Which Factors Affect Midline Crossing in Adult: Combined Effect of Task Complexity and Object Location

Similar documents
Line Bisection Judgments in Untreated and Undertreatment

Interlimb Transfer of Grasp Orientation is Asymmetrical

The Relationship between Mental Health and Internet Addiction

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Midline crossing movements: A teachable skill for developing children. Scott Pedersen University of Tasmania

Hill, Elisabeth L Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(1), pp ISSN [Article]

Pharmacophore COMPARING THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN FINE MOTOR SKILLS AND DEMOGRAPHIC INDEXES IN HEALTHY AND HARD OF HEARING ELEMENTARY STUDENTS

Comparison Effective Of Relaxation, Goal Setting And Breathing Methods On Decrease Sport Anxiety (Body And Mental)

HANDEDNESS, LIMB SELECTION, AND REACH CONTROL: A TEST OF THE DYNAMIC DOMINANCE HYPOTHESIS. A Dissertation WON DAE KIM

The Dutch Handedness Questionnaire

Effect of contextual interference on motor asymmetry of hand and bilateral transfer in basketball dribble

Long-Term Effects of Thalamic Deep Brain Stimulation on Force Control in a Patient with Parkinson s Disease-Driven Action Tremor

Upper limb asymmetries in the utilization of proprioceptive feedback

Intermanual differences on neuropsychological motor tasks in a Japanese university student sample 1

Lower Extremity Preference During Gross and Fine Motor Skills Performed in Sitting and -

Effect of Psycho-physical Stress on the Preference of Non- Athletic Youths for Intermittent or Continuous Bench- Stepping

Ralationship between perceived motivational climate and sport orientation with friendship quality scales in teenage volley ball players

Introductory Motor Learning and Development Lab

Electromyographic Comparison of Abdominal Muscle Activation during Sit-Up Exercise and Ab Crunch

The role of mental toughness in acquisition and retention of a sports skill

Improving Attention and Learning in Children & Adolescents: The Role of Working Memory

Effects of Kapalbhati on Peak Expiratory Flow Rate and Pulse Rate

Study of Meditational Role of Self-Esteem in the Relationship Between Perfectionism and Competitive Anxiety Elite Athletes

and the surrounding annuli. Effects on grasp scaling be-

Assessment of sexual function by DSFI among the Iranian married individuals

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen

Effect of Training Method on Serve Skill Improvement for Amateur Tennis Beginners

Definition and Description of Developmental Psychology

One hand or the other? Effector selection biases in right and left handers Main, J.C.; Carey, David

Available online at Pelagia Research Library. European Journal of Experimental Biology, 2014, 4(1):

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF)

Correspondence between the DRDP (2015) and the California Preschool Learning Foundations. Foundations (PLF)

Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and Autonomy

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

The Effectiveness of Problem-Solving Teaching with Two Methods of Group Games and Group Storytelling on the Slow-Learning Students' Social Skills

The Effect of Mental Imagery with Positive Thinking on Bilateral Transmission of Forehand and Backhand Skills in Table Tennis Players

UNIVERSITY OF WISCONSIN LA CROSSE. The ACCESS Center

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

The Effectiveness of Hypnotherapy to Increase Self-esteem of Patients Treated with Methadone

WHICH HANDEDNESS: PREFERENCE OR PERFORMANCE? 1

Lateralized hand-use is an easily observable sensorimotor skill that can be used as a

The Relationship of Mental Pressure with Optimism and Academic Achievement Motivation among Second Grade Male High School Students

Personality and Individual Differences

This is a refereed journal and all articles are professionally screened and reviewed

Solution Manual For Cognitive Psychology Connecting Mind Research and Everyday Experience 3rd Edition E Bruce Goldstein

The effect of perceptual-motor activities training on gross motor skills of autistic children

Cue Saliency and Age as Factors Affecting Performance in a Card Sorting Task

The Pennsylvania State University. The Graduate School. College of the Liberal Arts CONSTRAINT HIERARCHIES FOR HUMAN REACHING.

Human Paleoneurology and the Evolution of the Parietal Cortex

Male College Students Calculate the Density of the Body's Preferred Model Study

A Comparison of Emotional Intelligence in Elite Student Athletes and Non-Athletes

GENERAL GUIDELINES FOR PROVIDING DOCUMENTATION

Report. Responsibility Assignment in Redundant Systems

Case Study on Tele-Exercise for Calisthenics Using Consumer Internet Devices and a Software

Comparison of Simple and Choice Reaction Time in Tennis and Volleyball Players

A Comparative Study of Psychological Hardiness and Coping Strategies in Female Athlete and Non-Athlete Students

Human's Weight Perception from Container Handles

Innovative mechatronic system for testing laterality

Reliability. Internal Reliability

Lower Extremity Movement Preparation and Children With Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder

The Relationship between the Perception of Axes of Symmetry and Spatial Memory during Early Childhood

Left unilateral neglect or right hyperattention?

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

The effect of vestibular stimulation on dynamic balance in mentally retarded children

EFFECTIVENESS OF BEHAVIOR MANAGEMENT TRAINING TO MOTHERS ON HEALTH PHYSICAL OR COGNITIVE SKILLS OF CHILDREN WITH AUTISM IN TEHRAN CITY

The effectiveness of cognitive-behavioral play therapy on flexibility in aggressive children

Journal of Administrative Management, Education and Training (JAMET) Citation:

Comparison of Self- Efficacy in Athlete and Non- Athlete Male Students of Iran and India

Chapter 11. Experimental Design: One-Way Independent Samples Design

ADD / ADHD Verification Form To be completed by Psychiatrist/Psychologist/or Diagnosing Physician

INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF MANAGEMENT (IJM)

Dominant Limb Motor Impersistence Associated with Anterior Callosal Disconnection

The Relationship between Spiritual Well-Being and Academic Achievement

The Study of Relationship between Coping with Pain Experiences (Migraines) and Happiness

PREDICTION OF THE PERCEPTIONS OF SUCCESS AND GOAL ORIENTED STUDENTS ATHLETE BASED ON EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE AND FAMILY FUNCTIONING

Emotional Intelligence and Coaching Efficacy in Female Coaches

Visual & Auditory Skills Lab

Planning for Physical

Behavioural Brain Research

Fitness Assessment and Exercise Programs PEP Instructor: Dale Wagner, Ph.D. Semester: Fall 2014

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN LIFE SATISFACTION AND ATTACHMENT STYLES WITH PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING OF CHILDREN ON COVERED BY BEHZISTEY IN TEHRAN

Optimization of Computer presented Information for left-handed Observer

The relationship between students' studying habits, joyfulness and depression

Warm Up or Instant Activity: Time:

A comparison of the athletic participative motivation of the athletes of contact or noncontact sports

35-2 The Nervous System

The Evaluation of Scientific-Research Articles on Sports Management in Iranian Professional Journals

Repetitive behavior in toddlers may signal autism

Patterns of hand preference for pairs of actions and the classification of handedness. Marian Annett

Lateral Preference and Emotional Intelligence

TRANSFER EFFECTS OF EYE-HAND CO-ORDINATION SKILLS FROM THE RIGHT TO THE LEFT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERES IN SOUTH AFRICAN SCHOOLBOY RUGBY PLAYERS

Cognitive failures and metacognitive strategies of thought control in addicts and normal individuals

Group Counseling Interventions for Premenstrual Syndrome

Postural abnormalities in male and female students of Ferdowsi University Mashhad

The effect of little bird program in decreasing problem behaviors of autistic children

Relationship between personality and depression among High School Students in Tehran-Iran

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 237 ( 2017 )

P1: OTA/XYZ P2: ABC c01 BLBK231-Ginsberg December 23, :43 Printer Name: Yet to Come. Part 1. The Neurological Approach COPYRIGHTED MATERIAL

Assessing the Creativity and Its Obstacles based on the Individual Characteristics

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

Transcription:

ISSN 1750-9823 (print) International Journal of Sports Science and Engineering Vol. 04 (2010) No. 04, pp. 251-256 Which Factors Affect Midline Crossing in Adult: Combined Effect of Task Complexity and Object Location Meysam Rezaee 1,+, Masomeh Shojaei 2, Abdulah Ghasemi 3, Amir Moghadam 4 1 -Research and Science Branch of Islamic Azad University 2 -University of Alzahra, Tehran, Iran 3 -Research and Science Branch of Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran 4 -Mashhad Branch/ Islamic Azad University, Tehran, Iran (Received July 30, 2010, accepted September 9, 2010) Abstract. According to kinesthetic approach, motor system in order to increase mechanical efficiency prefers to response to the target with ipsilateral hand that this phenomena knows as hemispheric bias. However, accomplishing a preferred hand reach for contralateral object needs to cross the body midline that this is an inefficient act and also could be related with some learning disability. Different factors such as age and motor dominant influences the frequency of midline crossing (based on studies on samples of children and infants). So in order to investigate the effect of factor such as skill demand and objects location, the aim of current research was to study factors that affect midline crossing in adult. Method : in present semi experimental study,85 strong right handed (age18-25)as determined by annet handedness questionnaire were asked to reach 5 different objects at five different location and perform three different tasks(lift,pantomime and real use)with objects. On the other hand in present study (ANOVA) was used to analyze the data and alpha level of p<. /05 were used to determine significant. Result: 1- with increasing the task difficulty the frequency of preferred hand reaches and midline crossing were increased, so the least frequency of preferred hand reaches were made for lift task. But there was no significant difference between pantomime and actual use tasks. 2- The effect of object location showed that the least preferred hand reaches were made to the far left. Discussion: With respect to these findings, it is suggested that environmental factors as well as biological factor and some times more than them influence select of preferred hand and midline crossing. Keywords: object location, task complexity, midline crossing 1. Introduction: Human beings are required to use their hands for doing a lot of actions and using different objects (combing, brushing teeth, and writing), and, in order for any object to be used, it must be put in reaching. Therefore, it seems that reaching is an inseparable component of human actions (hand-related actions). However, people often prefer to do a lot of reaching actions and reach most of objects in their surrounding with preferred hand. Based on this we can conclude that repetition and frequency of reaching with preferred hand is much more than reaching with non-preferred hand (9, 14, 16, 24). However, according to some studies, people, at different ages, prefer to use their preferred hand for accessing to the most of objects in environment even objects in contra lateral hemispace that this phenomenon can result in crossing the body midline (4, 5). Based on Kinetic view, carrying out actions of each side of the body with ipsilateral hand is defined as hemisphere bias or kinetic efficiency. Hemisphere bias means that using ipsilateral hand for reaching can restrict the biomechanics constraints effects on movement, in addition, using contralateral hand in performing such actions requires longer trajectory and midline crossing which result in reduction in efficiency of the action (9). According to other studies, midline crossing also can be related to different variables such as learning disabilities (25, 26). Other features of this meaning, which have interested many growth specialists, drives from the relation between this concept and the developmental trend of preferred hand and also the differences in the amount of the midline crossing at different ages (2, 6, 7). For example, Carlier, Doyen, Lamard (2006), conducted a study on children between the ages of 3 to 10, concluded that older children perform midline crossing more frequently in comparison to younger children (7). Pryde, + Corresponding author. PHD student. Tel: +989157062015 Email address: mysm_rez@yahoo.com. Published by World Academic Press, World Academic Union

252 Meysam Rezaee, et al: Which Factors Affect Midline Crossing in Adult Bryden, Roy, (2000)also studied the usage of hand among people between the ages of 3 to 24, and according to the results, children between 3 to 4 and adults used preferred hand in the contralateral hemispace less frequently, which shows a reduction in body midline crossing. On the other hand, children between the ages of 6 to 10 showed high tendency in using their preferred hand in all parts of the hemispace; this confirms the concept of growth process and the effect of the age on the midline crossing (4). Another factor, which has been focused in some researches, is the variable of motor dominancy or handedness (22). Indeed, based on searches done, many of studies have focused on the effect of dominant limb among the group ages of children and infants (2, 6, 8, 17, 27). Based on the recent studies, especially studies on children, the two new concepts, the attentional need related to the object locations and task complexity can affect the hand selection in the contralateral hemispace (12,18,19,23). Now, the question is that whether just the effect of these factors is restricted to the childhood; is the significance sequence of these variables the same for all age periods. considering different features of children and adults in the process of reaching and hand selection, and in view of the limited background of studies about these factors in the researches about adults comparing to children, this research is trying to study the effects of task complexity and object location on the frequency of body midline crossing among adults. 2. Method: 2.1. Participants: This research statistical universe consisted physical education students of Mashhad University. Following performing, Aneet handedness questionnaire (Aneet 1970), 85 female and male subjects (39 female and 46 male) were selected among qualified people (strong right handed and healthy, age range= 18 to 25) as statistical sample (1). 2.2. Procedure and task: Implementation method and tools employed in this research were extracted from Mamlo et al, (2004) (21). Five tools were placed in five positions with 45 intervals on a half circle with diameter 30cm. position 1 was in far right, position 2 in near right, position 3 in midline and positions 4 and 5 in near left and far left. Used tools included pencil, paintbrush, small knife, small plastic saw, and a small hammer. According Waterloo s handedness questionnaire these tools require most recalling of preferred hand. Each participant was seated at a table in front of the experimental apparatus with his/her hands resting on the table. Then experimenter randomly asks subjects to accomplish one of following tasks with objects: 1.simple lifting of objects, 2. pantomime the use of the tool (first lifting the object and then its Pantomime 3. Real use (first lifting and then performing the action). In this test, subjects were asked to perform respective action naturally and quickly. In this case, experimenter stands before subject and records data related to each effort. Efforts related to each tool and actions were recorded in combined form and randomly in a 15-effort set format. After finishing the first block, tools location is changed so that each tools is placed in a specified position. At the end, each subject should accomplish 75 efforts. 2.3. Statistical analyses: Regarding the fact that each variable has different aspects (task =3, location = 5), research design is inter-group design and multifactor variance analysis (3.5) with repeated measures was used for variable study. Furthermore, Different tests such as, ANOVA, and MANOVA were used for determine the effect of each variable and interaction of them on frequency of preferred hand use. 3. Results: Based on the results of this study, the frequency of reaching with preferred hand in near right and far right point was at the maximum. Of course, only one participant of reaching with the left hand for the far right spot were observed, and considering the near-zero variance in far right and near right spots, and in view of the concept of midline crossing, the information for three spots of midline, near left and far left were used. Based on findings of this research the task effect was significant on frequency of using preferred hand (p=0.000, F=23.351); that is, by increasing level of task difficulty, preferred hand recalling also increases. According to statistical results, most use of preferred hand was observed in real use task (M= 24.99) and then in pantomime action (M = 24.89) and finally in simple lifting (M = 23.78). regarding Tukey s follow up test, SSci email for contribution: editor@ssci.org.uk

International Journal of Sports Science and Engineering, 4 (2010) 4, pp 251-256 253 it was found that there is significant difference between a mean frequency of preferred hand reaches in lift task and two other tasks (pantomime and real use) (p =.000), while this difference was not observed in pantomime and real use tasks (p =.873). According to results, object location has also impact on frequency of reaching with preferred hand (F=17.113 p=.000). Thus after position 1&2 in ipsilateral hemispace more reaching was observed with preferred hand in midline position (M = 14.91) compared to near (M = 14.68) and far left position (M = 14.12). However, using Tukey s follow up test didn t show differences between far and near right, midline and near left position. Then it was found that reaching frequency mean with preferred hand in far left position is significantly less than other positions (p =.000). Figure1. The mean frequency of preferred hand reaches at each position for right-handed participants. However, regarding combination and interaction of task and object location variables it was found that there is significant difference between different tasks in different positions (p =.000, F =.059), it also was found that mean frequency of using preferred hand in far left position for lift task (M = 4.2) has significant difference with two other tasks (pantomime mean=4.929 & real use mean=4.988) (p = 0.000). Figure2. The mean frequency of preferred hand reaches at each position for right-handed participants. The frequency of preferred hand reaches was summed across the five tools to give a maximum possible score. SSci email for subscription: publishing@wau.org.uk

254 4. Discussion: Meysam Rezaee, et al: Which Factors Affect Midline Crossing in Adult This study aims to evaluate factors affecting midline crossing and, in order for that to happen, variables affecting hand selection in working space, especially factors affecting hand selection in contralateral hemispace were studied. This research evaluated the interaction between the factors associated with attentional features related to the objects location and task demand. Based on the results, the variable related to the location of objects had impact on the frequency of reaching with preferred hand in adults. Thus with regard to the fact that almost all ipsilateral reaching efforts were performed with preferred hand, this amount changed significantly in contralateral hemispace so that frequency of reaching with preferred hand in far left position reduced significantly compared to other positions, especially positions related to ipsilateral hemispace and midline spot. These findings is consistent with the observations of Gabbard, Helbig (2004), Gabbard, Helbig, Gentry (2001), Gabbard, Rabb(2000), Mamolo, Roy, Bryden, Rohr (2006) (10, 11, 13, 20). On the other hand, with regard to effect of task demand, frequency of using preferred hand in different tasks changed significantly. This concept result to this that people prefer to cross body midline for performing pantomime actions and real use in contralateral hemispace with respect to biomechanical non efficiency of these movements. This indicates increase of recalling preferred hand regarding increase of task difficulty level. Based on review of related literature these findings are consistent with (20, 21), but it is not concur with observations (5) showing no effect of this variable on limb selecting, and observations (12) showing the effect of hemisphere bias on limb selecting in contralateral hemispace, or, in other words, the tendency of the body for doing actions with ipsilateral hand. Based on these findings, and considering their resemblance to the results of the studies on children, it is suggested that the variables of task demand and the objects location are effective among the groups of adults, as for children, and adults, consider this variables for hand selecting in different region, even contralateral hemispace same as children. However, at a closer look, this question is raised: is the amount of preference of each of these factors the same among adults and children; in other words, do adults and children react in the same way to all existing variables. Based on Bryden and Roy (2000), children at the ages of 3 and 4, same as adults, use their preferred hand in all spaces less frequently, comparing to the children between the ages of 6 to 10 (4). This concept can be explained this way: children move from accidental reaching with their hands at earlier ages toward fixed reaching with their preferred hand in all spaces (3). Of course, this much usage could explain by much practice and using preferred hand during the education period (11). It seems that the sequence of the effects of the variables differs among groups of adults and children, meaning that children at the ages of 6 to 10 use less flexible strategies for movements and it is because they are dealing with the process of movements refining (15). finally, based on the concepts mentioned in this study, it can be suggested that different factors can affect the hand selecting in contralateral hemispace or on midline crossing, on the other hand, based on dynamic system view, it seems that all environmental, biological factors and task demand, can affect hand selection in children and adults. However, in each age period the prominence of each of these factors changes. This way, it may be suggested that during the childhood, individual features or motor dominance is the most important factor for hand selecting in contralateral hemispace, and in adulthood attentional features of the environment and task demand replace motor dominance. 5. References [1] Annett M. The classification of hand preference by association analysis. Br J Psychol. 1970, 61(3): 303-21. [2] Bradshaw JL, Spataro JA, Harris M, Nettleton NC, Bradshaw J. Crossing the midline by four to eight year old children. Neuropsychologia. 1988, 26(2): 221-35. [3] Bishop, D. V. M. Handedness and developmental disorders. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific and Hove. Erlbaum 1990. [4] Bryden PJ, Pryde KM, Roy EA. A performance measure of the degree of hand preference. Brain Cogn. 2000, 44(3): 402-14. [5] Bryden PJ, Roy EA. Preferential reaching across regions of hemispace in adults and children. Dev Psychobiol. 2006, 48(2):121-32. [6] Cermak SA, Quintero EJ, Cohen PM. Developmental age trends in crossing the body midline in normal children. Am J Occup Ther. 1980, 34(5): 313-9 [7] Carlier M, Doyen AL, Lamard C. Midline crossing: developmental trend from 3 to 10 years of age in a preferential card-reaching task. Brain Cogn. 2006, 61(3): 255-61. SSci email for contribution: editor@ssci.org.uk

International Journal of Sports Science and Engineering, 4 (2010) 4, pp 251-256 255 [8] Karen S. Cornwell; Lauren Julius Harris; Hiram E. Fitzgerald. Task effects in the development of hand preference in 9-, 13-, and 20-month-old infant girls. Developmental Neuropsychology. 1991, pp.19-34 [9] Gabbard C, Iteya M, Rabb C. A lateralized comparison of handedness and object proximity. Can J Exp Psychol. 1997, 51(2):176-80. [10] Gabbard C, Helbig CR. What drives children's limb selection for reaching in hemispace? Exp Brain Res. 2004, 156(3): 325-32. [11] Gabbard C, Helbig CR, Gentry V. Lateralized effects on reaching by children. Dev Neuropsychol. 2001, 19(1): 41-51. [12] Gabbard C, Rabb C, Gentry V. Attentional stimuli and programming hand selection: a developmental perspective. Int J Neurosci. 1998, 96(3-4): 205-15. [13] Gabbard C, Rabb C. What determines choice of limb for unimanual reaching movements? J Gen Psychol. 2000, 127(2): 178-84. [14] Gabbard C, Tapia M, Helbig CR. Task complexity and limb selection in reaching. Int J Neurosci. 2003, 113(2): 143-52. [15] Haywood, K., & Getchell, N. Life span motor development. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics, 2001. [16] Helbig CR, Gabbard C. What determines limb selection for reaching? Res Q Exerc Sport. 2004, 75(1): 47-59. [17] Hinojosa T, Sheu CF, Michel GF. Infant hand-use preferences for grasping objects contributes to the development of a hand-use preference for manipulating objects. Dev Psychobiol. 2003, 43(4): 328-34. [18] Leconte P, Fagard J. Influence of object spatial location and task complexity on children's use of their preferred hand depending on their handedness consistency. Dev Psychobiol. 2004, 45(2): 51-8. [19] Leconte P, Fagard J. Which factors affect hand selection in children's grasping in hemispace? Combined effects of task demand and motor dominance. Brain Cogn. 2006, 60(1): 88-93. [20] Mamolo CM, Roy EA, Rohr LE, Bryden PJ. Reaching patterns across working space: the effects of handedness, task demands, and comfort levels. Laterality. 2006, 11(5): 465-92. [21] Mamolo CM, Roy EA, Bryden PJ, Rohr LE. The effects of skill demands and object position on the distribution of preferred hand reaches. Brain Cogn. 2004, 55(2): 349-51. [22] Mamolo CM, Roy EA, Bryden PJ, Rohr LE. The performance of left-handed participants on a preferential reaching test. Brain Cogn. 2005, 57(2): 143-5. [23] Marschik PB, Einspieler C, Strohmeier A, Plienegger J, Garzarolli B, Prechtl HF. From the reaching behavior at 5 months of age to hand preference at preschool age. Dev Psychobiol. 2008, 50(5): 511-8. [24] Pryde KM, Bryden PJ, Roy EA. A developmental analysis of the relationship between hand preference and performance: I. Preferential reaching into hemispace. Brain Cogn. 2000, 43(1-3): 370-4. [25] Screws DP, Eason BL, Surburg PR. Crossing the midline: a study of four-year-old children. Percept Mot Skills. 1998, 86(1): 201-3. [26] Stilwell JM. Relationship between development of the body-righting reaction and manual midline crossing behavior in the learning disabled. Am J Occup Ther. 1981, 35(6): 391-8. [27] Stilwell JM. The development of manual midline crossing in 2- to 6-year-old children. Am J Occup Ther. 1987, 41(12): 783-9. SSci email for subscription: publishing@wau.org.uk

256 Meysam Rezaee, et al: Which Factors Affect Midline Crossing in Adult SSci email for contribution: editor@ssci.org.uk