Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems

Similar documents
Stimulus-Response Compatibilitiy Effects for Warning Signals and Steering Responses

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

Available online at ScienceDirect. Procedia Manufacturing 3 (2015 )

The Danger of Incorrect Expectations In Driving: The Failure to Respond

OPTIC FLOW IN DRIVING SIMULATORS

Model-Based Human-Centered Task Automation: A Case Study in ACC System Design

DRIVER S SITUATION AWARENESS DURING SUPERVISION OF AUTOMATED CONTROL Comparison between SART and SAGAT measurement techniques

[EN-A-022] Analysis of Positive and Negative Effects of Salience on the ATC Task Performance

THE SPATIAL EXTENT OF ATTENTION DURING DRIVING

Modeling the Real World using STISIM Drive Simulation Software: A Study Contrasting High and Low Locality Simulations.

TIME-TO-CONTACT AND COLLISION DETECTION ESTIMATIONS AS MEASURES OF DRIVING SAFETY IN OLD AND DEMENTIA DRIVERS

A Process Tracing Approach to the Investigation of Situated Cognition

Subject: The Case for a Provincial 0.00% BAC Limit for All Drivers Under the Age of 21

Estimation of Driver Inattention to Forward Objects Using Facial Direction with Application to Forward Collision Avoidance Systems

A FIELD STUDY ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE, VISUAL ATTENTION, HEART RATE AND SUBJECTIVE RATINGS IN RESPONSE TO TWO TYPES OF COGNITIVE WORKLOAD

Verbal Collision Avoidance Messages of Varying Perceived Urgency Reduce Crashes in High Risk Scenarios

On the Fast Lane to Road Rage

2012 Course: The Statistician Brain: the Bayesian Revolution in Cognitive Sciences


Distracted Driving Effects on CMV Operators

Designing for Situation Awareness -the world behind the glass- EUROCONTROL scientific seminar January 21, 2011 prof dr ir Max Mulder

Overestimation of Skills Affects Drivers Adaptation to Task Demands

Ecological Interface to Enhance User Performance in Adjusting Computer-Controlled Multihead Weigher

Detecting and Reading Text on HUDs: Effects of Driving Workload and Message Location

Aging and the Detection of Collision Events in Fog

CAN WE PREDICT STEERING CONTROL PERFORMANCE FROM A 2D SHAPE DETECTION TASK?

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Iran. T. Allahyari, J. Environ. et Health. al., USEFUL Sci. Eng., FIELD 2007, OF Vol. VIEW 4, No. AND 2, RISK pp OF... processing system, i.e

Koji Sakai. Kyoto Koka Women s University, Ukyo-ku Kyoto, Japan

mycopter: Enabling Technologies for Personal Aerial Transportation Systems

An Investigation of the Effect of Latency on the Operator's Trust and Performance for Manual Multi-robot Teleoperated Tasks

The role of memory on patients with mild cognitive impairment. Sophia Vardaki, PhD National Technical University of Athens

Expert Focus Group on Driver Distraction:

Characterizing Visual Attention during Driving and Non-driving Hazard Perception Tasks in a Simulated Environment

Use of Structure Mapping Theory for Complex Systems

Partially Automated Driving as a Fallback Level of High Automation

Divided-attention task on driving simulator: comparison among three groups of drivers

Alcohol-impaired driving in the lab: Efforts to reduce the incidence of drinking and driving

Eye Movement Patterns and Driving Performance

DRIVING HAZARD DETECTION WITH A BIOPTIC TELESCOPE

Cognitive and Psychomotor Correlates of Self- Reported Driving Skills and Behavior

Pupil Dilation as an Indicator of Cognitive Workload in Human-Computer Interaction

COGNITIVE AND PSYCHOMOTOR CORRELATES OF HAZARD PERCEPTION ABILITY AND RISKY DRIVING

The Effect of Glare in Shnulated Night Driving

Naturalistic Driving Performance During Secondary Tasks

I just didn t see it It s all about hazard perception. Dr. Robert B. Isler Associate Professor School of Psychology University of Waikato Hamilton

