Mental representation of number in different numerical forms

Similar documents
Mental Representation of Number in Different Numerical Forms

The overlap of neural selectivity between faces and words: evidences

Neural correlates of short-term perceptual learning in orientation discrimination indexed by event-related potentials

Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5-year-old children and adults

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Title of Thesis. Study on Audiovisual Integration in Young and Elderly Adults by Event-Related Potential

Neural Strategies for Selective Attention Distinguish Fast-Action Video Game Players

Outline of Talk. Introduction to EEG and Event Related Potentials. Key points. My path to EEG

Neural Correlates of Complex Tone Processing and Hemispheric Asymmetry

Asymmetry between the upper and lower visual fields: An event-related potential study

Description of the Spectro-temporal unfolding of temporal orienting of attention.

Tracking the Development of Automaticity in Memory Search with Human Electrophysiology

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

In the first section, Introduction, we present our experimental design.

The attentional selection of spatial and non-spatial attributes in touch: ERP evidence for parallel and independent processes

ERP Studies of Selective Attention to Nonspatial Features

DATA MANAGEMENT & TYPES OF ANALYSES OFTEN USED. Dennis L. Molfese University of Nebraska - Lincoln

Electrophysiological Indices of Target and Distractor Processing in Visual Search

NeuroImage 50 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Profiling Attention s Pure Effect on the Sensory-Evoked P1 and N1 Event-Related Potentials of Human Electroencephalography

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

Material-speci c neural correlates of memory retrieval

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Beyond Blind Averaging: Analyzing Event-Related Brain Dynamics. Scott Makeig. sccn.ucsd.edu

The Neural Basis of Somatosensory Remapping Develops in Human Infancy

Dissociable neural correlates for familiarity and recollection during the encoding and retrieval of pictures

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Language Speech. Speech is the preferred modality for language.

This presentation is the intellectual property of the author. Contact them for permission to reprint and/or distribute.

Introduction to Electrophysiology

EEG History. Where and why is EEG used? 8/2/2010

Supplemental Information. Direct Electrical Stimulation in the Human Brain. Disrupts Melody Processing

The impact of numeration on visual attention during a psychophysical task; An ERP study

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Early posterior ERP components do not reflect the control of attentional shifts toward expected peripheral events


An ERP Examination of the Different Effects of Sleep Deprivation on Exogenously Cued and Endogenously Cued Attention

Dual Mechanisms for the Cross-Sensory Spread of Attention: How Much Do Learned Associations Matter?

AUTOCORRELATION AND CROSS-CORRELARION ANALYSES OF ALPHA WAVES IN RELATION TO SUBJECTIVE PREFERENCE OF A FLICKERING LIGHT

Independence of Visual Awareness from the Scope of Attention: an Electrophysiological Study

Identifying the ERP correlate of a recognition memory search attempt

Electrophysiological evidence of two different types of error in the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Cognitive Control Mechanisms Revealed by ERP and fmri: Evidence from Repeated Task-Switching

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

Do P1 and N1 evoked by the ERP task reflect primary visual processing in Parkinson s disease?

Report. Spatial Attention Can Be Allocated Rapidly and in Parallel to New Visual Objects. Martin Eimer 1, * and Anna Grubert 1 1

An Introduction to Translational Neuroscience Approaches to Investigating Autism

Supplementary Information on TMS/hd-EEG recordings: acquisition and preprocessing

The face-sensitive N170 encodes social category information Jonathan B. Freeman a, Nalini Ambady a and Phillip J. Holcomb a

Temporal attention enhances early visual processing: A review and new evidence from event-related potentials

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

For Non-Commercial Academic Use Only

ABSTRACT 1. INTRODUCTION 2. ARTIFACT REJECTION ON RAW DATA

Accepted Manuscript. Title: Sensory and cognitive processes of shifts of spatial attention induced by numbers: an ERPs study

Cortical source analysis of infant spatial cueing International Conference on Infant Studies, 2012 John E. Richards University of South Carolina

Neural basis of auditory-induced shifts in visual time-order perception

Control processes in verbal working memory: An event-related potential study

Visual Evoked Potentials and Event Related Potentials in Congenitally Deaf Subjects

Event-Related Potentials Recorded during Human-Computer Interaction

Timing and Sequence of Brain Activity in Top-Down Control of Visual-Spatial Attention

