Contribution of the human superior parietal lobule to spatial selection process: an MEG study

Similar documents
Mirror neurons. Romana Umrianova

Attention Response Functions: Characterizing Brain Areas Using fmri Activation during Parametric Variations of Attentional Load

Cerebral Cortex 1. Sarah Heilbronner

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications

Medical Neuroscience Tutorial Notes

Supporting Information

Topic 11 - Parietal Association Cortex. 1. Sensory-to-motor transformations. 2. Activity in parietal association cortex and the effects of damage

Motor Systems I Cortex. Reading: BCP Chapter 14

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Event-Related fmri and the Hemodynamic Response

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

PARIETAL LOBE. Vasilios A. Zerris MD, MPH, MSc, FAANS

Neurophysiology of systems

Cognitive Modelling Themes in Neural Computation. Tom Hartley

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

Homework Week 2. PreLab 2 HW #2 Synapses (Page 1 in the HW Section)

The Central Nervous System

Mirror Neurons in Primates, Humans, and Implications for Neuropsychiatric Disorders

AUXILIARIES AND NEUROPLASTICITY

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Submitted report on Sufi recordings at AAPB 2013 in Portland. Not for general distribution. Thomas F. Collura, Ph.D. July, 2013

ASSUMPTION OF COGNITIVE UNIFORMITY

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

Supplemental material

Social Cognition and the Mirror Neuron System of the Brain


HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

Left-hemisphere dominance for processing of vowels: a whole-scalp neuromagnetic study

A Brain Computer Interface System For Auto Piloting Wheelchair

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study

Announcement. Danny to schedule a time if you are interested.

Analysis of brain activity immediately before conscious teeth clenching using magnetoencephalographic method

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2008

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Essentials of Human Anatomy & Physiology. Seventh Edition. The Nervous System. Copyright 2003 Pearson Education, Inc. publishing as Benjamin Cummings

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves

This presentation is the intellectual property of the author. Contact them for permission to reprint and/or distribute.

Est-ce que l'eeg a toujours sa place en 2019?

Beyond bumps: Spiking networks that store sets of functions

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Clinically Available Optical Topography System

Cerebrum-Cerebral Hemispheres. Cuneyt Mirzanli Istanbul Gelisim University

The Tools: Imaging the Living Brain

Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception

Water immersion modulates sensory and motor cortical excitability

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation

Competing Streams at the Cocktail Party

Control of visuo-spatial attention. Emiliano Macaluso

On the tracking of dynamic functional relations in monkey cerebral cortex

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

AFOSR PI Meeting Dec 1-3, 2014 Program Director: Dr. Darema Dynamic Integration of Motion and Neural Data to Capture Human Behavior

EEG Analysis on Brain.fm (Focus)

Spatiotemporal separability in the human cortical response to visual motion speed: a magnetoencephalography study

Over-representation of speech in older adults originates from early response in higher order auditory cortex

CISC 3250 Systems Neuroscience

Mental Representation of Number in Different Numerical Forms

Prof. Greg Francis 5/23/08

What s the difference between EEG and MEG in practice?

Rhythm and Rate: Perception and Physiology HST November Jennifer Melcher

Synchrony and the attentional state

Lecture X. Higher Functions. Principal References. Additional References. Methods to Study Brain Activity in Awake Humans. What this lecture is about:

NEUROSCIENCE. Barbora Cimrová

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

Chapter 3: 2 visual systems

Development of a New Rehabilitation System Based on a Brain-Computer Interface Using Near-Infrared Spectroscopy

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Three-dimensional tuning profile of motor cortical activity during arm. [Running Head] motor cortical activity during arm movements

FRONTAL LOBE. Central Sulcus. Ascending ramus of the Cingulate Sulcus. Cingulate Sulcus. Lateral Sulcus

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

CEREBRUM. Dr. Jamila EL Medany

Lecture XI. Integrating Functions. Additional References! Principal References! Methods to Study Brain Activity in Awake Humans!

Supplementary Figure 1. Example of an amygdala neuron whose activity reflects value during the visual stimulus interval. This cell responded more

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Increased Synchronization of Neuromagnetic Responses during Conscious Perception

Event-related brain activity associated with auditory pattern processing

25/09/2012. Capgras Syndrome. Chapter 2. Capgras Syndrome - 2. The Neural Basis of Cognition

ERP Components and the Application in Brain-Computer Interface

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch!

Location of the Sensorimotor Cortex: Functional and Conventional MR Compared

Processing of the incomplete representation of the visual world

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

Visual Evoked Potentials. Outline. Visual Pathway Anatomy

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Gross Organization I The Brain. Reading: BCP Chapter 7

Neural Coding. Computing and the Brain. How Is Information Coded in Networks of Spiking Neurons?