Traffic Sign Detection and Identification

THE DIMENSIONS OF DRIVER PERFORMANCE DURING SECONDARY MANUAL TASKS

A Simulator-Based Street-Crossing Training for Older Pedestrians: Short and Long Term Effects

Pupillary Response Based Cognitive Workload Measurement under Luminance Changes

Observations on the Use of Ontologies for Autonomous Vehicle Navigation Planning

A NOVEL VARIABLE SELECTION METHOD BASED ON FREQUENT PATTERN TREE FOR REAL-TIME TRAFFIC ACCIDENT RISK PREDICTION

Distracted Driving among Teens. What We Know about It and How to Prevent It May 31 st, 2017

Attention Estimation by Simultaneous Observation of Viewer and View

Validity of Haptic Cues and Its Effect on Priming Visual Spatial Attention

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

SELECTIVE ATTENTION AND CONFIDENCE CALIBRATION

ScienceDirect. Towards a social psychology-based microscopic model of driver behavior and decision-making: modifying Lewin s Field Theory

Using the method of adjustment to enhance collision warning perception

In-Vehicle Communication and Driving: An Attempt to Overcome their Interference

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

SPATIAL AUDIO VS. VERBAL DIRECTIONAL CUES: AN EXAMINATION OF SALIENCE AND DISRUPTIVENESS WITHIN A SIMULATED DRIVING CONTEXT

Using A Logic Model Framework For Program Planning And Evaluation

DRIVER BEHAVIOUR STUDIES IN THE MOTORWAY OPERATIONS PLATFORM GRANT

Does Workload Modulate the Effects of In-Vehicle Display Location on Concurrent Driving and Side Task Performance?

CS 771 Artificial Intelligence. Intelligent Agents

Identification of human factors criteria to assess the effects of level crossing safety measures

Semiotics and Intelligent Control

ASSESSING DRIVING PERFORMANCE WITH MODERATE VISUAL FIELD LOSS

From Few to Many: Using Copulas and Monte Carlo Simulation to Estimate Safety Consequences

Supporting operator reliance on automation through continuous feedback

A critical analysis of an applied psychology journal about the effects of driving fatigue: Driver fatigue and highway driving: A simulator study.

Artificial Intelligence CS 6364

APPENDIX FOR "DRIVING SIMULATOR FOR PERSONS WITH IMPAIRED COGNITION", PI WILLIAM K DURFEE

Correlating Trust with Signal Detection Theory Measures in a Hybrid Inspection System

Cooperative Intersection Collision Avoidance System Stop Sign Assist: Experiments to Validate Use of an In-Vehicle Interface Design

Augmented Cognition to enhance human sensory awareness, cognitive functioning and psychic functioning: a research proposal in two phases

ROAD SAFETY MONITOR. ALCOHOL-IMPAIRED DRIVING IN THE UNITED STATES Results from the 2018 TIRF USA Road Safety Monitor

Driver Distraction: Towards A Working Definition

THE TACTILE DETECTION RESPONSE TASK: PRELIMINARY VALIDATION FOR MEASURING THE ATTENTIONAL EFFECTS OF COGNITIVE LOAD

Proceedings of the 2012 Winter Simulation Conference C. Laroque, J. Himmelspach, R. Pasupathy, O. Rose, and A.M. Uhrmacher, eds

Driving Under the Influence - of Allergy Medicine

EMG-Driven Human Model for Orthosis Control

THE DANGERS OF DROWSY DRIVING. The Costs, Risks, and Prevention of Driver Fatigue

Assessing cognition in ELDERLY drivers

A SAMPLING MODEL TO ASCERTAIN AUTOMATION-INDUCED COMPLACENCY IN MULTI-TASK ENVIRONMENTS

PREVENTING DISTRACTED DRIVING. Maintaining Focus Behind the Wheel of a School Bus

