Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Similar documents
Evidence for a Refractory Period in the Hemodynamic Response to Visual Stimuli as Measured by MRI

Functional Anatomic Study of Episodic Retrieval

Stimulus Repetition and Hemodynamic Response Refractoriness in Event-Related fmri

Introduction to Functional MRI

Temporal Sensitivity of Event-Related fmri

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Event-Related fmri of Tasks Involving Brief Motion

PHYSICS OF MRI ACQUISITION. Alternatives to BOLD for fmri

International Journal of Innovative Research in Advanced Engineering (IJIRAE) Volume 1 Issue 10 (November 2014)

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri

Does stimulus quality affect the physiologic MRI responses to brief visual activation?

Left Anterior Prefrontal Activation Increases with Demands to Recall Specific Perceptual Information

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

Experimental Design!

Brain Areas Involved in Rapid Categorization of Natural Images: An Event-Related fmri Study

Hemodynamics and fmri Signals

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Neural Netw. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 November 1.

Design Optimization. SBIRC/SINAPSE University of Edinburgh

The BOLD fmri refractory effect is specific to stimulus attributes: evidence from a visual motion paradigm

Design Optimization. Optimization?

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Experimental design. Experimental design. Experimental design. Guido van Wingen Department of Psychiatry Academic Medical Center

Neural Modeling and Functional Brain Imaging: An Overview. Barry Horwitz Brain Imaging & Modeling Section NIDCD, NIH

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

The Tools: Imaging the Living Brain

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications

Experimental Design. Outline. Outline. A very simple experiment. Activation for movement versus rest

Deconvolution of Impulse Response in Event-Related BOLD fmri 1

fmri: What Does It Measure?

CS/NEUR125 Brains, Minds, and Machines. Due: Friday, April 14

Neuroimaging. BIE601 Advanced Biological Engineering Dr. Boonserm Kaewkamnerdpong Biological Engineering Program, KMUTT. Human Brain Mapping

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

The physiology of the BOLD signal What do we measure with fmri?

Brain and Cognition. Cognitive Neuroscience. If the brain were simple enough to understand, we would be too stupid to understand it

Hemodynamic responses in visual, motor, and somatosensory cortices in schizophrenia

Experimental Design I

ARTICLE IN PRESS. St. Louis, MO 63130, USA d Department of Radiology, Washington University, St. Louis, MO 63130, USA

Functional-Anatomic Correlates of Object Priming in Humans Revealed by Rapid Presentation Event-Related fmri

Contribution of Working Memory to Transient Activation in Human Inferior Prefrontal Cortex during Performance of the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Hemodynamics and fmri Signals

Discriminative Analysis for Image-Based Population Comparisons

Nonlinear Temporal Dynamics of the Cerebral Blood Flow Response

Edinburgh Imaging Academy online distance learning courses. Functional Imaging

Experimental design for Cognitive fmri

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

Memory Processes in Perceptual Decision Making

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

The Contribution of TMS-EEG Coregistration in the Exploration of the Human Connectome

The Central Nervous System

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

BOLD signal compartmentalization based on the apparent diffusion coefficient

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Prediction of Successful Memory Encoding from fmri Data

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Functional organization of spatial and nonspatial working memory processing within the human lateral frontal cortex

A model of dual control mechanisms through anterior cingulate and prefrontal cortex interactions

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

Functional MRI evidence for a role of frontal and inferior temporal cortex in amodal components of priming

MEDICAL REVIEW Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: From Acquisition to Application

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Causality from fmri?

Table 1. Summary of PET and fmri Methods. What is imaged PET fmri BOLD (T2*) Regional brain activation. Blood flow ( 15 O) Arterial spin tagging (AST)

Define functional MRI. Briefly describe fmri image acquisition. Discuss relative functional neuroanatomy. Review clinical applications.

