THE "FOUR STIMULUS - TWO CHOICE " - PARADIGM IN MULTIDIMENSIONAL PSYCHOPHYSICS: SIZE, BRIGHTNESS, AND COLOR DIMENSIONS COMBINED *

Similar documents
MODELS FOR THE ADJUSTMENT OF RATING SCALES 1

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Free classification: Element-level and subgroup-level similarity

JUDGMENT DYNAMICS IN SUPRATHRESHOLD COLOR PSYCHOPHYSICS

Learning to classify integral-dimension stimuli

SUBJECTIVE RESPONSES TO HAND-ARM VIBRATION: IMPLICATIONS FOR FREQUENCY-WEIGHTING AND GENDER DIFFERENCES

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Human experiment - Training Procedure - Matching of stimuli between rats and humans - Data analysis

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

ADAPTATION TO RACIAL CONTENT OF EMERGENT RACE FACES: GRADIENT SHIFT OR PEAK SHIFT?

Bar Ilan University Ramat-Gan, Israel. Ph.D. in Cognitive Psychology 1999.

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

PSYC 441 Cognitive Psychology II

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

= add definition here. Definition Slide

. R EAS O ~ I NG BY NONHUMAN

PERCEPTUAL Motor Development

A Review of Capacity Limitation From Visual Perception to Short-Term Visual Memory of a Single Curved Contour. Koji Sakai

EVOKED RESPONSE CORRELATES OF LETTER RECOGNITION

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Analyzing Distances in a Frontal Plane. Stephen Dopkins. The George Washington University. Jesse Sargent. Francis Marion University

The term stereotype comes from the Greek, where the stereós means solid, hard, and typos. Stereotype

Automatic detection, consistent mapping, and training * Originally appeared in

Lesson 5 Sensation, Perception, Memory, and The Conscious Mind

The Difference of Imagery Perception between Visual and Tactile Sensation

General recognition theory of categorization: A MATLAB toolbox

Although most previous studies on categorization

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2016 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness

Seeing Color. Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms. Brindley (1960) Psychophysical Linking Hypotheses

LAB 4 BALANCE, PERCEPTUAL JUDGMENT, AND FINE MOTOR SKILLS DEVELOPMENT

Discrimination and Generalization in Pattern Categorization: A Case for Elemental Associative Learning

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Fukuoka University of Education

APPLICATION OF FUZZY SIGNAL DETECTION THEORY TO VIGILANCE: THE EFFECT OF CRITERION SHIFTS

Modeling Human Perception

A frame-based analysis of synaesthetic metaphors

Aesthetic preferences in the size of images of real-world objects

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

The Role of Feedback in Categorisation

Psych 333, Winter 2008, Instructor Boynton, Exam 2

Behavioural Brain Research

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Confidence in Causal Inferences: The Case of Devaluation


Psychology of Perception PSYC Spring 2017 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness

Absolute Identification Is Relative: A Reply to Brown, Marley, and Lacouture (2007)

Publications Blough, D. S. Dark adaptation in the pigeon. Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Ratliff, F., & Blough, D. S.

LIFETIME OF MEMORY FOR SELF-GENERATED INFORMATION

The effect of time pressure and the spatial integration of the stimulus dimensions on overall similarity categorization.

Lab 3: Perception of Loudness

innate mechanism of proportionality adaptation stage activation or recognition stage innate biological metrics acquired social metrics

An Experimental Study of the Relationship Between Focality and Short-Term Memory of Colors in the Japanese Language

Auditory temporal order and perceived fusion-nonfusion

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

Blocking Effects on Dimensions: How attentional focus on values can spill over to the dimension level

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

The Color Between Two Others

METHODOLOGICAL STUDIES OF SIZE CONSTANCY (1) : SIZE CONSTANCY MEASURED BY TWO EXPERIMENTAL METHODS

Using Inverse Planning and Theory of Mind for Social Goal Inference

Jeffrey R. W. Mounts Curriculum Vita March Education: 1995 Ph.D. (Cognitive Psychology) Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN

Demonstrations of limitations in the way humans process and

CONCEPT LEARNING WITH DIFFERING SEQUENCES OF INSTANCES

Gathering and Repetition of the Elements in an Image Affect the Perception of Order and Disorder

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3

Classifying pictures and words: Implications for the dual-coding hypothesis

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

Effects of context in judgments of sweetness and pleasantness

Detection of Task-Relevant Cues in Field Hockey

The Influence of Perceptual Difficulty on Family Resemblance Sorting

Lab 4: Perception of Loudness

Categorical Perception

Eye fixations to figures in a four-choice situation with luminance balanced areas: Evaluating practice effects

Colour Fitting Formulae Used for Predicting Colour Quality of Light Sources

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

Span Theory: An overview

Offsets and prioritizing the selection of new elements in search displays: More evidence for attentional capture in the preview effect

The Color of Similarity

Detection Theory: Sensory and Decision Processes

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

How Categories Shape Causality

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

Emoacoustics: a study of the physical and psychological dimensions of emotional sound design

Mechanisms of generalization perceptual learning

Transcription:

THE "FOUR STIMULUS - TWO CHOICE " - PARADIGM IN MULTIDIMENSIONAL PSYCHOPHYSICS: SIZE, BRIGHTNESS, AND COLOR DIMENSIONS COMBINED * Petra Hauf & Viktor Sarris Institute of Psychology, University of Frankfurt, Mertonstr. 17, D-654 Frankfurt/M. P.Hauf@psych.uni-frankfurt.de Abstract Multidimensional judgment strategies were investigated by means of a Four Stimulus Two Choice paradigm (4A2C) which combined different dimensions, namely two gradations of size and nine shades of brightness, two shades of brightness and nine gradations of size, or two colors and nine gradations of size. Three types of hypothetical strategies were analyzed: relative judgments based on only one dimension (type I), absolute judgments based on the similarity between test and training stimuli (type II), and relative judgments based on both dimensions (type III). The stimuli consisted of fairy-tale figures differing in size and brightness in the first condition, brightness and size in the second condition, and color and size in the last one. A total of 144 participants (4 and 18 year-olds) were tested individually. During the discrimination training a basic categorization was learned. Whereas the 18 years old did not show any categorization differences depending on the dimensions employed, the 4-year-olds did (weaker results with color and size). During the subsequent test period all three judgment types appeared in both age groups and characterized the main part of the analyzed judgments. Additionally, the 18-year-olds showed no distinct preference for one of the postulated judgment types, while the 4-year-olds changed their preferences depending on the combination of dimensions (type I with size and brightness, and type II with color and size). The two-dimensional judgment type III remained constant and was used infrequently in both age groups. Further investigations will be concerned with two-dimensional psychophysics in animals (see also Sarris, Hauf & Arlt, 21 in this volume). The frame-of-reference (FR) approach in psychophysics deals systematically with the interaction between focal stimuli and their context (Lockhead, 1992; Sarris, 1994, 21). Until recently, most FR models predict context-induced changes in judgments solely in one- * Datas also presented at the 1 st Annual Meeting of the Vision ScienceS Society, Sarasota, Fl., USA in May 21.

dimensional stimulus situations. They have neglected the fact that most natural objects differ in more than one stimulus dimension (Zoeke & Sarris, 1983). In everyday life perceptual judgments are usually based on multi-dimensional objects. Therefore different kinds of information have to be integrated before judgments can take place. A simple two-categorical example illustrates this problem: The size-judgment of a person will depend on the frame-of-reference which is used. Everyone knows that Jockeys are usually small and basketball players tall ; but at the same time we are able to judge the same average-sized man as being tall, if he was introduced as a Jockey, and being small if he was classified as a basketball player. Our present experiments illustrate this common phenomenon by analyzing context-induced judgments across a combination of the dimensions size, brightness, and color. Thereby, the main objective was the study of age-related developments of two-dimensional perceptualcognitive judgment strategies. M ETHOD A total of 144 participants in two age groups was trained and tested individually. During a discrimination training, a set of two-dimensional psychophysical stimuli was learned by means of a Four Stimulus Two Choice Paradigm (4A2C-method). The two-dimensional stimulus-generalization tests were presented subsequently (test phase). One group was tested under a size-brightness context condition, another under a brightness-size condition, and the last group under a color-size context condition. The stimuli (fairytale figures) were shown with a special slide projector (see Fig.1). The participant sat in front of the screen and had to judge the size of each stimulus by pressing the corresponding response key (e.g. small = left button, large = right button). The stimuli differed in size (9 gradations) and color (blue and green) and were presented successively. Fig.1: Apparatus.- Slide projector with screen and a board with two response keys. By pressing corresponding button (e.g. small = left button, large = right button) the participant had to judge the size of the stimuli which were presented successively.

During training the participant learned the correct categorization between the small blue and the large blue stimulus on the one hand and the small green and the large green on the other hand. The smaller stimulus of each pair belonged together in one category, the larger ones to the other category (4A2C-method). After reaching a test criterion of 2 correct responses in sequence two-dimensional generalization tests were administered (all sizes in both colors; see Fig. 2). Set of Training and Test Stimuli Training Stimuli Test Stimuli Stimulus Number small large Fig.2: Set of Training and Test Stimuli.- Shown are the training and test stimuli in blue or green (color-size condition: blue printed in white, green in grey). During training the participant learned the discrimination between the small blue and the large blue stimulus on the one hand and the small green and the large green on the other hand. The smaller ones of each pair belonged together to one category, the larger ones to the other (4A2C-method). During the test all sizes in both colors were shown. (The marked numbers refer to the training stimuli) The training and test stimuli of the other conditions were analogous. Predictions Based on this paradigm three hypothetical judgment types were expected: relative judgments based on only one dimension (type I), absolute judgments based on the similarity between test and training stimuli (type II), and relative judgments based on both dimensions (type III; see Fig. 3, next page).