Pushing the Right Buttons: Design Characteristics of Touch Screen Buttons

Models of Information Retrieval

Capturing Driver Response to In-Vehicle Human- Machine Interface Technologies Using Facial Thermography

REACTION TIME MEASUREMENT APPLIED TO MULTIMODAL HUMAN CONTROL MODELING

STPA Applied to Automotive Automated Parking Assist

Test-Retest Reliability of Simulated Driving Performance: A Pilot Study

Driver Alertness Detection Research Using Capacitive Sensor Array

Chapter 5 Car driving

TRAINING TO IMRPOVE COLLISION DETECTION IN OLDER ADULTS

Spatial Orientation Using Map Displays: A Model of the Influence of Target Location

2017 Human Performance Conference

A PCT Primer. Fred Nickols 6/30/2011

Transcription:

Application of ecological interface design to driver support systems J.D. Lee, J.D. Hoffman, H.A. Stoner, B.D. Seppelt, and M.D. Brown Department of Mechanical and Industrial Engineering, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA Center for Computer Aided Design, 116 ERF, Iowa City, IA 52242, USA Abstract This paper presents the results of two experiments to assess the benefits of using Cognitive Work Analysis (CWA) and Ecological Interface Design (EID) to guide the development of driver support systems. EID suggests novel interface alternatives that have substantial theoretical promise. However, it is not possible to anticipate how drivers will perceive and respond to these displays. Two experiments compared a traditional display with three EID-inspired displays and found complementary results regarding the ability of these displays to support the judgment of safe gaps in a lane change situation. The first experiment showed that participants adapted with a similar speed to the traditional and EID-inspired displays and that one EID-inspired display performed at least as well as the traditional display while promoting more precise calibration between judgment accuracy and confidence in the judgment. The second experiment showed that when participants could only view the situation for a short period, an EID-inspired display substantially outperformed the more traditional display. Importantly, not all the EID-inspired displays outperformed the traditional display. The results are discussed in terms of alternate evaluation venues for emerging in-vehicle technology. Keywords: Collision warning systems, Automation, cognitive work analysis, adaptive cruise control, lane change 1. Introduction Emerging vehicle sensor and control technologies offer great potential for improving driver safety and performance. These technologies, which range from adaptive cruise control to steering assist systems, promise to change the driving task substantially [1]. Without a principled approach to design, it can be quite difficult to develop this new technology in a way that enhances rather than degrades driver performance and satisfaction. The CWA (Cognitive Work Analysis) and EID (Ecological Interface Design) frameworks may provide the underlying structure for such a principled approach to design driver support systems [2, 3]. A focus on identifying domain constraints that govern effective behavior distinguishes EID and CWA from most other human-technology design approaches [2]. Interfaces that represent domain constraints in a way that is compatible with human perceptual and cognitive capabilities have met with success in domains as diverse as neonatal intensive care to nuclear power plants [4]. CWA and EID aim to support an adaptive response to both routine and unanticipated situations. This design philosophy may also benefit the driving domain, where a heterogeneous driver population adapts in various ways to a diverse set of driving situations.

Lane change maneuvers might particularly benefit from EID. EID may help transform radarbased sensor data into a visual representation drivers can use to make better judgments regarding the safety of a potential lane change. In most lane change crashes drivers do not attempt an avoidance maneuver, suggesting the driver is not aware of the impending collision [5, 6]. More specifically, approximately 75% of lane change or merge crashes involve the failure of the driver to recognize the impending collision [7]. Using EID to create a visual representation of the threat posed by an approaching vehicle may help drivers make better judgments regarding whether it is safe to merge into a lane. The philosophy of EID differs substantially from that of many driver support systems. The focus of many systems is to provide discrete warnings that alert drivers to threatening situations. These warnings are often binary rather than graded and provide little information regarding the type of threat or how it is evolving over time. EID inspires displays that show information at multiple levels of abstraction and leave the determination of what constitutes a threat up to the driver. Traditional driver support systems often use a many-to-one mapping of conditions to a single indicator. This has the advantage of simplicity, but lacks the ability to support the full range of drivers and driving situations. In contrast, EID reveals the full many-tofew mapping of the environment state. Although theoretically promising, the response of drivers to ecological interfaces is uncertain. A strength of ecological interfaces is their ability to translate low-level data into meaningful information that is made accessible to people through configural graphics. This often inspires relatively complex and unfamiliar displays, which may undermine rather than enhance driver performance. Display complexity may be a particular challenge in applying EID to driving because driving demands decisions that are made in seconds, compared to other domains where decision are made over many minutes. To address these issues we conducted two experiments. In the first we exposed drivers to many lane change decisions to assess how increasing familiarity influences display effectiveness. In the second experiment, we manipulated the time available to monitor the situation to assess performance when only short glances are possible. 2. Methods 2.1. Participants Drivers between the ages of 18 and 40 participated in these experiments. Twenty-four drivers (M = 22, SD = 4.6) participated in the first experiment, and 32 drivers (M = 22, SD = 3.3) participated in the second experiment. All were licensed drivers and had normal or corrected to normal vision. Each participant was paid a base rate of $15.00 for their time. To encourage the participants to make correct decisions, performance above 33% was rewarded at $0.15 per 1%. For example, if a participant scored 80%, they received $22.05 ($15 (base rate) + $0.15*(80-33)). 2.2. Apparatus Data were collected using a part-task simulation that showed only the left side-view mirror of a car. The lane-change display was shown on an image of the mirror as it would appear on a real vehicle. Figure 1 shows the combined image as it appeared on a 19 inch computer monitor at a resolution of 1280 x 1024. Participants did not drive a vehicle, but instead judged whether or not a lane change would be safe by watching the behavior of the display for 2 seconds in the first experiment and between 0.25 and 2.0 seconds in the second experiment. The scenarios developed for this experiment were chosen from a state space of range and range rate. Range or R is defined as the distance between the participants vehicle and the vehicle approaching from behind. Range rate or R & is defined as the difference in velocities between the two vehicles. This space describes a range of lane change situations, some of which were defined as safe, others as safe with acceleration, and others as unsafe. These determinations were made using the equations of motion, assuming the approaching vehicle would remain at a constant speed, and the time headway must remain above two seconds. Figure 2 shows schematic diagrams of the EIDinspired displays. The more traditional display, not included in Figure 2, was simply a horizontal bar that was proportional to a weighted combination of the inverse time-to-collision and time headway.

Figure 1. Experimental apparatus showing the EID-Yield display and the image of the car mirror that the participants used to judge whether it was safe to pass or not. Range - 1 ( t ) Time Headway - 1 Time to Collision - 1 =.5 sec Or Time to Collision = 2 sec. R a n g e R a t e (t ) Time Headway ( t ) Range - 1 (t - 2 ) Slope TTC V e h ic le Proximity Zone D is ta n c e i - 1 Time to Collision - 1 V e h ic le Time Headway ( t) Range Rate T im e t o C o llis io n - 1 V e h ic le Time headway 2 seconds Time headway 2 seconds EID Relative EID Yield Figure 2. The three EID-inspired displays (EID-Relative motion, EID-TTC, and EID-Yield). 2.3. Experimental design Experiment 1 was a 4 (display type) x 8 (block) between and within experimental design, with display as a between subjects variable and block as a within-subjects variable. The four levels of display type corresponded to the four displays shown above (, EID-TTC, EID-Relative, EID- Yield). Block had eight levels corresponding to the eight experimental blocks of 50 trials that each participant experienced. Experiment 2 was a 4 (display type) x 4 (viewing time) between and within experimental design, with display as a between-subjects variable and viewing time as a within subject variable. As in experiment 1, display type had four levels corresponding to the four displays described above. Viewing time had four levels: 0.25, 0.5, 1.0, and 2.0 seconds. 2.4. Procedure Upon arrival, each participant was randomly assigned to one of the four display groups. The lane change judgment task was explained and participants practiced with a block of 50 trials. Each trial in this block was a unique lane change situation. Following the practice, participants completed eight additional blocks of 50 trials for a total of 450 experimental trials. Participants initiated each trial by clicking on an on-screen button. After the button press, participants watched the scenario evolve for two seconds in experiment 1, or for viewing times between 0.25 and 2.0 seconds in experiment 2. They were then required to decide whether a lane change was safe, safe if they accelerated during the maneuver, or unsafe, and provide a rating of their confidence in their decision from 0 to 100%. These responses were entered by clicking on the scales