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

Neuroimaging methods vs. lesion studies FOCUSING ON LANGUAGE

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Titelmaster The physics of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri)

Integration of diverse information in working memory within the frontal lobe

An introduction to functional MRI

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

A Neurally-Inspired Model for Detecting and Localizing Simple Motion Patterns in Image Sequences

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Visual Context Dan O Shea Prof. Fei Fei Li, COS 598B

Imaging neuroscience: Principles or maps?

MSc Neuroimaging for Clinical & Cognitive Neuroscience

P2 Visual - Perception

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

5th Mini-Symposium on Cognition, Decision-making and Social Function: In Memory of Kang Cheng

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Chantal E. Stern,*, Adrian M. Owen, Irene Tracey,*, Rodney B. Look,* Bruce R. Rosen,* and Michael Petrides

Outline. Biological Psychology: Research Methods. Dr. Katherine Mickley Steinmetz

fmri Study of Face Perception and Memory Using Random Stimulus Sequences

Classifying instantaneous cognitive states from fmri data

Advanced Data Modelling & Inference

Retrieval Success is Accompanied by Enhanced Activation in Anterior Prefrontal Cortex During Recognition Memory: An Event-Related fmristudy

Classification and Statistical Analysis of Auditory FMRI Data Using Linear Discriminative Analysis and Quadratic Discriminative Analysis

Functional magnetic resonance imaging: basic principles of and

Computational Cognitive Neuroscience of the Visual System Stephen A. Engel

Interpretation of clinical functional neuroimaging studies

Attentional Blink Paradigm

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

Linear Systems Analysis of Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging in Human V1

Transcription:

Human Brain Mapping 6:373 377(1998) Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response Randy L. Buckner 1,2,3 * 1 Departments of Psychology, Anatomy and Neurobiology, and Radiology, Washington University, St. Louis, Missouri 2 Massachusetts General Hospital Nuclear Magnetic Resonance Center, Boston, Massachusettes 3 Department of Radiology, Harvard Medical School, Cambridge, Massachusettes Abstract: Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging (ER-fMRI) methods are allowing a new spectrum of task designs to be explored with brain imaging techniques. Individual trial events can be presented rapidly, in randomly intermixed order, and the hemodynamic responses associated with individual trial events appreciated. The basis of ER-fMRI is that the hemodynamic response tracks neuronal activity on the order of seconds and, in many situations, summates over trials in a manner well predicted by a linear model even for trials spaced as briefly as 2 sec apart. These properties are discussed, as well as certain basic characteristics of the hemodynamic response in the context of ER-fMRI. Hum. Brain Mapping 6:373 377, 1998. 1998 Wiley-Liss, Inc. Key words: single trial; neuroimaging; MRI; fmri; imaging INTRODUCTION The spectrum of task designs that can be examined with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri) has been broadly expanded by the development of event-related (ER-fMRI) procedures. ER-fMRI allows paradigms to depart from blocked testing procedures in which long periods of task performance are integrated to paradigms that isolate individual trial events or subcomponents of trial events (Fig. 1A). ER-fMRI is based on the observation that changes in hemodynamics are rapid and occur within seconds after a neuronal event. Blamire et al. [1992], for example, noted that brief visual stimuli (2-sec) produce detectable hemodynamic responses when measured *Correspondence to: Dr. Randy L. Buckner, Department of Psychology, Box 1125, Washington University, One Brookings Drive, St. Louis, MO 63130. Received for publication 1 March 1998; accepted 6 July 1998 using blood oxygenation level-dependent (BOLD) contrast fmri. Savoy and colleagues have shown that visual stimulation as brief as 34 msec in duration is sufficient to elicit a detectable BOLD hemodynamic response [see Rosen et al., 1998]. In the realm of cognitive task paradigms, Buckner et al. [1996] observed that subtle signal changes in visual and prefrontal brain areas can be detected during isolated trials of a word generation task. Taken collectively, these and other observations make clear that fmri is sensitive to transient signal change to brief neuronal events [see Rosen et al., 1998, for review]. The observation that fmri is sensitive to transient signal change is important for several reasons. First, behavioral and evoked response potential (ERP) studies typically use paradigms where individual trial events are the focus. fmri, by detecting signals to individual trial events, can parallel behavioral studies by examining responses to individual trial events rather than blocks of trials. Second, classification of events often cannot be determined apriori such as 1998 Wiley-Liss, Inc.

Buckner Figure 1. 374

Event-Related fmri when the subject s response to the event is a factor. Being able to isolate fmri signals allows the BOLD response to be sorted by subject performance (e.g., by whether an event is performed correctly or not, or based on how long an event takes to complete). Finally, there are certain kinds of events that are not stable and are therefore best addressed by dynamic measurement. Paradigms exploring novelty effects and learning paradigms provide prime examples. Thus, the benefit of ER-fMRI is that the flexibility afforded to paradigm design allows new classes of behavioral paradigms to be tested. The manner in which we can perform such tests is constrained by the nature of the hemodynamic response. The hemodynamic response In order to map brain activity based on the transient fmri signal, it is important to understand the basic nature of the BOLD-contrast hemodynamic response. For a brief sensory event, the hemodynamic response is delayed in onset occurring about 2 sec after neuronal activity, and is prolonged in duration [Blamire et al., 1992] (see Fig. 1B). For a neural event that lasts a second, the robust positive deflection of the BOLDcontrast response will evolve over a 10 12-sec period while certain, more subtle, components of the response will have considerably longer recovery periods. On first appearance, this would suggest that hemodynamic events to be determined must be well separated in time. Fortunately, this is not the case; ER-fMRI can measure the separate contributions of rapidly occurring neuronal events even when the hemodynamic responses they elicit overlap. The reason for this is that, on first approximation, the hemodynamic response summates in a roughly linear fashion over time [Boynton et al., 1996; Dale and Buckner, 1997]; the hemodynamic response of one neural event adds on top of the preceding events. Figure 1B shows linear summation for multiple presentations of a 1-sec visual stimulus. Reliable departures from linear summation can be observed but may be subtle enough, in certain situations, to be considered approximately linear. Situations in which stimulus events occur extremely rapidly (e.g., 1 sec apart), can show marked departures from linearity [e.g., Friston et al., 1997]. Although the sources of such departures are incompletely understood, a tentative explanation may be that the basic coupling between net neuronal activity and hemodynamic response is roughly linear, but the relation between neuronal response and stimulus/task parameters is often nonlinear [Rosen et al., 1998]. The upshot is that separate sensory and higherlevel cognitive events can be elicited in rapid succession (1 event per 2 sec), and their response contributions separated and analyzed [Dale and Buckner, 1997; Buckner et al., 1998a; Clark et al., 1998] (Fig. 1C). Variance A central issue in analyzing ER-fMRI data is variance of the hemodynamic response. As would be expected in any real-world system, variation is present; the relevant issues are: 1) the magnitude and A: Schematic diagrams illustrate the difference between two forms of ER-fMRI paradigms and a more traditional blocked trial paradigm. Each schematic shows two trial types indicated by either yellow or red arrows. In blocked trial paradigms (labeled blocked), the trial types are clustered together in succession so that the same trial type or condition occurs for an extended period of time. ER-fMRI, by contrast, allows paradigms that randomly intermix different trial types either by spacing them widely apart to allow the hemodynamic response from one trial to decay before the next trial occurs (labeled spaced mixed trial), or by presenting them rapidly (labeled rapid mixed trial) and using analysis methods to account for the overlap of the hemodynamic response across trials. B: Data are displayed that support the critical feature (linear summation), which allows the BOLD response to be estimated by signal deconvolution even in situations where trial responses overlap considerably [Dale and Buckner, 1997]. The hemodynamic response is plotted for 3 separate conditions that either contained a single trial (labeled one-trial), 2 separate trials each spaced 2 sec Figure 1. apart (labeled two-trial), or three separate trials each spaced 2 sec apart (labeled three-trial). Each trial consisted of a 1-sec full-field flickering checkerboard. The relative positions of the three trials are shown by shaded rectangles on the x-axis. Linear summation over trials is apparent as each additional trial adds on top of the preceding trials. The basic shape of the hemodynamic response can be appreciated by examining the one-trial condition; it does not start until about 2 sec after the stimulus onset, and lasts for about 10 12 sec. C: Rapid ER-fMRI procedures were applied to the examination of repetition priming [adapted from Buckner et al., 1998a]. Novel and repeated objects were randomly intermixed, one object per 2 sec. ER-fMRI procedures were used to extract the time-course of activation for multiple regions throughout the brain (labeled A, B, C, D). Differential signal change was observed between novel and repeated objects for certain extrastriate visual (B) and left prefrontal regions (C); while other regions, such as early visual cortex (A) and motor cortex (D), showed similar responses to novel and repeated objects. 375

Buckner practical implications of the variance, and 2) what the sources of variance can tell us about and/or do to limit our exploration of brain function. While only preliminary analyses have been conducted, it appears that the hemodynamic response is reasonably stable across subjects. In one analysis, the hemodynamic responses in two cortical regions (SMA and extrastriate visual cortex) were examined across 13 subjects during a memory recognition task [Buckner et al., 1998b]. Each response was derived from many observations within a subject so that stable within-subject estimates of the responses could be obtained. The question then asked was: if the timing and shape of one subject s hemodynamic response was known, how well could it predict the other subjects hemodynamic responses? The answer was clear: the basic shape and timing of the hemodynamic was stable across subjects. 72% of the variance of the shape of one subject s response could be predicted on average by any other subject. Moreover, the absolute range of the timing of the response was a few seconds, indicating that the standard error estimate of the mean response time was fractions of a second for the group of 13 subjects. As a further empirical demonstration, the response of one group of 6 subjects was compared to a second independent group of 7 subjects. They nearly overlapped. Despite the stability of the response for similar regions of cortex across subjects, marked variation in the timing and shape of responses has been observed across regions, even within the same subjects. Schacter et al. [1997] and Buckner et al. [1998b] have noted delays across separate prefrontal regions on the order of seconds. The source of such variance in hemodynamic response timing is presently unclear, but may originate from differential sampling of vessels across regions [Lee et al., 1995], or from delays in underlying neuronal activity [see Rosen et al., 1998, for discussion]. Further investigation will be necessary to distinguish among these and other possibilities. One practical implication of variance in the hemodynamic response across regions is that present methods are unlikely to inform us about the cascades of neural activity that occur between regions on the timescale of msec. Bandettini, O Craven, and Savoy performed a procedure for normalization of hemodynamic delay across regions and showed that a 500 msec offset in stimulus-induced neuronal activity could be appreciated [see Rosen et al., 1998]. It seems likely that significantly better temporal resolution, on the order of tens of msec, will require integration with techniques that are sensitive to signals timelocked to neural activity on the order of msec such as EEG, MEG, and optical imaging. A further implication of hemodynamic variance across brain regions is that statistical methods used to identify areas of signal change will need to allow for variance in the timing and/or shape of the hemodynamic response to be sensitive to all forms of signal change. Several currently available statistical methods possess this feature [e.g., Friston et al., 1997; Schacter et al., 1997]. ER-fMRI may further benefit from analysis procedures that make no or minimal assumptions about the shape and timing of the hemodynamic response. Future Directions ER-fMRI procedures promise to expand the array of paradigms that can be explored with functional neuroimaging techniques. As with any methodological advancement, the utility of ER-fMRI will be evaluated by the new scientific questions that are asked and informed by their application. A number of studies already presented in the literature foreshadow the impact these research techniques may eventually have. Courtney et al. [1997], for example, used a variant of ER-fMRI to separate subcomponents of trial events in relation to working memory. They noted that a distributed network of brain areas were active during a working memory task; certain areas differentially contributed to processes related to perception and others to maintenance of information over time. Posterior visual areas showed mostly transient stimulus-locked activity which links them most directly with perceiving the stimulus; while anterior areas, particularly regions in prefrontal cortex, demonstrated sustained activity as would be expected of regions contributing to the maintenance of information over time. In standard blocked trial procedures, these processes would be blurred together and the differential contributions across regions difficult to appreciate. McCarthy et al. [1997] examined the classical odd-ball paradigm by observing stimulus-locked activity to infrequent target events, again a procedure best addressed by ER-fMRI techniques. In their study, target events elicited transient increases in prefrontal and parietal cortex activity. Going forward, these kinds of applications are likely to become routine in functional neuroimaging studies. A number of central issues are still in debate. Statistical analyses for mapping event-related hemodynamic responses are just beginning to evolve. Methods that make few or no assumptions about the shape of the hemodynamic response as well as those that consider the possibility of nonlinear summation of the hemodynamic response are on the horizon. In addition, we have yet to come to fully understand the relative signal-to-noise tradeoffs between blocked trial 376

Event-Related fmri and event-related paradigms, and for event-related paradigms that space or position trials in fundamentally different ways. Empirically, we know that randomly intermixed trial events spaced just a few seconds apart can be used to generate activity maps and time-course information for the hemodynamic responses associated with the intermixed event types [Dale and Buckner, 1997; Buckner et al., 1998a; Clark et al., 1998]. Such demonstrations make clear the potential of ER-fMRI methods. REFERENCES Blamire AM, Ogawa S, Ugurbil K, Rothman D, McCarthy G, Ellerman JM, Hyder F, Rattner Z, Shulman RG (1992): Dynamic mapping of the human visual cortex by high-speed magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 89:11069 11073. Boynton GM, Engel SA, Glover GH, Heeger DJ (1996): Linear systems analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging in human V1. J Neurosci 16:4207 4221. Buckner RL, Bandettini PA, O Craven KM, Savoy RL, Petersen SE, Raichle ME, Rosen BR (1996): Detection of cortical activation during averaged single trials of a cognitive task using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 93:14878 14883. Buckner RL, Goodman J, Burock M, Rotte M, Koutstaal M, Schacter DL, Rosen B, Dale AM (1998a): Functional-anatomic correlates of object priming in humans revealed by rapid presentation eventrelated fmri. Neuron 20:285 296. Buckner RL, Koutstaal W, Schacter DL, Dale AM, Rotte MR, Rosen BR (1998b): Functional-anatomic study of episodic retrieval: II. Selective averaging of event-related fmri trials to test the retrieval success hypothesis. NeuroImage 7:163 175. Clark VP, Maisog JM, Haxby JV (1998): An fmri study of face perception and memory using random stimulus sequences. J Neurophysiol 79:3257 3265. Courtney SM, Ungerleider LG, Keil K, Haxby JV (1997): Transient and sustained activity in a distributed neural system for human working memory. Nature 386:608 611. Dale AM, Buckner RL (1997): Selective averaging of rapidly presented individual trials using fmri. Hum Brain Mapping 5:329 340. Friston KJ, Josephs O, Rees G, Turner R (1997): Nonlinear eventrelated responses in fmri. Magn Reson Med 39:41 52. Lee AT, Glover GH, Meyer CH (1995): Discrimination of large venous vessels in time-course spiral blood-oxygen-level-dependent magnetic-resonance functional neuroimaging. Magn Reson Med 33:745 754. McCarthy G, Luby M, Gore J, Goldman-Rakic P (1997): Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI. J Neurophysiol 77:1630 1634. Rosen BR, Buckner RL, Dale AM (1998): Event related fmri: Past, present, and future. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 95:773 780. Schacter DL, Buckner RL, Koutstaal W, Dale AM, Rosen BR (1997): Late onset of anterior prefrontal activity during true and false recognition: An event-related fmri study. NeuroImage 6:259 269. 377