Confounding and Bias

Similar documents
INTERNAL VALIDITY, BIAS AND CONFOUNDING

Confounding and Interaction

Bias and confounding. Mads Kamper-Jørgensen, associate professor, Section of Social Medicine

Biases in clinical research. Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University

Main objective of Epidemiology. Statistical Inference. Statistical Inference: Example. Statistical Inference: Example

Rapid appraisal of the literature: Identifying study biases

Bias. A systematic error (caused by the investigator or the subjects) that causes an incorrect (overor under-) estimate of an association.

Observational Study Designs. Review. Today. Measures of disease occurrence. Cohort Studies

Confounding Bias: Stratification

University of Wollongong. Research Online. Australian Health Services Research Institute

Bias. Sam Bracebridge

INTERPRETATION OF STUDY FINDINGS: PART I. Julie E. Buring, ScD Harvard School of Public Health Boston, MA

Bias. Zuber D. Mulla

Welcome to this series focused on sources of bias in epidemiologic studies. In this first module, I will provide a general overview of bias.

Incorporating Clinical Information into the Label

Online Supplementary Material

Epidemiologic Methods and Counting Infections: The Basics of Surveillance

EPIDEMIOLOGY-BIOSTATISTICS EXAM Midterm 2004 PRINT YOUR LEGAL NAME:

Declaration of interests. Register-based research on safety and effectiveness opportunities and challenges 08/04/2018

Challenges of Observational and Retrospective Studies

Biases in clinical research. Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University

Recent developments for combining evidence within evidence streams: bias-adjusted meta-analysis

Pharmacy practice research is

Protocol Development: The Guiding Light of Any Clinical Study

CHECKLIST FOR EVALUATING A RESEARCH REPORT Provided by Dr. Blevins

Controlling Bias & Confounding

Confounding, Effect modification, and Stratification

Confounding. Confounding and effect modification. Example (after Rothman, 1998) Beer and Rectal Ca. Confounding (after Rothman, 1998)

Purpose. Study Designs. Objectives. Observational Studies. Analytic Studies

Biases in clinical research. Seungho Ryu, MD, PhD Kanguk Samsung Hospital, Sungkyunkwan University

Epidemiologic Methods I & II Epidem 201AB Winter & Spring 2002

Complier Average Causal Effect (CACE)

What is a case control study? Tarani Chandola Social Statistics University of Manchester

The ROBINS-I tool is reproduced from riskofbias.info with the permission of the authors. The tool should not be modified for use.

DECLARATION OF CONFLICT OF INTEREST

Stratified Tables. Example: Effect of seat belt use on accident fatality

The role of Randomized Controlled Trials

Chapter 2. Epidemiological and Toxicological Studies

Joseph W Hogan Brown University & AMPATH February 16, 2010

Checklist for appraisal of study relevance (child sex offenses)

A NEW TRIAL DESIGN FULLY INTEGRATING BIOMARKER INFORMATION FOR THE EVALUATION OF TREATMENT-EFFECT MECHANISMS IN PERSONALISED MEDICINE

Problem solving therapy

Welcome to this third module in a three-part series focused on epidemiologic measures of association and impact.

Understanding Statistics for Research Staff!

Introduction. Step 2: Student Role - Your Plan of Action. Confounding. Good luck and have fun!

Presenter Disclosure Information

Penelitian IKM/Epidemiologi. Contact: Blog: suyatno.blog.undip.ac.id Hp/Telp: /

Bias and confounding special issues. Outline for evaluation of bias

Epidemiologic Study Designs. (RCTs)

Epidemiology & Evidence Based Medicine. Patrick Linden & Matthew McCall

Propensity Score Methods to Adjust for Bias in Observational Data SAS HEALTH USERS GROUP APRIL 6, 2018

EVect of measurement error on epidemiological studies of environmental and occupational

Epidemiological study design. Paul Pharoah Department of Public Health and Primary Care

Lecture Notes Module 2

Propensity score methods to adjust for confounding in assessing treatment effects: bias and precision

Epidemiology: Overview of Key Concepts and Study Design. Polly Marchbanks

GRADE. Grading of Recommendations Assessment, Development and Evaluation. British Association of Dermatologists April 2014

Can you guarantee that the results from your observational study are unaffected by unmeasured confounding? H.Hosseini

Epidemiologic study designs

Controlled Trials. Spyros Kitsiou, PhD

Lecture Slides. Elementary Statistics Eleventh Edition. by Mario F. Triola. and the Triola Statistics Series 1.1-1

Is There An Association?

Evidence-Based Medicine Journal Club. A Primer in Statistics, Study Design, and Epidemiology. August, 2013

115 remained abstinent. 140 remained abstinent. Relapsed Remained abstinent Total

Case-control studies. Hans Wolff. Service d épidémiologie clinique Département de médecine communautaire. WHO- Postgraduate course 2007 CC studies

PubH 7405: REGRESSION ANALYSIS. Propensity Score

EPI 200C Final, June 4 th, 2009 This exam includes 24 questions.

Causal Association : Cause To Effect. Dr. Akhilesh Bhargava MD, DHA, PGDHRM Prof. Community Medicine & Director-SIHFW, Jaipur

HARM. Definition modified from the IHI definition of Harm by the QUEST Harm Workgroup

Methodological Challenges in Observational Research: A Pharmacoepidemiological Perspective

Section 6.1 Sampling. Population each element (or person) from the set of observations that can be made (entire group)

Distraction techniques

WORKSHEET: Etiology/Harm

Methods for Addressing Selection Bias in Observational Studies

Measures of Association

Causal Inference Framework for Considering the Cardiovascular Risks Associated with Ortho Evra

Results. NeuRA Hypnosis June 2016

CPH601 Chapter 3 Risk Assessment

Structural Approach to Bias in Meta-analyses

First author. Title: Journal

Glossary From Running Randomized Evaluations: A Practical Guide, by Rachel Glennerster and Kudzai Takavarasha

Health Studies 315. Clinical Epidemiology: Evidence of Risk and Harm

Søren Friis. Center for Kræftforskning, Kræftens Bekæmpelse. Epidemiologi/Bias & confounding/phd/sep 2012/SF

Confounding and Effect Modification. John McGready Johns Hopkins University

Clinical research in AKI Timing of initiation of dialysis in AKI

Goal: To become familiar with the methods that researchers use to investigate aspects of causation and methods of treatment

Propensity scores and instrumental variables to control for confounding. ISPE mid-year meeting München, 2013 Rolf H.H. Groenwold, MD, PhD

Study design. Chapter 64. Research Methodology S.B. MARTINS, A. ZIN, W. ZIN

Søren Friis. Center for Kræftforskning, Kræftens Bekæmpelse. Epidemiology/Bias and confounding/phd/sep 2012/SF

Methods to control for confounding - Introduction & Overview - Nicolle M Gatto 18 February 2015

Traumatic brain injury

Goal: To become familiar with the methods that researchers use to investigate aspects of causation and methods of treatment

(i) Is there a registered protocol for this IPD meta-analysis? Please clarify.

Overview of Study Designs

Clinical problems and choice of study designs

OBSERVATIONAL MEDICAL OUTCOMES PARTNERSHIP

Critical Appraisal Series

Types of Biomedical Research

12/26/2013. Types of Biomedical Research. Clinical Research. 7Steps to do research INTRODUCTION & MEASUREMENT IN CLINICAL RESEARCH S T A T I S T I C

Overview of Study Designs in Clinical Research

Transcription:

28 th International Conference on Pharmacoepidemiology and Therapeutic Risk Management Barcelona, Spain August 22, 2012 Confounding and Bias Tobias Gerhard, PhD Assistant Professor, Ernest Mario School of Pharmacy and Institute for Health, Health Care Policy, and Aging Research Rutgers University Conflict of Interest The views and opinions expressed in this presentation are solely mine and do not represent the position or opinion of ISPE or any other institution. I have no conflicts of interest to declare. I have freely plagiarized lecture materials from previous years. Many thanks to my predecessors!

Outline Bias General Concepts Types of bias and what to do about it Confounding Bias Selection Bias Information Bias Summary and Implications

Bias and Chance Bias = Systematic Error Unaffected by study size Many specific biases have been described but generally all biases can be classified into one of three types: (1) confounding, (2) selection bias, and (3) information bias. Chance = Random Error Decreases as study size increases Confidence intervals, p-values Bias, Chance, and Causality Risk estimate differs from null value. What now? no no Chance? Bias? Cause? yes yes Not Causal Not Causal Statistics Epidemiology

Bias General Concepts Bias can affect all types of studies though observational studies are particularly vulnerable. Bias has a direction Bias towards the null observed value is closer to the null hypothesis than the true value Null Observed Truth Bias away from the null observed value is farther from the null hypothesis than the true value Null Truth Observed Null: 1 for ratio estimates, 0 for difference estimates Types of Bias Confounding Bias due to a third factor that distorts the association between exposure and outcome. Mixing of effects (from lat. confundere). Selection Bias Bias that results from the selection and retention of the study population. Information Bias Bias that results from poor measurement of study variables - exposure, outcome, confounders.

Precision and (Internal) Validity Random Error large large small small Systematic Error small large large small Terminology imprecise, valid imprecise, invalid precise, invalid precise, valid accurate External Validity External Validity Extended / Different Population Internal Validity Population Base Generalization Selection Bias Study Population

Confounding Confounding The quantitative association between exposure and outcome is distorted by a third factor with the following characteristics. The confounder is a risk factor for the outcome associated with the exposure (better: a direct or indirect predictor of the exposure) not an intermediate on the causal pathway between exposure and outcome

Confounding Exposure Outcome Confounder The confounder is a risk factor for the outcome associated with the exposure not an intermediate Confounding Exposure Outcome Confounder The confounder is a risk factor for the outcome associated with the exposure not an intermediate

Confounding Exposure Confounder Outcome The confounder is a risk factor for the outcome associated with the exposure not an intermediate Confounding Timing Matters Important Confounder Intermediate Variable Blood Pressure HCTZ Blood Pressure time

Confounding - Example Does drinking coffee cause cancer? Coffee? Cancer Cancer+ Cancer- Total Coffee+ 105 11395 11500 Coffee- 45 8455 8500 Observed RR = 1.72 (105/11395)/(45/8500) Confounding - Example Does drinking coffee cause cancer? or could there be confounding Coffee? Cancer Smoking?

Confounding - Example Stratified analysis: (Separation of factors so any mixture of their effect is removed) Full Cohort (N=20,000) Cancer+ Cancer- Coffee+ 105 11395 11500 Coffee- 45 8455 8500 Observed RR = 1.72 (105/11500)/(45/8500) Non Smokers (N=15,000) Smoker- Cancer+ Cancer- Coffee+ 25 7475 7500 Coffee- 25 7475 7500 Observed RR = 1.0 (25/7500)/(25/7500) Smokers (N= 5,000) Smoker+ Cancer+ Cancer- Coffee+ 80 3920 4000 Coffee- 20 980 1000 Observed RR = 1.0 (80/4000)/(20/1000) Confounding - Example Does drinking coffee cause cancer? Observed RR = 1.72 smoking(coffee+): 35% vs. smoking(coffee-): 12% cancer(smoking+): 2% vs. cancer (smoking-): 0.33% Coffee Observed True RR RR = 1.0 = 1.7 Cancer x~ 3 x~ 6 Smoking

Confounding - Example Coffee True RR= 1.0 Cancer x~ 3 x~ 6 Smoking Smoking acts as a confounder in our study of cancer risk from coffee drinking. Without controlling for this confounder, it appears that coffee drinkers are ~70% more likely to develop cancer. After stratification by smoking status, the analysis shows no increased risk of cancer for coffee drinkers. The apparent association resulted from the fact that (1) coffee drinkers are more likely to smoke and (2) smoking is a strong risk factorfor cancer. Confounding by Indication Good prescribing creates confounding Indication for treatment or severity of disease commonly predict the initiation of treatments Indication for treatment and disease severity are associated with the outcome of interest Exposure Outcome Indication, Severity