Increased Synchronization of Neuromagnetic Responses during Conscious Perception

HST 583 fmri DATA ANALYSIS AND ACQUISITION

On the role of response conflicts and stimulus position for hemispheric differences in global/local processing: an ERP study

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

Manuscript under review for Psychological Science. Direct Electrophysiological Measurement of Attentional Templates in Visual Working Memory

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

The speed of voluntary and priority-driven shifts of visual attention. *Corresponding Author. Malet Street, London, WC1E 7HX, UK

Supporting Information

Activation of brain mechanisms of attention switching as a function of auditory frequency change

Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection: intracranial human recordings

Implicit memory influences the allocation of attention in visual cortex

The neurolinguistic toolbox Jonathan R. Brennan. Introduction to Neurolinguistics, LSA2017 1

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Supplementary material

Supplementary materials for: Executive control processes underlying multi- item working memory

The role of top-down spatial attention in contingent attentional capture

Biomedical Research 2013; 24 (3): ISSN X

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Event-Related Potential Indices of Attentional Gradients Across the Visual Field

Evidence for Multiple Representations of Number in the Human Brain. Thesis. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for

Seeing faces in the noise: Stochastic activity in perceptual regions of the brain may influence the perception of ambiguous stimuli

Dissociation of Category-Learning Mechanisms via Brain Potentials

EFFECT OF COHERENT MOVING STIMULUS ON THE VISUAL EVOKED POTENTIAL (CMVEP) ABSTRACT

Accounts for the N170 face-effect: a reply to Rossion, Curran, & Gauthier

Gender difference in N170 elicited under oddball task

An Overview of BMIs. Luca Rossini. Workshop on Brain Machine Interfaces for Space Applications

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Generating nonsymbolic number stimuli

Running Head: VISUAL WORKING MEMORY LOAD AND ATTENTIONAL CONTROL

ERP correlates of retrieval orientation: Direct versus indirect memory tasks

Selective Attention to Face Identity and Color Studied With fmri

Active suppression after involuntary capture of attention

Action Preparation Helps and Hinders Perception of Action

WAVELET ENERGY DISTRIBUTIONS OF P300 EVENT-RELATED POTENTIALS FOR WORKING MEMORY PERFORMANCE IN CHILDREN

Activation of brain attention systems in individuals with symptoms of ADHD

Transcription:

Submitted to Current Biology Mental representation of number in different numerical forms Anna Plodowski, Rachel Swainson, Georgina M. Jackson, Chris Rorden and Stephen R. Jackson School of Psychology The University of Nottingham University Park Nottingham NG7 2RD UK Correspondence to: Professor SR Jackson School of Psychology The University of Nottingham University Park Nottingham NG7 2RD Fax: 0115 951 5324 Email: stephen.jackson@nottingham.ac.uk

Critical issues for understanding how numerical operations are implemented within the human brain are to determine the cognitive processes associated with manipulating different forms of numerical notation and to identify the neural substrates associated with each numerical form. It has been suggested that there are at least three different codes for representing number [1]: A verbal code that is used to manipulate number words and perform mental numerical operations (e.g., multiplication); a visual code which is used to decode frequently used visual number forms (e.g., Arabic digits); and, an abstract, analogue, code which may be used to represent numerical quantities. Furthermore, each of these codes is associated with a different neural substrate [1-3]. We extend these studies using dense-sensor eventrelated EEG recording techniques to investigate the temporal pattern of notationspecific effects observed in a parity judgement (odd vs. even) task in which single numbers were presented in one of four different numerical notations (English number words, Arabic numerals, Roman numerals, and random dot configurations). Contrasts between different notations demonstrated clear modulations in the amplitude and latency of the visual evoked potentials (VEP) recorded. We observed increased amplitudes for the P1 and N1 components of the VEP that were specific to Arabic numerals and to dot configurations, but differed for random and recognisable (die-face) dot configurations. These results demonstrate clear, notation-specific, differences in the time course of numerical information processing and provide electrophysiological support for the triple code model of numerical representation. - 2 -

Results and Discussion Several features of number are of interest to cognitive neuroscientists. First, investigations of animals and infants indicate that the ability to process numerical magnitude can be independent of language. Second, identical numerical quantities can be represented in several different notations. Third, different numerical operations can be performed on the same operands. Dehaene [1] has proposed a triple code model that distinguishes between an auditory verbal code, a visual code for Arabic digits, and an analogue magnitude code that represents numerical quantities as variable distributions of brain activation. Dehaene and colleagues [1-3] propose that there are specific relationships between individual numerical operations and different numerical codes. The analogue magnitude code is used for magnitude comparison and approximate calculation, the visual Arabic number form for parity judgements and multi-digit operations, and the auditory verbal code for arithmetical facts learned by rote (e.g. addition and multiplication tables). Previous studies have used behavioural and neuroimaging techniques (both ERP and fmri) to explore the effects of notation (i.e., Arabic vs. verbal code) on magnitude estimation [2,3]. Here we extend these studies by using dense-sensor ERP recording techniques to compare a wider range of numerical codes across different forms of numerical operation (e.g., magnitude estimation and parity judgement). FIGURE 1 ABOUT HERE - 3 -

Figure 1A contains a graphical representation of the behavioural task used within this study. RT data for correct trials were entered into a repeated-measures ANOVA with two factors, numeric notation (English words, Arabic numerals, Roman numerals, and, Dot configurations) and numeric value (1-8). Error rates were examined but were too small to merit report. Figure 1B shows mean response times (RT) for each numerical value within each numerical format. Inspection of this figure indicates that mean RT was similar for common numeric forms (English words and Arabic digits) but was substantially slowed for the more unfamiliar representations (Roman numerals and random dot configurations). The ANOVA revealed significant main effects of numeric form (F[3,39] = 263.1, p < 0.0001) and numeric value (F[7,91] = 103.4, p < 0.0001). Planned comparisons (linear contrasts) between appropriate numeric forms revealed that mean RT for English words did not differ from that for Arabic numerals (p = 0.25). However, mean RT was significantly slower for Roman numerals and dot configurations compared to both English words and Arabic numerals (minimum F[1,13] = 41.3, p < 0.0001). Inspection of Figure 1 also suggests that, for Roman numerals and random dot configurations only, mean RT differed for numerical values greater than three. This was confirmed by statistical analyses as the ANOVA demonstrated a significant numerical form x numerical value interaction effect (F[21,273] = 89.4, p < 0.0001). In contrast, mean RT for English words and for Arabic digits were not affected by changes in numerical value (maximum F[1,13] = 0.1, p = 0.5). This suggests that values presented in common numerical formats may be recognised directly; along with numerical values in less familiar formats that can be subitised (values 1-3), or, individual values in less familiar formats that are particularly familiar (e.g., the Roman numeral V ). - 4 -

FIGURE 2 ABOUT HERE The EEG data was analysed in two ways. First, data were average referenced and baseline corrected to produce, grand-averaged, event-related brain potentials (ERPs) for each form of numerical notation (Figure 2a). Grand averaged visual evoked potentials were averaged over twelve sensor locations previously shown to yield a significant numerical distance effect on a magnitude estimation task [8]. A priori comparisons were then made between different notations, and t-tests carried out between appropriate numerical notations at each time sample. The method proposed in [4] was used to determine threshold for statistical significance for consecutive samples. The second method was Current Source Density (CSD) analysis that provides an evaluation of the topography of current sources and sinks on the scalp, where CSD values are proportional to the current entering and exiting the scalp. CSD is sensitive to high spatial frequency local cortical potentials but less sensitive to low spatial frequency signals arising from distant sources [5]. For this reason, CSD analyses can be thought of as providing an estimate of local sources (tangential and radial) originating within neural generators situated relatively close to the scalp electrode. To examine the influence of the different forms of numerical notation on the time course of information processing, the following a priori comparisons of ERPs were made: all notations were compared against the English word form, all notations were compared against the Arabic numeral form, and all notations against the dots form. Figure 2a - 5 -

shows that the amplitude of the P1 component of the VEP is significantly greater for Arabic digits compared to all other numerical forms. This was tested in a set of planned comparisons (Figure 2b), which revealed that ERP differences between Arabic digits and other conditions first reached statistical significance at 108ms. This difference was maintained for at least 4 consecutive samples [4] for English words (72ms), Roman numerals (80ms) and dots (32ms). These data support Dehaene s proposal for a separate visual code for Arabic digits, and illustrate that the P1 component of the VEP may be an electrophysiological marker for this mechanism. Figure 2c illustrates current source density (CSD) topographical plots for key notations at the time of the average peak of the P1 component (indicated by the broken vertical line in Figure 2a). Inspection of Figure 2c reveals bilateral current sources over the inferior occipito-temporal scalp for Arabic digit forms. These bilateral sources were less apparent for other numerical formats. This finding is consistent with Dehaene's proposal that visual identification of Arabic digits depends upon bilateral regions of the occipito-temporal cortex [1] and the finding that the fusiform gyrus responds selectively to Arabic digits [6]. It is also consistent with the proposal that the visual P1 represents early visual processing in the pathway from occipital to inferior temporal cortex [7]. FIGURE 3 ABOUT HERE Figure 3a shows that the amplitude of the N1 component of the VEP is significantly increased and also delayed for random dot configurations compared to all other numerical - 6 -

forms. This was tested in a set of planned comparisons (Figure 3b). These a priori comparisons revealed that ERP differences between dot configurations and English words or Arabic digits first reached significance 176ms after stimulus onset. This difference was maintained for at least 4 consecutive samples for English words v dots (124ms) and Arabic digits v dots (124ms). It should be noted that the above effect was not observed in a previous ERP study in which dot configurations were presented [6]. However, there were two key differences between that study and our own. Firstly, Temple and Posner [8] used familiar dot stimuli (die face) while we made use of random dot configurations and it is likely that the processing of familiar dot configurations draws upon similar visual mechanisms to those used to analyse other highly familiar visual number forms. In contrast, processing random dot configurations may activate the abstract magnitude estimation or 'number sense' mechanisms proposed by Dehaene [1], and associated with bilateral regions of posterior parietal cortex. If this is the case then our data indicate that the N1 component of the VEP, which has been associated with the occipito-parietal stream of visual processing [7], may provide an electrophysiological marker for these mechanisms. Secondly, Temple and Posner used a magnitude estimation task whereas we made use of a parity judgement task. An important difference between these tasks is that parity judgements require an exact representation of numerosity. In contrast, an exact representation of numerosity is not required in order to accurately estimate relative magnitude. - 7 -

An alternative interpretation of the N1 effect that we observed might be that our enhanced N1 amplitude reflects a counting strategy in the parity judgement task that is not be required for familiar stimuli or when performing the magnitude estimation task. To further investigate this issue we ran two further experiments in which we directly compared die-face and random dot displays for both parity judgement and magnitude estimation tasks (Experiments 4 and 5). Figure 4 shows mean RTs for subitisable (1-3) and non-subitisable (4-9) stimuli for each numeric form of representation for parity judgement and magnitude estimation tasks. Inspection of this figure shows clearly that for the parity judgement task mean RTs for subitisable (1-3) and non-subitisable (4-8) stimuli were not significantly different from one another for English words or for Arabic numerals (p > 0.1). By contrast, mean RTs for non-subitisable stimuli were significantly longer than for subitisable stimuli for both die face (p < 0.05) and random dot (p < 0.05) configurations. Moreover, for non-subitisable stimuli only, there was a significant increase in mean reaction time for random dot stimuli compared to die face stimuli (p < 0.05). Note, this difference was not statistically significant for subitisable stimuli (p > 0.1). These behavioural data, together with other evidence [9], indicate that counting may be necessary to perform the parity judgement task when presented with unfamiliar stimuli (e.g., random dot configurations) or stimuli that cannot be easily subitised. FIGURE 4 ABOUT HERE Inspection of Figure 4 also reveals that the pattern observed for the parity judgement task with respect to die face and random dot stimuli was not seen for the magnitude estimation task. Specifically, for both subitisable and non-subitisable stimuli there was no - 8 -

significant difference in mean RT between die face and random dot stimuli (p > 0.1). This confirms that the magnitude estimation task and the parity judgement task make quite different demands upon subjects, with only the latter requiring a counting strategy for unfamiliar or non-subitisable dot configurations. FIGURE 5 ABOUT HERE A remaining question concerns the interpretation of the increased amplitude of the N1 VEP. Specifically, does the does N1 amplitude reflect counting? To directly investigate this we compared N1 amplitudes separately random dot configurations for each numerical value (i.e., 1-9) [parity judgement task, Experiment 4]. VEPs for random dot configurations for numerical values 1-4 are presented in Figure 5. Planned comparisons for each numerical value revealed statistically significant differences between 1 and 2 dots (p < 0.05) and between 2 and 3 dots (p < 0.05), but thereafter no significant difference between adjacent numbers of dots were observed (p > 0.1). These data clearly confirm that the N1 VEP is not indexing a counting strategy. Instead it appears that the N1 VEP indexes a rather crude form of stimulus complexity which distinguishes between a single stimulus, two stimuli, and configurations of more than two stimuli. In conclusion, previous studies have used ERP and fmri techniques to examine the effect of different numerical forms, notably arabic and verbal codes, on magnitude estimation. We extend these studies by using ERP recording techniques to investigate the temporal pattern of notation-specific effects observed in magnitude estimation and parity - 9 -

judgement tasks over a wide range of numerical forms. We demonstrate clear modulations in the amplitude and latency of the visual evoked potentials. Specifically, an increased amplitude for the P1 and N1 components of the VEP, that are specific to Arabic digits and to dot configurations, and amplitude differences between Arabic digits & dot configurations compared to English words & Roman numerals. We also find clear evidence of amplitude differences in the processing of 1-3 dots compared to 4-8 dots, further clarifying the boundary of subitisation. Our results demonstrate clear, notationspecific, differences in the time course of numerical information processing and provide electrophysiological support for Dehaene s triple code model of numerical representation [1]. Materials and Methods Participants Twenty-two neurologically normal right-handed native English speakers (age: 15 to 36 years) participated in the experiment. Participants gave informed consent and were paid for their participation. Data from eight participants had to be excluded from analysis after artefact rejection (see ERP analyses). Procedure and apparatus Participants viewed visual stimuli projected onto a screen from a distance of 2.2 metres. Stimuli consisted of a number in the numerical range from one to eight, presented in one - 10 -

of four numerical notations: English words, Arabic digits, Roman numerals or random dot configurations (Figure 1A). Participants were instructed to press one of two buttons to indicate whether the stimulus was an odd or even number. The assignment of odd and even to the left or right button was counterbalanced across participants. Stimuli were presented in yellow on a blue background. Alphanumeric-based stimuli were presented in Helvetica with the font size being varied to maintain as similar a degree of visual angle (3 vertically and between 2-12 horizontally) formats as possible (font sizes were: English words 80; Arabic numerals 120; Roman numerals 100). English words were presented in lower case, and Roman numerals in upper case. Dot stimuli consisted of one to eight dots, presented in a trial-specific random configuration about the centre of the screen. Each dot had a diameter of 12 pixels, and was no more than 60 pixels from the centre of the screen, with a minimum distance of 30 pixels between each dot and subtended a visual angle of about 5. Stimuli were presented until 500ms after a response was recorded. The inter-stimulus interval (ISI) was varied randomly between 1500 and 2500ms after the response. Sixteen practice trials in each format were performed in single format blocks before the start of the experiment. Experimental trials were delivered in eight blocks of 64 trials (a total of 128 trials in each format), pseudo-randomised across numerical size and format so that there was no repetition of numerical size or format on consecutive trials. ERP analyses High-density event-related electrical potentials (ERPs) were recorded from each participant using a 128-channel geodesic sensor net coupled to a high input impedance - 11 -

amplifier. EEG was continuously recorded and digitised at 250Hz and vertex referenced. Impedances were reduced to 50KΩ prior to recording. Where this level could not be attained by adjusting or re-wetting the sensor with electrolyte solution, and where this led to noisy recordings, the sensor was excluded before analysis. The continuous EEG was segmented into one-second epochs time-locked to the onset of each visual stimulus, commencing 100ms prior to stimulus onset. ERPs were baseline-corrected with respect to a 100ms pre-stimulus interval, digitally low-pass filtered at 45 Hz, and average referenced. Trials not meeting behavioural criteria (i.e., responses that were incorrect, absent, used both keys or occurred less than 200msec or more than 8000msec after stimulus onset) were rejected from analysis. Trials were also rejected from analysis if they contained eye movement artefacts (i.e. an EOG channel difference greater than 70µV) or more than ten bad channels (channels with voltage amplitudes over 200µV or a change in amplitude between adjacent samples of more than 100µV). Channels that were bad for more than 25% of trials for a given participant were excluded from that participant s data. The sensors selected for analysis (left hemisphere 67, 66, 60, 65, 71, 70; right hemisphere 78, 85, 86, 91, 84, 90) were those previously found to show a distance effect on a magnitude estimation task using Arabic digits and die-face dots [8]. Grand average waveforms, collapsed across these twelve sensors, were plotted for all four conditions. Individual t-tests were carried out on the waveforms for these comparisons, both to test for significant differences in electrical activity, and to indicate the latency of any such differences. References - 12 -

1. Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition 44, 1-42. 2. Dehaene, S. (1996). Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 8, 47-68. 3. Pinel, P., Dehaene, S., Riviere D. and LeBihan D. (2001). NeuroImage 14, 1013-1026. 4. Guthrie, D., Buchwald, J. S. (1991) Significance Testing of Difference Potentials. Psychphysiology 28, 240-244. 5. Nunez, P. L., Silberstein, R. B., Cadusch, P. J., Wijesinghe, R. S., Westdorp, A. F., and Srinivasan, R. (1994). A theoretical and experimental study of high resolution EEG based on surface Laplacians and cortical imaging. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 90,40 57. 6. Allison, T., McCarthy, G., Nobre, A., Puce, A., and Belger, A. (1994). Human extrastriate visual cortex and the perception of faces, words, numbers and colors. Cerebral Cortex 5, 544-554. 7. Mangun, G. R. and Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Electrophysiological studies of visual selective attention in humans. In The neurobiological foundations of higher cognitive function (Ed. A. B. Scheibel and A. Wechsler). Guilford, New York. 8. Temple, E. and Posner, M. I. (1998). Brain mechanisms of quantity are similar in 5- year-old children and adults. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 95, 7836-7841. 9. van Oeffelen, M. P. and Vos, P. G. (1982) A Probabilistic Model for the Discrimination of Visual Number Perception & Psychophysics 32 (2) 163-170. - 13 -

Figure 1. Behavioural task and mean RT data a. Graphical representation of the behavioural task. Participants made parity judgments (odd vs. even) to number stimuli (1-8) presented in one of four different numerical formats. b. Mean response time data for each numerical value within each numerical form. Error bars represent standard errors. - 14 -

Figure 2. Analyses of P1 component of the VEP a. Grand average showing the time course of the VEP for each numerical form illustrating an increased P1 amplitude for Arabic digits. The vertical grey bars indicate the segment of the epoch within which statistical analyses were carried out. The vertical broken line indicates the point in time at which CSD topographical plots were calculated. b. Pairwise comparisons between Arabic digits (solid black line) and other numerical notations (solid grey line). Areas of grey connecting the two lines represent time samples on which the difference between the two conditions was statistically significant (t-test). c. Current source density (CSD) plots showing the topography of current sources and sinks. Lighter regions reflect current sources and darker region current sinks. Contour lines represent 1mn graduations. - 15 -

Figure 3. Analyses of N1 component of the VEP a. Grand average showing the time course of the VEP for each numerical form illustrating an increased N1 amplitude for random dot configurations. The vertical grey bars indicate the segment of the epoch within which statistical analyses were carried out and the vertical broken line indicates the point in time at which CSD topographical plots were calculated. b. Pairwise comparisons between Arabic digits or English words (solid black lines) and random dot configurations (solid grey lines). Areas of grey connecting the two lines represent time samples on which the difference between the two conditions was statistically significant (ttest). c. Current source density (CSD) plots showing the topography of current sources and sinks. Lighter regions reflect current sources and darker region current sinks. Contour lines represent 1mn graduations. - 16 -

Figure 4. Mean RT data Experiments 4 and 5. Participants made parity judgments (odd vs. even) to number stimuli (1-8) or magnitude estimation judgements (greater than or less than 5) to number stimuli (1-4 and 6-9) presented in one of four different numerical formats (words, Arabic numerals, dots in die-face configurations, and dots in random configurations). Mean response time data are shown for subitisable (1-3) and non-subitisable (4-9) stimuli for each numerical form for parity judgement and magnitude estimation tasks. Error bars represent standard errors. - 17 -

Figure 5. Mean RT data and visual ERPs a. Experiment 4. Mean response time data for each numerical value within each numerical form. Error bars represent standard errors. b. Grand average VEPs recorded over the occipital-temporal scalp data for random dot stimuli for numerical values 1-4 illustrating that N1 amplitude increases for 1-3 dots and but does not increase thereafter as additional dots are added to the display. - 18 -