and buttons shown on the right of Figure1. Participants were then given feedback that their judgment was Correct or Incorrect. If the response was Incorrect, they received additional feedback explaining why the response was incorrect. At the end of each block of 50 trials, participants rated the workload experienced during the block using the NASA TLX scale. 3. Results Figure 3 shows that the display outperforms most of the other displays in terms of decision time, F(3,20)= 16.21, p<0.0001, and accuracy, F(3,20) =15.34, p<0.0001. However, the EID-Yield display performs at essentially the same level after the first few blocks. As might be expected, the workload, as rated with the NASA TLX subjective workload scale, shows that the simple bar display imposed the least load, F(3,20)=5.56, p=0.0061. For all measures, there were no interactions between display and block, F<0.5. Table 1 shows a higher correlation between confidence and decision accuracy for the EID-Yield display compared to the display, particularly during the unsafe passing situations. Table 1. Correlations between confidence and accuracy for each type of situation by display. Unsafe Safe with Safe Acceleration -0.01 0.36 0.66 EID-TTC 0.22 0.29 0.06 EID-Relative 0.33 0.45 0.55 EID-Yield 0.44 0.49 0.40 2.5 2 100 Decision RT (s) 1.5 1 Percent Correct 90 80 70 0.5 60 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Block 50 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Block Figure 3. Decision time and accuracy across time. 2.5 2 100 90 Decision RT (s) 1.5 1 Percent Correct 80 70 0.5 60 0 50 0.25 0.5 1 2 View Time (s) Figure 4. Decision time and accuracy as a function of view times. 0.25 0.5 1 2 View Time (s)

Figure 4 shows main effects of display type for both reaction time, F(3,28)=10.44, p<.0001, and judgment accuracy, F(3,28)=34.6, p<.0001, when the viewing time was varied. In terms of reaction time, the EID-Relative display had a substantially longer reaction time than the others. In terms of accuracy, the EID-Yield display outperformed the other displays across all viewing times. Longer viewing time resulted in shorter reaction times for all the displays, F(3,84)=4.82, p=0.0038. In contrast, the decision accuracy depended on an interaction between display type and viewing time, F(9,84)=3.76, p=0.0005. As the viewing time decreased the performance of the display deteriorated. In contrast, the EID-Yield display was remarkably robust to changes in the viewing time, while performance with the other ecological displays actually declined as the viewing time increased. The EID-TTC display and the EID-Yield display consistently supported more confident judgments, F(3,28)=22.73, p<.0001, generally with lower workload, F(3,28)=16.76, p<.0001, compared to the and EID-Relative displays. The display resulted in lower confidence and higher workload, particularly for short viewing times. 4. Discussion The two experiments provide complementary results regarding the assessment of four potential displays to support the judgment of safe gaps in the lane change situation. The first experiment showed that the and EID-Yield displays outperform the others on the basis of performance (judgment accuracy and reaction time) as well as attitudes (confidence and workload). Although performance and attitudes changed with experience these changes did not depend on the display type. People seemed to adapt to all the displays at a similar rate. Interestingly, the display performed as well as or better than all the EID-inspired displays in the first experiment, suggesting that the EID-inspired displays may be inferior. However, such a conclusion must be tempered by considering the correlation between judgment accuracy and judgment confidence. Both the and the EID- TTC displays showed very poor correspondence between confidence and accuracy for certain decision situations. Specifically, there was little link between confidence and accuracy in unsafe passing situations. Although judgment accuracy is a critical performance metric, the calibration of judgment accuracy and confidence may be just as important. On this metric, the display performed poorly, which may reflect the many-toone mapping of conditions based on a single level of abstraction included in the display. This metric is particularly important because displays that make it difficult for drivers to calibrate their confidence with their judgment accuracy will leave them unlikely to use the driver support system in safety critical situations. The second experiment provided an important complement to the first by examining the effect of time pressure on the judgment task. In most lane change situations, drivers will not have two seconds to watch a display. Instead, they may have less than half a second. The second experiment showed that the duration people could view the display had a strong effect on performance. More importantly, this effect depended on display type. The display performed particularly poorly for short viewing times, while the EID-Yield display performed well over a range of viewing times. This effect was also reflected in the attitudes. Workload was higher for the display for short durations compared to longer viewing times and confidence was generally low for judgments with the display. These results show that display quality must be assessed over a range of situations. Robust performance across situations may be a more critical design consideration than relatively high performance in a single situation. Overall, these results show that drivers may not always be able to extract information efficiently from the relatively complex EID-inspired displays. The complexity of some of the EID images seemed to hinder performance in the longer duration exposures, possibly reflecting an overload of visual working memory due to the amount of information presented in the display. In the case of the traditional display, for long viewing times, it seems drivers were able to extract rate-based information from the display, but this ability was compromised by short viewing times. The robust performance of the EID-Yield display suggests that it enabled drivers to perceive ratebased information in a way that did not depend on viewing time. This finding suggests that the EID- Yield display may represent a novel and particularly promising way of presenting

