driver fatigue and highway driving: A simulator study.

UNDERSTANDING AND INFLUENCING PUPILS CHOICES AS THEY PREPARE TO LEAVE SCHOOL EXECUTIVE SUMMARY. Dr Kathryn Asbury. University of York

The effects of age on crash risk associated with driver distraction

Exploring the Utility of an Extended Theory of Planned Behavior Framework for School-Based Gambling Prevention Programs

Keywords: consultation, drug-related problems, pharmacists, Theory of Planned Behavior

Cultural Differences and Traffic Safety.

How to change young drivers attitudes and behaviour with respect to drivingwhile-intoxicated

The damaged product, named Parmigiano- Reggiano Terremotato (PR-T, namely Parmigiano-Reggiano damaged by earthquake ) was:

Undiagnosed ADHD Among Unionized Drivers in Ghana: Public Health and Policy Implications

Transportation Research Part F

Perceived usefulness. Intention Use E-filing. Attitude. Ease of use Perceived behavioral control. Subjective norm

Novice Driver Simulation Training Potential for Improving Hazard Perception and Self-Confidence While Lowering Speeding Risk Attitudes for Young Males

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site

The moderating impact of temporal separation on the association between intention and physical activity: a meta-analysis

Hazard Perception and Distraction in Novice Drivers: Effects of 12 Months Driving Experience

Mark Asbridge, PhD Donald Langille, MD Jennifer Cartwright, MA. Department of Community Health and Epidemiology Dalhousie University

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 143 ( 2014 ) CY-ICER 2014

This is the author s version of a work that was submitted/accepted for publication in the following source:

EFFECTS OF DISTRACTION TYPE, DRIVER AGE, AND ROADWAY ENVIRONMENT ON REACTION TIMES AN ANALYSIS USING SHRP-2 NDS DATA

TEEN TEXTING WHILE DRIVING: FACTORS INFLUENCING THIS EPIDEMIC BEHAVIOR

Explaining binge drinking among adolescent males using the Theory of Planned Behaviour

Title: The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) and Texting While Driving Behavior in College Students MS # Manuscript ID GCPI

An application of the prototype willingness model to drivers speeding behaviour

Predictors & Training

Walking when intoxicated: An investigation of the factors which influence individuals drink walking intentions

Factors influencing alcohol use among Korean adolescents: A comparison between middle and high school students

User Experience: Findings from Patient Telehealth Survey

Improving Driving Safety for Teenagers with Attention Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)

Why Is Theory Important?

REDUCING MOBILE PHONE USE WHILE DRIVING IN SOUTH AUSTRALIA

Testing an extended theory of planned behavior to predict young people s intentions to join a bone marrow donor registry

RSA Alcohol and Driving Research 2017

Race and willingness to cooperate with the police: The roles of quality of contact, attitudes towards the behaviour and subjective norms

Predicting and facilitating upward family communication as a mammography promotion strategy

REDUCING FRIDAY ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION AMONG MODERATE, WOMEN DRINKERS: EVALUATION OF A BRIEF EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTION

The Road Safety Monitor Drowsy Driving

Survey Methodology DELIVERABLE 2-3 P8 ABDN UNI

Heavy Smokers', Light Smokers', and Nonsmokers' Beliefs About Cigarette Smoking

Gender differences in condom use prediction with Theory of Reasoned Action and Planned Behaviour: the role of self-efficacy and control

Key Points For Physicians To Review With Adolescents and Adults Who Have ADHD

I just didn t see it It s all about hazard perception. Dr. Robert B. Isler Associate Professor School of Psychology University of Waikato Hamilton

Ontario Injury Prevention Resource Centre.

VISITOR'S PERCEPTIONS OF THEIR OWN IMPACTS AT A SPECIAL EVENT

How effective is deterrence theory in explaining driver behaviour: A case study of unlicensed driving. Barry Watson

INVESTIGATION SLEEPLESS DRIVERS

Safer driving agreements in the Australian context: Can they be effective?

Perception and Processing of Safe Driving Messages: A Multi-Method Approach

Journal of Exclusive Management Science November Vol 6 Issue 11 ISSN

The End of the Decline in Drink Driving in Britain?

To the Point of Distraction

UNDERSTANDING EXERCISE INTENTIONS AMONG WOMEN EXERCISING IN FITNESS CLASSES: AN APPLICATION OF THE THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOUR

Normative Outcomes Scale: Measuring Internal Self Moderation

C.J. (2004) 2004: ISBN

The Relationship Between State-Trait Anxiety and Students' Sense of Social Self-Efficacy

Can Multimodal Real Time Information Systems Induce a More Sustainable Mobility?

Transcription:

Evaluation of the Learn2Live Road Safety Intervention for Young People Plymouth Pavilions event 25 th November 2011 Research by Emma Dunnett, Amy Goodwin, Georgina Hobson, Claire Hubbard, Daniel Orton, Jennifer Wing. Supervised and summary by Dr Cris Burgess Psychology, College of Life & Environmental Sciences University of Exeter 1

Executive summary Road traffic collisions are the leading cause of death amongst 15-24 year-olds in the UK (ONS, 2011). The presence of adolescent passengers in vehicles driven by adolescents is associated with a significant increase in crash likelihood (e.g. Simons-Morton, Learner and Singer, 2005). However, it has been demonstrated that the presence of a passenger can encourage road-safety appropriate behaviours in young drivers (Simons-Morton et al, 2011). Thus, influencing young peoples behavior when they are travelling as passengers in cars driven by one of their contemporaries is likely to result in positive road safety outcomes. Method The current study evaluates the Learn2Live road safety intervention for young people, using an online survey. The study set out to assess the effects of attendance on young peoples intentions to behave in a road safety-appropriate manner when travelling as a passenger. Measures were collected in the two weeks prior to attendance at Learn2Live (T1) and within a two-week period following attendance (T2). The measures were based on components of the Theory of Planned Behaviour (Ajzen, 1991) in order to assess the effect of attendance on the behavioural intentions reported by attendees (please see the Explanatory Notes, p.7, for descriptions of the TPB components). 1500 6th form college students attended the Learn2Live presentation evaluated in this study. Of those, 900 students completed the pre-intervention (T1) questionnaire. Once respondents with incomplete or inappropriate responses were removed, 529 valid cases were available at T1. Of these respondents, 126 students provided complete data sets for the post-intervention measure at T2. Thus 126 participants contributed to the study, 65 female and 61 male, with a mean age of 16.27 years. Only 6 participants reported having a driving license, but over three-quarters of the sample reported travelling as a passenger in a car at least 3 times per week or more. Only 37 participants had ever been involved in a road traffic collision. Results The effectiveness of the intervention was assessed using a paired samples t-test comparing scores on the T1 scale and T2 scale, for each factor of the TPB. Lower scores on questionnaire items represented less risky and more safety-appropriate responses. Thus significantly lower scores at T2 than at T1 suggest road safety-appropriate changes occur as a result of the Learn2Live intervention in the behavioural intentions, perceived control, subjective norms, beliefs, and attitudes of its attendees (Table 1). In order to explain why attendance to the Learn2Live intervention increased safety appropriate behavioural intentions we conducted two regression analysis using changes in components of the TPB as regressors. The first was conducted to determine the relative contributions of changes in the perceived social norms of friends and family and changes in behavioural beliefs on changes in attitudes. While controlling for change in social desirability, the regression model described 32.2% of the variance in change in attitude (R 2 adj=29%) and was statistically significant (F 4,90 = 10.21, p= <.001). The second regression model then determined the contribution of attitude change and changes in perceived behavioural control, on changes in behavioural intention. The model containing these factors described 37.2% of the variance in behavioural intent (R 2 adj=35.3%) and was again, statistically significant (F 3,102 = 19.57, p= <. 001). Both models excluded case 588, which was shown in preliminary analysis to be a significant outlier. The relationships and respective contributions of each factor of the TPB are graphically represented as a path analysis in Figure 1 (overleaf; please see Explanatory Notes, p.7) and summarized in Key Findings, p.6. 2

Variable T1 Mean (s.d.) T2 Mean (s.d.) N Lower confidence interval (95%) Upper confidence interval (95%) t d.f. p Behavioural Intention 15.25 (4.55) 13.37 (4.81) 126 1.09 2.67 4.71 125 <.001 Perceived Behavioural Control 19.1 (4.84) 17.15 (5.32) 125 1.18 2.71 5.01 124 <.001 Behavioural Beliefs 16.95 (3.9) 14.92 (4.22) 123 1.35 2.72 5.86 122 <.001 Social Norms of Friends 18.12 (4.45) 15.93 (5.1) 121 1.41 2.99 5.5 120 <.001 Social Norms of Family Attitude 13.15 (3.81) 28.00 (5.08) 12.23 (4.16) 24.78 (6.11) Table 1. Change in TPB component scores, T1 to T2. 122.333 1.50 3.11 121 <.001 120 2.14 4.31 5.91 119 <.001 High score indicates risky response, low score indicates road safety-appropriate response Figure 1. Path-analysis showing the relative contributions of the TPB components to behavioural intention. ** = p<0.01, * = p<0.05. Generalisability Although these findings are based on only 8.7% of the target population (126 of circa 1500 attendees), there is some evidence that suggest the respondents were representative of the T1-only sample (n=529, 35% of attendees), thus widening the generalisability of these findings to the T1-only group. 3

Figure 2. Behavioural Intention scores, T1 (before intervention) vs T2 (after intervention), by gender Gender Differences Male respondents report significantly less appropriate Behavioural Intention scores than girls before attendance at Learn2Live (Boys: M=17.5, s.d.=5.1; Girls: M=15.1, s.d.=4.7; t 527 =5.74, p<.001). In addition, female respondents report larger appropriate change in behavioural Intention scores (Figure 2), though the difference between male and female respondents in terms of this change is not statistically significant (F 1,124 =2.15, p=.145, n.s.). In order to confirm the effect of gender on Behavioural Intention, a regression analysis was conducted. This showed that while gender explained just 7.4% of the variance in Behavioural Intention (R 2 adj= 6.6%), the model was statistically significant (F 1,125 =9.87, p=.002). Females were found to have scores significantly lower by 2.46, than men (t 125 = 3.14, p =.002). This gender difference was also significant at T2 (F 1,166 =17.77, p<.001), with females reporting scores 2.86 lower than males, suggesting that males had less appropriate Behavioural Intentions after the intervention. 4

Figure 3. Path-analysis showing the relative contributions of the TPB components to behavioural intention, for only male participants (n=61). ** = p<0.01, * = p<0.05. Figure 4. Path-analysis showing the relative contributions of the TPB components to behavioural intention, for only female participants (n=65). ** = p<0.01, * = p<0.05. Separate path analyses carried out for males and females indicate that the influences on Behavioural Intentions are rather different for each gender (Figures 3 and 4). Whilst males report a strong influence of perceived norms (both friends and family) on attitudes, resulting in changes in intentions, females report a greater influence of behavioural beliefs and perceived behavioural control. Female respondents report being less influenced by changes in the perceptions of the social norms of their family than do males. 5

Key findings Does Learn2Live work? Results suggest that the Learn2Live intervention was successful in appropriately modifying the behavioural intentions of attendees in relation to their passenger-related behaviours. The mean difference is relatively small, but consistent across the sample. The proportion of variance in change in behavioural intentions arising from TPB components (37.2%) is comparable with the 39% found by Armitage and Connor (2001). Why does it work? Gender differences are apparent, with the male respondents consistently more risky than females at both stages of the research. The changes in the behavioural intentions of males are mainly due to more appropriate attitudes towards road safety after Learn2Live attendance, those due to changes in perceived subjective norms of friends and family members. Females appear more influenced by greater perceived behavioural control, as well as changes in attitudes, themselves due to changes in perceived subjective norms of their friends. The attitudes of male respondents become more appropriate as a result of changes in the perceived norms of family and friends in almost equal measure. They care what both groups think of them and appear to follow what is seen by them as normal. However, the effect of perceived norms on female respondents appears to rest entirely on their friends, possibly revealing differences in social independence in the relatively young sample. In addition, female respondents behavioural beliefs influence their intentions. They appear to be more certain after the intervention that if they ask the driver to behave appropriately, the driver will respond positively to the request. As well as this, the intervention appears to increase the sense in female respondents that they are able to make requests of the driver to drive more safely, what may be described as empowerment. This is likely a reflection of gender differences in social confidence and self-efficacy in the specific environment of a moving vehicle that is reflected here. The intervention appears to have increased that social confidence in female respondents. Future research The major improvement would be the inclusion of a control group: a sample of young people who do not experience any form of intervention between T1 and T2. The addition of a longer-term follow-up measure would assess the degree of effect attenuation noted by Poulter and McKenna (2010) within their evaluation of the young driver-focused Safe Drive Stay Alive intervention. Also, the literature reports gender differences in behavioural intentions when a same- or different-sex driver is at the wheel and future evaluations could investigate this possibility, thus providing more specific focus for some of the material in Learn2Live. In addition, the TPB component of anticipated regret should be included, along with a valid measure of habitual responding, shown to have a substantial influence on actual behavior. Finally, a longerterm follow-up would indicate the longevity of the effects of attendance at Learn2Live. 6

Explanatory notes Attitude Behavioural beliefs Behavioural intention Norms Path analysis Generally defined as a cognitive orientation towards an attitude object, either strong or weak, positive or negative, in support of or not. The extent to which a respondent believes that behaving in a particular way will result in certain consequences. In this context, the extent to which respondents belief that they can have a positive influence on the behaviour of the driver. How the respondent believes he/she will behave in future, how he/she intends to behave in a given situation. How respondents believe people close to them view particular behaviours, whether they approve or disapprove. A technique used here to present the statistical relationships between TPB components. The arrow indicates the direction of influence, the thickness of the arrow represents the strength of influence and the sign of the coefficient (+ or -) shows whether the influence is positive or negative. Perceived behavioural control The extent to which a respondent believes his/her behavior is purely voluntary, compared with, for example, peer pressure to behave in a particular way, threat of embarrassment or ridicule. 7

References Ajzen, I. (1991) The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Process, 50, 179-211. Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471-499. Office of National Statistics, ONS (2011) Mortality statistics: Deaths registered in 2010 (series DR), Table 8. Retruieved 6/3/12 from http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/publications/re-reference_tables. html. Poulter, D.R. & McKenna, F.P. (2010) Evaluating the effectiveness of a road safety intervention for predrivers: An application of the Theory of Planned Behaviour. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 163-181. Simons-Morton, B.G., Lerner, N., & Singer, J. (2005) The observed effects of teenage passengers on the risky driving behavior of teenage drivers. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 37(6), 973-982. Simons-Morton, B.G., Ouimet, M.C., Zhang, Z., Klauer, S.E., Lee, S.E., Whang, J., Cheng, R., Albert, P., & Dingus, T.A., (2011) The effects of passengers and friends on risky driving and crashes/near crashes among novice teenagers. Journal of Adolescent Health, 49, 587-593. 8