BESA Research Quick Guide

Myers Psychology for AP*

How strong is it? What is it? Where is it? What must sensory systems encode? 9/8/2010. Spatial Coding: Receptive Fields and Tactile Discrimination

Spatial Coding: Receptive Fields and Tactile Discrimination

Attention: Neural Mechanisms and Attentional Control Networks Attention 2

Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Transcription:

Brain Research 897 (2001) 164 168 www.elsevier.com/ locate/ bres Short communication Contribution of the human superior parietal lobule to spatial selection process: an MEG study * Tadahiko Shibata, Andreas A. Ioannides Laboratory for Human Brain Dynamics, RIKEN-Brain Science Institute, 2-1 Hirosawa, Wako-shi, Saitama, 351-0198, Japan Accepted 2 January 2001 Abstract The magnetoencephalographic signal was collected in a visually guided response-finger selection GO/ NOGO task. The minimum norm distributed source analysis identified the sources in bilateral superior parietal lobules (SPL), with stronger activity for contralateral finger movement. Our results suggest that the human SPL plays a role in the spatial selection in a visuomotor task similar to that identified recently in monkeys. 2001 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. Theme: Motor systems and sensorimotor integration Topic: Control of posture and movement Keywords: Human; Magnetoencephalography; GO/ NOGO paradigm; Current density analysis; Superior parietal lobule; Spatial selection Recently, the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) has been The subjects were six right-handed adults (five males, a proposed to play an important role in the visuomotor task female, aged 23 36). Ethical committee approval was [2,7,12,14]. Animal studies suggested that the PPC partici- obtained from the host institution and informed consent pates in motor planning with respect to both eye and hand from the subjects before the experiment. The subject was movements [12,14] and carry out the coordinate trans- seated in a magnetically shielded room (MSR) and asked formations necessary to convert sensory signals into motor to fixate a small cross on the screen placed 60 cm ahead. commands [2]. Moreover, the cells in the superior parietal Six types of visual stimuli were used using red and green lobule (SPL) showed directionally selective responses even arrows pointing to the left, right or to both directions (Fig. in cases where the movement is never actually executed, 1a). Each stimulus was back-projected for 32 ms onto the suggesting that potential movements are coded by SPL screen by a video-projector placed outside the MSR. In cells [7]. However, few studies reported PPC activities in each active and control run, 60 stimuli for each type were humans, so how the human PPC contributes to the presented in random order with inter-stimulus-interval visuomotor task is still unclear. ranging from 1.2 to 2.0 s. In the active task, the subject In the present work, we employed a response-finger was required to extend the index finger(s) indicated by selection GO/ NOGO task, which consists of sub-processes green arrow(s) immediately after the stimulus onset (GO in multiple areas [4,10,13]. We used magnetoence- condition), but to withhold movement for red arrow(s) phalography (MEG) and examined the PPC activity in the (NOGO condition). In the control task, the subject watched GO/ NOGO task, especially its role in the spatial selection the same stimuli without discrimination or any finger process (i.e. selection of the response finger). Here, we movement. focused on the brain activities related to the most promi- The MEG signal was collected with a 151-channels nent complex of the signal observed in the parietal area whole head system (OMEGA-151, CTF Systems Inc.) at a around 300 ms in the GO condition. rate of 1250 Hz with DC-300 Hz filter. The electrooculogram and the electromyography (EMG) of both arms were *Corresponding author. Tel.: 181-48-462-1111 (ext. 7182); fax: 181- recorded simultaneously. The response times (RT) of both 48-467-9731. fingers were measured by optical switches. The recorded E-mail address: shibata@brain.riken.go.jp (T. Shibata). MEG signal was converted to 3rd-order synthetic gradient 0006-8993/ 01/ $ see front matter 2001 Elsevier Science B.V. All rights reserved. PII: S0006-8993(01)02088-1

T. Shibata, A.A. Ioannides / Brain Research 897 (2001) 164 168 165 Fig. 1. The activation curves in the superior part of the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) extracted from the signal averaged with the EMG onset for four subjects. The left (right) graph shows the activation curves in three GO conditions at the red spot in the adjacent MRI; the red spot marks the local maximal activity within the left (right) superior PPC in the GO-right (-left) condition. The yellow lines mark the central sulcus. The number with mm in the MRI indicates the distance of the display plane from the AC PC line. For each subject, the same scale is used for left and right hemisphere. formation and was resampled off-line at 625 Hz with from subject s magnetic-resonance-image (MRI) was used additional 90 Hz low-pass filter. The signal was averaged for the forward calculation (i.e. one compartment model). for each condition separately with respect to the stimulus The computation used a 3-dimensional grid restricted onset (2400 to 600 ms) and the onset of the arm EMG (at inside of the brain with grid points every 10 mm. More least 2500 to 150 ms). The pre-stimulus period was used than 1100 grid points were used and MNLS method for a baseline. The trials contaminated with artifacts or computed the optimal orientation and moment at each grid wrong response were removed. point from the measured MEG signal. The activation curve The source reconstruction was performed using mini- (i.e. time course of strength) at each grid point was mum norm least squares (MNLS) method provided with computed and its mean values were compared between the CURRY4 (NeuroScan Labs.), which produces esti- conditions using the two-factor analysis of variance mates for cortical activation without a priori assumption (ANOVA) and a post-hoc test. The first factor was the about the location, number and spatial extent of the conditions and the second one the subjects. generators [3,6,8]. The inner shape of a skull extracted The mean (6S.D.) RT of six subjects was 425.2650.7

166 T. Shibata, A.A. Ioannides / Brain Research 897 (2001) 164 168 ms. The percentage of wrong responses was 12.6% in the GO conditions and 0.8% in the NOGO conditions. In all subjects, a prominent MEG signal was observed around 300 ms after the stimulus in the GO conditions, but not in the NOGO or control conditions (Fig. 1a). This signal was clearly the most prominent in terms of strength and duration. On the signal averaged with the EMG onset, the prominent peak was observed around the EMG onset (Fig. 1b). The signal of subject 6 was too noisy and was not used for further analysis. Since the prominent activity was observed only in the GO condition and was closer to the finger response, we report on the reconstructed source distributions obtained from the signal averaged with the EMG onset. The mean Fig. 2. Subject 1. (a) Superimposed MEG signal from all 151 channels for the six conditions in the GO/ NOGO task after averaging with the stimulus onset. The prominent peaks are observed around 300 ms in the GO conditions (underlined). (b) The corresponding waveforms for the three GO conditions after averaging with the EMG onset. (c) The local maxima (white area indicated by yellow arrow) of estimated current sources and its activation curves around the left central sulcus (upper, the plane displayed is 50 mm above the AC PC line) and in the superior part of the left posterior parietal cortex (PPC) (lower, 60 mm above the AC PC line) with the GO-right condition, computed from the MEG signal averaged with the EMG onset. Activity in the PPC appears before the EMG onset and it is followed by two peaks around the central sulcus. (d) As in (c) but for the right hemisphere from the GO-left condition. In (c) and (d), the white horizontal lines indicate the vertical anterior commissure (VAC) plane and the vertical posterior commissure (VPC) 2 plane, respectively. The level of the contour map is normalized for each map. The unit is uamm/mm. The latency of each map is indicated by an arrow on the corresponding activation curve.

T. Shibata, A.A. Ioannides / Brain Research 897 (2001) 164 168 167 unexplained variance of the fit over the range 2100 ms to the way potential movements are coded by cells in area 5 100 ms was 9.9666.07%. Local maxima within the [7]. Interpreting the response-finger selection in our study parietal lobe were observed bilaterally around the central as the spatial selection in the subject s body coordinate sulci (all subjects), the superior part of the PPC (four system [2], our result together with the previous monkey subjects) and around the parieto-occipital region (two studies suggested that the human SPL relates to the spatial subjects). Some local maxima were also observed in the selection process in visuomotor task. inferior part of the PPC with considerable inter-subject It should be noted however that in monkeys activity variability. We focused on the activation curves around the related to the spatial selection was also identified in the superior PPC and around the central sulcus which were the NOGO condition [7], but it was only observed in the GO most common local maxima across subjects. condition in our study. An explanation for this may be that The activation curves of the sources around central the presence of many options forced our subjects to sulcus showed two peaks and the one in the superior PPC distinguish the GO or NOGO first and afterwards select the showed a peak with contralateral finger response. Ac- response finger(s) in only the GO condition cases. The high tivities in both areas started before the EMG onset. The error percentage in GO condition comparing with NOGO superior PPC activity preceded the one around the central condition and relatively slow RT supported this interpretasulcus (Fig. 1c and d). While the second peak around the tion [5,13]. In summary, our findings provide evidence in central sulcus was clearly observed in all subjects with the humans for an SPL role which is intermediary between the mean latency of 116.5 ms (S.D. 13.9 ms), the first peak visual input processing and motor output within a spatial was not apparent with subjects 2, 4 and 5. Fig. 2 compares selection task as already reported for monkeys [7]. the activation curves of the superior PPC among three GO conditions. In the left superior PPC, the ANOVA (3 conditions34 subjects) showed significant difference References among the mean values (from 0 to 50 ms) of the three conditions (P,0.05). The mean values of the GO-right and [1] R.A. Andersen, Visual and eye movement functions of the posterior the GO-both conditions were significantly larger than that parietal cortex, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 12 (1989) 377 403. of the GO-left condition (P,0.05). On the other hand, the [2] R.A. Andersen, L.H. Snyder, D.C. Bradley, J. Xing, Multimodal activation in the right superior PPC showed larger activity representation of space in the posterior parietal cortex and its use in planning movements, Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 20 (1997) 303 330. with the GO-left and GO-both conditions than the GO- [3] T. Ball, A. Schreiber, B. Feige, M. Wagner, C.H. Lucking, R. right condition (P,0.05). Kristeva-Feige, The role of higher-order motor areas in voluntary The activities around the central sulci relate to the motor movement as revealed by high-resolution EEG and fmri, Neuroimage execution process [3], with the first activity consistent in 10 (1999) 682 694. latency to the motor field (MF), the second to the [4] J. Dammers, A.A. Ioannides, Neuromagnetic localization of CMV generators using incomplete and full-head biomagnetometer, Neuromovement-evoked field I (MFEI) [5,9]. The activities in image 11 (2000) 167 178. the superior part of the PPC exhibit some inter-subject [5] H. Endo, T. Kizuka, T. Masuda, T. Takeda, Automatic activation in variability for location, not too surprising a finding given the human primary motor cortex synchronized with movement the highly variable and asymmetrical surface anatomy of preparation, Cogn. Brain Res. 3 (1999) 229 239. this area [15]. The strongest loci of activity were within the [6] M. Fuchs, M. Wagner, T. Kohler, H.A. Wischmann, Linear and nonlinear current density reconstructions, J. Clin. Neurophysiol. 16 SPL and in the Brodmann s areas 5 and 7a. The onset of (1999) 267 295. the SPL activity preceded the EMG onset and the motor- [7] J.F. Kalaska, D.J. Crammond, Deciding not to GO: neuronal related activities. The ANOVA showed that each SPL was correlates of response selection in a GO/ NOGO task in primate significantly strongly activated with responses which in- premotor and parietal cortex, Cereb. Cortex 5 (1995) 410 428. [8] T. Knosche, cluded the contralateral finger movement. Such SPL P. Praamstra, D. Stegeman, M. Peters, Linear estimation discriminates midline sources and a motor cortex contribution to the activity was not observed in unilateral voluntary finger readiness potential, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 99 movement [3]. (1996) 183 190. The latency of our SPL activity is consistent with a [9] R. Kristeva, D. Cheyne, L. Deecke, Neuromagnetic fields accompanying P300 type response which is implicated with activity in the unilateral and bilateral voluntary movements: topography temporal-parietal cortex [11]. This explanation is neverthephysiol. 81 (1991) 284 298. and analysis of cortical sources, Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neuro- less rejected because of the laterality of our SPL response [10] M.J. Liu, P.B.C. Fenwick, J. Lumsden, J.C. Lever, K.M. Stephan, relative to the finger movement. Although correspondence A.A. Ioannides, Averaged and single-trial analysis of cortical between function in the parietal cortex of monkeys and activation sequences in movement preparation, initiation, and inhibition, humans is not straightforward [1], recent monkey studies Hum. Brain Mapp. 4 (1996) 254 264. provided useful analogies to the SPL activity we have [11] V. Menon, J.M. Ford, K.O. Lim, G.H. Glover, A. Pfefferbaum, Combined event-related fmri and EEG evidence for temporalidentified. In these studies, the PPC activity was associated parietal cortex activation during target detection, Neuroreport 8 with coordinate transformation operations [2], information (1997) 3029 3037. processing directly linked to motor planning [12,14], and [12] H. Sakata, M. Taira, A. Murata, S. Mine, Neural mechanisms of

168 T. Shibata, A.A. Ioannides / Brain Research 897 (2001) 164 168 visual guidance of hand action in the parietal cortex of the monkey, [14] L.H. Snyder, A.P. Batista, R.A. Andersen, Coding of intention in the Cereb. Cortex 5 (1995) 429 438. posterior parietal cortex, Nature 386 (1997) 167 170. [13] T. Shibata, I. Shimoyama, T. Ito, D. Abla, H. Iwasa, K. Koseki, N. [15] S.F. Witelson, D.L. Kigar, Sylvian fissure morphology and Yamanouchi, T. Sato, Y. Nakajima, The synchronization between asymmetry in men and women: bilateral differences in relation to brain areas under motor inhibition process in humans estimated by handedness in men, J. Comp. Neurol. 323 (1992) 326 340. event-related EEG coherence, Neurosci. Res. 31 (1998) 265 271.