information to drivers regarding the relative motion of vehicles. Moreover, these results show that EID can inspire novel displays that can outperform conventional displays, but that these require empirical evaluation. These results also point towards part-task simulation as a way of efficiently evaluating potential displays. The part-task simulation made it possible to collect data for tens of thousands of lane change judgments. Collecting similar data in a fullscale simulator or with instrumented vehicles would have cost much more and taken months rather than weeks. The part-task evaluation in this experiment tests the compatibility of a display with basic perceptual and cognitive capabilities of drivers. This compatibility is a necessary, but not sufficient criterion for success [8]. A full evaluation requires full-scale simulator and on-road studies. The simple part-task evaluation demonstrated in this paper is particularly valuable because it provides an efficient means of evaluating a range of display concepts before resources are invested in the more expensive and time-consuming evaluation approaches. report: Crash problem analysis. 1994, National Highway Traffic Safety Administration: Washington, DC. [6] Tijerina, L., Operational and behavioral issues in the comprehensive evaluation of lane change crash avoidance systems. Journal of Transportation Human Factors. 1(2) (1999) 159-176. [7] Wang, J.A., R.R. Knipling, and L.J. Blincoe, The dimensions of motor vehicle crash risk. Journal of Transportation and Statistics. 2(1) (1999) 19-44. [8] Rasmussen, J., A.M. Pejterson, and L.P. Goodstein, Cognitive systems engineering New York: John Wiley, 1994. Acknowledgments This research was supported by Nissan Motor Co., Ltd., with special thanks to Erwin Boer, project coordinator. References [1] Walker, G.H., N.A. Stanton, and M.S. Young, Where is computing driving cars? International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction. 13(2) (2001) 203-229. [2] Vicente, K.J., Cognitive work analysis: Towards safe, productive, and healthy computer-based work Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1999. [3] Vicente, K.J. and J. Rasmussen, Ecological interface design: Theoretical foundations. IEEE Transactions on Systems, Man, and Cybernetics. SCM-22(4) (1992) 589-606. [4] Vicente, K.J., Ecological interface design: Progress and challenges. Human Factors. 44(1) (2002) 62-78. [5] Eberhard, C.D., et al., Development of performance specifications for collision avoidance systems for lane change, merging and backing. Task 1 interim