P r e d i c t i o n s Type I one dimension only similarity both dimensions 1 1 1 R e s p o n s e s (%) S t i m u l u s S t i m u l u s S t i m u l u s Fig.3: Predictions.- Shown are three types of hypothetical judgment strategies (test phase): Judgments based on one dimension only (Type I); judgments based on the similarity between test and training stimuli (); judgments based on both dimensions (). R E S U LTS Two-dimensional Discrimination Training.- During the discrimination training a basic twodimensional categorization was learned. All participants showed at least 9% correct responses. Whereas the 18 year-old participants did not show any differences depending on the dimensions employed, the 4-year-olds did. There were weaker results for the color-size condition in this age group. Two-dimensional Choice Behavior.- For analyzing the choice behavior during the test sessions the percentage of choice responses were plotted separately for both age groups as well as for the three judgment types. These empirical psychophysical functions are shown in Figure 4. During the test period all three predicted types of judgment were found in both age groups and characterized the main part of the analyzed behavior. No significant differences in the manifestation of the types were found, neither between the age groups, nor between the combination of stimulus dimensions used. Age-related Frequency of Strategies.- Subsequent analyses were occupied with the frequencies of the three postulated judgment types depending on the combination of dimensions used. Figure 5 shows the age-related occurrence of the three judgment types for the size-brightness condition, the brightness-size condition and the color-size condition. The 18-year-olds showed no distinct preference for one of the postulated judgment types, while the 4-year-olds changed their preferences depending on the combination of dimensions: type I (based on one dimension only) with size and brightness resp. brightness and size, and type II (based on similarity) with color and size. The two-dimensional type III (based on both dimensions) remained constant and was used infrequently in both age groups.

Two-dimensional Choice Behavior Type I one dimension only similarity both dimensions 4 years 1 1 1 Responses (%) 18 years 1 1 1 Responses (%) Stimulus Stimulus Stimulus Fig.4: Two-dimensional choice behavior.- Shown are the empirical psychophysical functions of choice responses (%) separately for both age groups as well as for the three judgment types depending on the used combination of dimensions ( size-brightness; brightness-size; color-size). Age-related Frequency of Strategies size - brightness brightness - size color - size Occurrence (%) 1 75 25 Type I 1 75 25 Type I 1 75 25 Type I 4 18 Age (years) 4 18 Age (years) 4 18 Age (years) Fig.5: Age-related frequency of strategies.- Shown are the age-related percentage of occurrence for the three judgment types. Left: size-brightness condition; middle: brightness-size condition and right color-size condition. The 18-year-olds showed no distinct preference, while the 4-year-olds did (Type I with the dimensions size and brightness, but type II with the dimension color.

D I S CUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS During the discrimination training not only the older participants, but even the 4-year-old children were able to manage the two-dimensional categorization task. In the subsequent generalization test conditions the predicted context-induced judgment strategies occurred in both age groups, regardless of which psychophysical dimensions were used. This occurrence demonstrated age-related differences depending on the combination of dimensions used. The 4-year-old children preferred judgments for the color-size condition, and Type I judgments for the remaining conditions. Summarizing the training and test data, the color dimension seems to be particularly important for the 4-year-olds. In sessions with color dimension, they master the discrimination training using a strategy of memorizing the absolute values of the stimuli and afterwards demonstrating one-dimensional, absolute choice behavior () during the test sessions. In contrast the dimensions size and brightness are learned relative relative to each other and therefore lead to a preference in Type I judgments. Interestingly enough comparative studies with baby chickens also indicate a distinct difference in the absolute resp. relative components of choice behavior depending on color or size dimensions (see Sarris, Hauf & Arlt, 21, this volume). Further investigations should not only continue to analyze dimension-related differences in choice behavior, but also how age-related differences in cognitive abilities, such as memory and problem solving interact with judgment types (Hauf, 21). R E F E R E NCES Hauf, P. (21). Untersuchungen zum altersspezifischen mehrdimensionalen perzeptiv-kognitiven Urteilsverhalten in der Psychophysik. In F. Wilkening et al. (Hrsg.), Psychologia Universalis, Neue Reihe, Bd. 24. Lengerich: Pabst (im Druck). Hauf, P. & Sarris, V. (21, May). Multidimensional choice behavior in developmental psychophysics: Size, brightness, and color combined. Poster presented at the 1 st Annual Meeting of the Vision ScienceS Society, Sarasota, Fl., USA. Lockhead, G. R. (1992). Psychophysical scaling: Judgments of attributes or objects? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 543-61. Sarris, V. (1994). Contextual effects in animal psychophysics: Comparative perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17, 763-764. Sarris, V. (21, in press). Frame of reference models in psychophysics: A perceptual-cognitive approach. In C. Kaernbach et al. (Eds.), Perception beyond sensation. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Press. Sarris, V., Hauf, P. & Arlt, M. (21, May). One- and two-dimensional psychophysics in humans and chickens: Size and color data. Poster presented at the 1 st Annual Meeting of the Vision ScienceS Society, Sarasota, Fl., USA. Zoeke, B. & Sarris, V. (1983). A comparison of frame-of-reference paradigms in human and animal psychophysics. In H.-G. Geissler, H. F. J. M. Buffart, E. L. J. Leeuwenberg & V. Sarris (Eds). Modern issues in perception. (pp. 283-317). Berlin, VEB Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften.