Stimulus Typicality Determines How Broadly Fear Is Generalized

Similar documents
Author. Published. Journal Title. Copyright Statement. Downloaded from. Link to published version. Griffith Research Online

Neurobehavioral Mechanisms of Fear Generalization in PTSD. Lei Zhang. Duke University, Durham Veteran Affairs Medical Center

JOSEPH E. DUNSMOOR, PhD New York University New York, NY

Prof. Anagnostaras, Lecture 7: Fear

Macmillan Publishers Limited. All rights reserved

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

Running head: INSTRUCTIONS IN FEAR CONDITIONING 1. A review on the effects of verbal instructions in human fear conditioning: Empirical

Relations between premise similarity and inductive strength

Safe From Harm: Learned, Instructed, and Symbolic Generalization Pathways of Human. Threat-Avoidance. Baltimore, MD 21205, USA , USA.

Emotion I: General concepts, fear and anxiety

Safe From Harm: Learned, Instructed, and Symbolic Generalization Pathways of Human Threat-Avoidance

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Distinguishing between Category-based and Similarity-based Induction

OBJECTIVES. To examine the influence of conditioned fear on pain threshold

Generalization of Human Fear Acquisition and Extinction within a Novel Arbitrary Stimulus Category

A Unique Safety Signal: Social-Support Figures Enhance Rather Than Protect From Fear Extinction

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Ideals Aren t Always Typical: Dissociating Goodness-of-Exemplar From Typicality Judgments

When Sample Size Matters: The Influence of Sample Size and Category Variability on Children s and Adults Inductive Reasoning

THANK YOU! Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) Fear Potentiated Startle (FPS) 1/30/2015

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

Evidence for recovery of fear following immediate extinction in rats and humans

Rule-based processes in generalisation and peak shift in human fear conditioning

Effect of Conditioned Stimulus Generalization on Regression of Fear Memory Fading

Emotional arousal enhances lexical decision times

Stress promotes generalization of older but not recent threat memories

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Learned Fear of Unseen Faces After Pavlovian, Observational, and Instructed Fear Andreas Olsson and Elizabeth A. Phelps

Features of Similarity and Category-Based Induction

The spread of fear: Symbolic generalization mediates graded threat-avoidance in specific phobia

Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience fmri results in context. Doug Schultz

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

The two faces of typicality in category-based induction *

Class 16 Emotions (10/19/17) Chapter 10

Bayesian models of inductive generalization

Activation of the left amygdala to a cognitive representation of fear

Afterword: How are emotions, mood and temperament related?

However, as Sloman (1993) noted, there are other instances of nonmonotonicity that are not explainable by dilution of category coverage. His participa

Neurobehavioral Mechanisms Supporting the Generalization of Learned Fear in Humans. Joseph E. Dunsmoor

The spread of fear: Symbolic generalization mediates graded threat-avoidance in specific phobia

Abnormalities in the Anxiety Disorders. A Thesis SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY. Brian Gregory van Meurs

Running Head: SENSITIVITY TO TAXNOMIC ASSOCIATIONS

Generalization of Conscious Fear Is Positively Correlated with Anxiety, but Not with Depression

Learning Outcome: To what extent do cognitive and biological factors interact in emotion?

Lateral Inhibition Explains Savings in Conditioning and Extinction

Emotion. Fear Generalization Gradients in Visuospatial Attention

The official electronic file of this thesis or dissertation is maintained by the University Libraries on behalf of The Graduate School at Stony Brook

REFERENCES. Hemispheric Processing Asymmetries: Implications for Memory

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Erasing fear from memory Soeter, M. Link to publication

Does Imminent Threat Capture and Hold Attention?

Abundant research has been devoted to understanding anxiety

Interaction of Taxonomic and Contextual Knowledge in Categorization of Cross-Classified Instances

Feed-forward response of three physiological factors in men and women due to a startle stimulus

Intentional and Incidental Classification Learning in Category Use

Aversive picture processing: Effects of a concurrent task on sustained defensive system engagement

NeuroImage 55 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Chapter 7. Mental Representation

The use of an unpleasant sound unconditional stimulus in an aversive conditioning procedure with 8- to 11- year-old children

Affective Learning Increases Sensitivity to Graded Emotional Faces

DEVELOPMENT OF ANXIETY: THE ROLE OF THREAT APPRAISAL AND FEAR LEARNING

Affective Priming: Valence and Arousal

Modeling a reaction time variant of the Perruchet effect in humans

Neuroanatomy of Emotion, Fear, and Anxiety

Limbic system outline

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Bayesian models of inductive generalization

ANTICIPATORY SKIN CONDUCTANCE RESPONSES: A POSSIBLE EXAMPLE OF DECISION AUGMENTATION THEORY

Effects of lesions of the nucleus accumbens core and shell on response-specific Pavlovian i n s t ru mental transfer

Semantic Contamination of Visual Similarity Judgments

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

The Development of Fear Learning and Generalization in 8 13 Year-Olds

Cognitive Level of Analysis - Cognition and Emotions

CATS IN SHORTS. Easy reader of definitions and formal expressions. Holger Ursin March Uni Helse Universitetet i Bergen.

Behavioral Neuroscience: Fear thou not. Rony Paz

Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 13 Golcbia St., ?

UPDATED META-ANALYSIS OF CLASSICAL FEAR CONDITIONING IN THE ANXIETY DISORDERS

Topics in Animal Cognition. Oliver W. Layton

Role of the anterior cingulate cortex in the control over behaviour by Pavlovian conditioned stimuli

Physiological Responses in the Anticipation of an Auditory. and Visual Stimulus

Running Head: A Relevance Theory of Induction

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions

EBCC Data Analysis Tool (EBCC DAT) Introduction

Chapter 5 Study Guide

Fear conditioning: What does experience add? 1. Do CS-US pairings actually matter? A within-subject comparison of instructed fear

Slide 3.1. Learning and memory

Reviewers' comments: Reviewer #1 (Remarks to the Author):

The Intermixed Blocked Effect in Human Perceptual Learning Is Not the Consequence of Trial Spacing

The renewal of extinguished conditioned fear with fearrelevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli by a context change after extinction

Templates for Rejection: Configuring Attention to Ignore Task-Irrelevant Features

Delayed Extinction Attenuates Conditioned Fear Renewal and Spontaneous Recovery in Humans

Non-categorical approaches to property induction with uncertain categories

Chapter 6. Learning: The Behavioral Perspective

Computational Explorations in Cognitive Neuroscience Chapter 7: Large-Scale Brain Area Functional Organization

Nondeclarative memory. July 25, 2016

Concepts and Categories

The organism s problem: Avoid predation and injury so you can reproduce

Innate behavior & Learning

A Comparison of Three Measures of the Association Between a Feature and a Concept

Rajeev Raizada: Statement of research interests

Transcription:

535401PSSXXX10.1177/0956797614535401Dunsmoor, MurphyTypicality Determines Fear Generalization research-article2014 Research Report Stimulus Typicality Determines How Broadly Fear Is Generalized Psychological Science 2014, Vol. 25(9) 1816 1821 The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalspermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0956797614535401 pss.sagepub.com Joseph E. Dunsmoor and Gregory L. Murphy New York University Abstract The ability to represent knowledge at the category level promotes the transfer of learning. How this ability integrates with basic forms of conditioned learning is unknown but could explain why conditioned fear is overgeneralized after aversive experiences. We examined the impact of stimulus typicality an important determinant of category-based induction on fear learning and generalization. Typicality is known to affect the strength of categorical arguments; a premise involving typical exemplars (e.g., sparrow) is believed to apply to other members, whereas a premise about atypical exemplars (e.g., penguin) generalizes more narrowly to similar items. We adopted this framework to human fear conditioning and found that fear conditioned to typical exemplars generalized more readily to atypical members than vice versa, despite equal feature overlap across conditions. These findings have implications for understanding why some fearful events lead to broad overgeneralization of fear whereas others are regarded as isolated episodes. Keywords fear conditioning, category-based induction, generalization, skin conductance responses, reasoning Received 1/29/14; Revision accepted 4/20/14 Classical fear conditioning is a basic form of associative learning found throughout the animal kingdom. In it, a conditioned stimulus (CS; e.g., a tone) paired with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US; e.g., electrical shock) comes to elicit a defensive conditioned response (e.g., sweating). This process is essential to much behavioral control, from the fight-or-flight response to the avoidance of dangerous situations (Lang, Davis, & Öhman, 2000; LeDoux, 2000). It remains unclear, however, how this simple form of learning integrates with higher-order forms of reasoning in humans. Humans induce unknown properties of an object on the basis of the known properties of another object. Knowledge of whole categories promotes such generalizations. For example, a fear of doctors may be generalized to nurses, hospitals, or medical equipment. Research on category-based induction investigates how people generalize properties using conceptual knowledge (Murphy, 2002). One relevant phenomenon of induction is typicality asymmetry (Osherson, Smith, Wilkie, Lopez, & Shafir, 1990). Consider the following arguments: 1. Mice have sesamoid bones. Therefore bats have sesamoid bones. 2. Bats have sesamoid bones. Therefore mice have sesamoid bones. Although the arguments involve the same categories, Argument 1 is perceived as stronger than Argument 2, because typical items are better sources of generalization than atypical items are (Osherson et al., 1990; Rips, 1975). This asymmetry depends on the items conceptual status, classifying them as mammals and evaluating their typicality. Although conditioning is widely regarded as a simple form of learning governed by low-level systems, conceptual knowledge can be recruited when humans generalize fear. For example, Dunsmoor and his colleagues found that subjects who experienced shock paired with Corresponding Author: Joseph E. Dunsmoor, New York University Psychology, 6 Washington Pl., New York, NY 10003 E-mail: joseph.dunsmoor@nyu.edu

Typicality Determines Fear Generalization 1817 Table 1. Animal Exemplars Used in the Experiment Typicality Birds Mammals Typical Crow, hummingbird, sparrow Bear, horse, rabbit Atypical Emu, ibis, penguin Aardvark, armadillo, otter Intermediate (unpaired controls) Cockatiel, duck, dove, finch, macaw, owl Bison, camel, deer, gorilla, panda, ram some category members (e.g., various tools) generalized fear to other category members (Dunsmoor, Kragel, Martin, & LaBar, 2013; Dunsmoor, Martin, & LaBar, 2012). These results suggest that fear was associated to the concept tool as a whole rather than just the paired stimuli. However, an alternative explanation relies on feature overlap, as category exemplars tend to share perceptual features. The concept may have played no role. To discover whether fear learning involves conceptual knowledge, we created a fear conditioning task that paralleled tests of typicality asymmetry in category-based induction. In a between-groups manipulation, subjects in two experimental groups underwent fear conditioning to pictures of either typical or atypical category members and then were tested for generalization of conditioned responses to pictures of either atypical or typical category members, respectively. For example, they might be conditioned to pictures of a crow, a hummingbird, and a sparrow (typical birds) and tested on pictures of an emu, an ibis, and a penguin (atypical birds) or vice versa. Because the feature overlap of the learning and test items was identical in the two groups, any difference in generalization of conditioned responding would be due to conceptual properties rather than to overlap of perceptual features. Given the typicality asymmetry observed in studies of category-based induction, we hypothesized that fear conditioned to typical exemplars would generalize to atypical exemplars more readily than vice versa. Method Forty-six healthy volunteers provided written informed consent in accordance with the New York University Committee on Activities Involving Human Subjects and were compensated for their participation. Sample size was determined on the basis of a similar study in which category-based fear conditioning was measured by skin conductance response (SCR; Dunsmoor et al., 2012). Nine subjects were excluded because of an overall lack of measurable SCR, technical problems, or failure to follow instructions. The final sample included 37 subjects (26 women, 11 men; mean age = 23.4 years, SD = 4.8). Subjects were either conditioned to fear typical category members and then tested on atypical category members (typical- group: n = 19) or were conditioned to fear atypical category members and then tested on typical category members (atypical- group: n = 18). Subjects were assigned randomly to conditions. All stimuli were black-and-white pictures of animals on a white background. They had been rated for typicality of category members by 22 raters on Amazon s Mechanical Turk, who used a scale from 1 (highly atypical) to 7 (highly typical). Mean ratings were 2.67 (SD = 0.68) for the atypical category members, 4.76 (SD = 0.41) for the intermediate category members, and 6.09 (SD = 0.37) for the typical category members. Each image appeared for 6 s; each presentation of an image was followed by a 10-s fixation cross. During the conditioning phase, one animal category (e.g., birds), the, was paired with a mildly aversive electrical shock to each subject s right wrist (US). The items included three highly typical or highly atypical basic level exemplars presented three times each (nine total trials), and each exemplar was paired with shock on two of three trials (66% reinforcement rate). An equal number of pictures from the other animal category (e.g., mammals), the CS, were presented during fear conditioning and never paired with shock, which provided a within-subjects control. Unpaired category exemplars (CS s) were of intermediate typicality. Table 1 lists the birds and mammals whose images were used as stimuli. Stimulus order and animal categories serving as and CS were counterbalanced across subjects. In a test of fear generalization immediately following conditioning, subjects viewed three novel exemplars from the category (e.g., new birds, referred to as, presented three times each) and three novel exemplars from the CS category (e.g., new mammals, referred to as GEN, presented three times each). No shocks were delivered during the generalization test; however, subjects were not informed that shocks would not occur and continued to make shock-expectancy ratings. For subjects conditioned to typical exemplars, were atypical exemplars, whereas for subjects conditioned to atypical exemplars, were typical exemplars. The GEN exemplars were of intermediate typicality. On each trial, subjects rated expectancy for a shock using a three-alternative forced-choice scale (1 = no risk, 2 = moderate risk, or 3 = high risk); this procedure was based on that described by Lissek et al. (2008). These ratings were used primarily to determine that subjects

1818 Dunsmoor, Murphy Mean SCR Difference Scores (µs) 0.3 0.2 0.1 0.0 Typical- Group Atypical- Group ** ** Conditioning understood the CS-US contingencies, given that the exemplars varied throughout the conditioning phase, and CS-US pairing was intermittent. Shock ratings during conditioning were lost from 2 subjects because of technical problems with the presentation software in which button responses were not recorded; an additional subject failed to respond. Shock was delivered using a stimulator from Grass Technologies (Warwick, RI), and the shock was calibrated separately for each subject to reach a level deemed highly annoying but not painful. SCR electrodes were placed on the hypothenar eminence of the palmar surface of the left hand and collected with a sampling rate of 200 Hz using a BIOPAC system (Goleta, CA). SCRs were scored according to criteria described previously (Dunsmoor et al., 2012; Green, Kragel, Fecteau, & LaBar, 2014) and square-root transformed before statistical analysis to normalize the distribution. The conditioned responses for the fear conditioning phase were calculated per subject as a difference score ( minus ) using the mean SCRs to 2 through 9 of the and stimuli (Fig. 1). The first and trials were excluded because learning had not yet occurred. The generalized conditioned response for the generalization phase was calculated per subject as the difference score minus using the mean SCRs to 1 through 9 of the and stimuli. * Generalization Fig. 1. Mean skin conductance response (SCR) difference scores for each experimental group and phase. For subjects in the typical- group, the conditioned stimuli paired with shock (s) were images of typical animals (birds or mammals, counterbalanced), and the subjects were tested on new atypical animals from the same animal category () in the generalization test. For subjects in the atypical- group, the s were images of atypical animals, and the stimuli were typical animals from the same category. See the Method section for an explanation of how the difference scores were calculated. Error bars represent 1 SE. Asterisks indicate scores significantly different from zero (*p <.05, **p <.01). Results Both groups exhibited successful conditioning as revealed by greater SCRs to the than to the : typical- group t(18) = 6.63, p <.001; atypical- group t(17) = 4.12, p =.001. There were no differences between groups in mean SCRs to the (p =.26) or CS (p =.21). In short, learning was equal in the two groups. At the generalization test, the group trained on typical items generalized conditioned fear to atypical exemplars (i.e., vs. GEN ), t(18) = 2.79, p =.014, but the group trained on atypical items showed no difference in response between and GEN items, p =.34. Mean SCRs to items were also greater in the typical- group than in the atypical- group, t(17) = 2.96, p =.009; there were no group differences in mean SCRs to the GEN items, p =.17. Figure 1 illustrates the mean SCR difference scores for each phase. To examine the course of learning and generalization (see Fig. 2), we conducted analyses of variance using stimulus type and trial (1 9) as within-subjects factors and group (typical, atypical ) as a betweensubjects factor. During fear conditioning, SCRs showed a Stimulus Type (, CS ) Trial interaction, F(8, 280) = 2.56, p =.01, reflecting learning over trials, but no Stimulus Type Group interaction, p =.85, and no Stimulus Type Trial Group interaction, p =.30. Shock-expectancy ratings during fear conditioning were similar across groups, as indicated by the fact that there was a Stimulus Type Trial interaction, F(8, 216) = 5.89, p <.001, but no Stimulus Type Group interaction, p =.13, or Stimulus Type Trial Group interaction, p =.79. These data demonstrate that learning, as measured by SCR and shock expectancy, was similar in the two groups. Analysis of SCRs during the generalization test showed a Stimulus Type (, GEN ) Group interaction, F(1, 35) = 8.42, p =.006, but no Stimulus Type Trial interaction, p =.98, or Stimulus Type Trial Group interaction, p =.48. These results are in line with the effects shown in Figure 1, and group differences did not vary across trials. Shock-expectancy ratings during the generalization test were similar across groups; the Stimulus Type Trial interaction was significant, F(8, 256) = 4.05, p <.001, but the Stimulus Type Trial Group interaction was not, p =.13, and the Stimulus Type Group interaction only approached significance, p =.053. Discussion We have provided evidence that stimulus typicality influences generalization of conditioned fear in humans; fear was generalized from typical to atypical exemplars but was not generalized in the reverse direction, despite the

Typicality Determines Fear Generalization 1819 1.2 1.2 Mean Square Root SCRs (µs) Shock Expectancy Rating a Typical- Group Atypical- Group 0.8 0.4 0.8 0.4 0.0 0.0 b 3 Typical- Group 3 Atypical- Group 2 2 1 1 Fig. 2. Mean skin conductance response (SCR; a) and shock-expectancy rating (b) as a function of trial block, stimulus type, and experimental phase. Data are plotted separately for each group. Note that no shocks were delivered during the generalization test. Error bars represent ±1 SE. = animal category paired with shock; = animal category never paired with shock; = novel exemplars from the category; = novel exemplars from the category. fact that feature overlap did not differ between these two conditions. In the literature on category-based induction, this asymmetry is explained by the idea that typical items generate stronger expectations that the property holds for the entire category (Osherson et al., 1990). A similar mechanism could explain the fear generalization we observed shock was more strongly associated to the concept (rather than to the specific learning exemplars) when typical items were used for conditioning than when atypical items were used. Because atypical items, such as penguins, were accepted as category members, conditioning to typical exemplars extended to atypical category members at test as a result of this stronger association. Although people can learn to associate atypical items with shock, this learning appears to be confined to the fear-conditioned exemplars, and fear expression does not as readily generalize from atypical items to other category members. The primary question in this study concerned possible asymmetries in an autonomic measure of conditioned-fear

1820 Dunsmoor, Murphy expression (i.e., SCR). But the findings have implications for the potential contribution of higher-order cognitive processes to the generalization of conditioned fear. In this case, learning that typical exemplars predict shock may activate the internal structure of conceptual representations more than learning the same information about atypical CSs does. If a CS is a good representation of its category, pairing it with a shock may generate the proposition that other category members will be a potential threat as well (at least within the context of the fear-conditioning experiment). This explanation is in line with suggestions that associative learning in humans involves cognitive processes such as propositional and declarative knowledge about the CS-US relationship (Mitchell, De Houwer, & Lovibond, 2009). The present study adds novel evidence that typicality, a relatively complex attribute, influences generalization of fear learning and opens the way for further research on how other phenomena of category-based induction (conclusion specificity, premise diversity, mixed premises, etc.; Osherson et al., 1990) integrate with humans acquisition and expression of fear. The present experiment does not constitute a robust test of high-level reasoning processes in the course of learning and generalization. We provided subjects with a crude, three-alternative forced-choice scale for rating the perceived risk of shock on each trial and found no statistical evidence that these ratings were affected by stimulus typicality. Future research will be needed to investigate possible differences in fear generalization between autonomic and declarative measures. These results also highlight different routes of stimulus generalization in humans that go beyond mere physical similarity. Fear-conditioning research almost universally employs simple sensory cues such as lights and tones. Many real-world fears are complex, however, and feared stimuli can often be represented by a network of interrelated concepts and information and can assume different forms from one encounter to the next. It remains a challenge to predict what information might acquire emotional significance and later trigger fear and anxiety in real-world situations. For example, combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder may show a categorical fear of certain objects reminiscent of the region in which they were deployed. Emerging research is beginning to reveal behavioral and neural mechanisms associated with fear generalization in humans (Dymond et al., 2011; Lissek et al., 2013; Vervliet, Kindt, Vansteenwegen, & Hermans, 2010). The present research suggests that how a person conceptualizes the CS may have a significant effect on how he or she generalizes fear to new stimuli. It therefore provides avenues for further study on how generalized fear learning is governed by semantic categories and their structure. Author Contributions J. E. Dunsmoor performed the testing and collected and analyzed the data. Both authors developed the study concept and design, interpreted the data, drafted the manuscript, and approved the final version of the manuscript for submission. Acknowledgments We thank Elizabeth Phelps for helpful comments. Declaration of Conflicting Interests The authors declared that they had no conflicts of interest with respect to their authorship or the publication of this article. Funding This research was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Training Award in Systems and Integrative Neuroscience Grant T32-MH019524 (to J. E. Dunsmoor) and by National Science Foundation Grant BCS 1128769 (to G. L. Murphy). References Dunsmoor, J. E., Kragel, P. A., Martin, A., & LaBar, K. S. (2013). Aversive learning modulates cortical representations of object categories. Cerebral Cortex. Advance online publication. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht138 Dunsmoor, J. E., Martin, A., & LaBar, K. S. (2012). Role of conceptual knowledge in learning and retention of conditioned fear. Biological Psychology, 89, 300 305. Dymond, S., Schlund, M. W., Roche, B., Whelan, R., Richards, J., & Davies, C. (2011). Inferred threat and safety: Symbolic generalization of human avoidance learning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 6121. Green, S. R., Kragel, P. A., Fecteau, M. E., & LaBar, K. S. (2014). Development and validation of an unsupervised scoring system (Autonomate) for skin conductance response analysis. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 91, 186 193. Lang, P. J., Davis, M., & Öhman, A. (2000). Fear and anxiety: Animal models and human cognitive psychophysiology. Journal of Affective Disorders, 61, 137 159. LeDoux, J. E. (2000). Emotion circuits in the brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 23, 155 184. Lissek, S., Biggs, A. L., Rabin, S. J., Cornwell, B. R., Alvarez, R. P., Pine, D. S., & Grillon, C. (2008). Generalization of conditioned fear-potentiated startle in humans: Experimental validation and clinical relevance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 678 687. Lissek, S., Bradford, D. E., Alvarez, R. P., Burton, P., Espensen- Sturges, T., Reynolds, R. C., & Grillon, C. (2013). Neural substrates of classically conditioned fear-generalization in humans: A parametric fmri study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. Advance online publication. doi:10.1093/scan/nst096 Mitchell, C. J., De Houwer, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning [Target

Typicality Determines Fear Generalization 1821 article and commentaries]. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 32, 183 246. Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Osherson, D. N., Smith, E. E., Wilkie, O., Lopez, A., & Shafir, E. (1990). Category-based induction. Psychological Review, 97, 185 200. Rips, L. J. (1975). Inductive judgments about natural categories. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 665 681. Vervliet, B., Kindt, M., Vansteenwegen, D., & Hermans, D. (2010). Fear generalization in humans: Impact of prior nonfearful experiences. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48, 1078 1084.