Confounding by Indication Good prescribing creates confounding Indicationfor treatment or severity of disease commonly predict the use or choice of treatment Indicationfor treatment and disease severity are associated with the outcome of interest SSRI Suicide Depression Confounding by Indication Good prescribing creates confounding Indication for treatment or severityof disease commonly predict the use of Indication for treatment and disease severityare associated with the outcome of interest ACEI vs. HCTZ MI Severity of hypertension

Confounding and Outcome of Interest Schneeweiss, CPT 2007 Confounding and Choice of Comparator Choice of comparator directly affects the presence and magnitude of confounding Non-use is generally a poor comparator ( confounding) Potential for confounding smaller for comparators with - Same indication - Similar contraindications - Same treatment modality - Similar adverse effects Comparator choice can be used to minimize confounding. Make sure that the clinical question remains relevant!

Addressing Confounding Identify potential confounders learn about determinants of treatment in your population Addressing Confounding Identify potential confounders learn about determinants of treatment in your population - Cannot solely be done from guidelines, literature - Misleading if obtained from (academic) super-specialists - Might vary in different populations (countries, health plans, etc) - Important role for drug utilization studies Confounders may be measured or unmeasured Measured confounders: Restriction, matching, stratification, multivariate analysis

Addressing Confounding Confounding vs. Effect Modification Exposure Outcome Effect Confounder modifier

Effect Modification Also called treatment effect heterogeneity Occurs when the effect of the exposure is different in different (sub)groups of the population Examples include genetics, age, sex There is no average true effect Results are best presented stratifies Facilitates personalized medicine Effect Modification - Example Effect of exposure differs by carrier status of a specific SNP: Stratified analysis: Full Cohort (N=2,000) AE+ AE- Exp+ 30 370 400 Exp- 160 1440 1600 Observed RR = 0.75 (30/400)/(160/1600) Treatment highly protective in SNP+ No effect in SNP- Average treatment effect misleading SNP- (N=1,600) SNP+ AE+ AE- Exp+ 20 180 200 Exp- 140 1260 1400 Observed RR = 1.0 (20/200)/(140/1400) SNP+ (N=400) SNP- AE+ AE- Exp+ 10 190 200 Exp- 20 180 200 Observed RR = 0.5 (10/200)/(20/200)

Selection Bias Selection Bias Distortions that result from procedures used to select subjects and from factors that influence participation/retention in the study. A distortion in the estimate of the effect due to the manner in which subjects are selected for the study. Bias of the estimated effect of an exposure on an outcome due to conditioning on a common effect of the exposure and the outcome (or of causes of the exposure and the outcome)

Selection Bias With disease Without disease Population base Exposed Study population Not exposed Should include equal proportions from each category Selection Bias With disease Without disease Population base Exposed Study population Not exposed Distorted picture of the population base

Example I: Selection Bias in Case Control Studies Imagine a cumulative case-control study conducted in one large hospital. The study aims to explore whether smoking increases the risk of experiencing a stroke. Cases are patients admitted for stroke, controls are patients admitted for everything else. In order to have an unbiased result, the controls need to be representative of the non-cases in the source population, particularly in regards to the exposure of interest (smoking). However, because smokers are also at higher risk for other diseases that lead to hospitalizations than non-smokers (lung cancer, COPD, etc), smoking is more common among hospitalized non-cases than among non-cases in the source population. This will result in an underestimation of the effect of smoking on stroke risk. Unbiased Control Selection Source Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.5) No Smoker 60 30000 Non-Smoker 40 60000 True OR = 3.0 Random Sample Cumulative Case-Control Study (4:1); (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.48) No Smoker 60 130 Estimated OR = 3.1 Non-Smoker 40 270

Biased Control Selection Source Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.5) No Smoker 60 30000 Non-Smoker 40 60000 True OR = 3.0 Hospitalization Hospitalized Population Smoker Non-Smoker No Biased Control Selection Source Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.5) No Smoker 60 30000 Non-Smoker 40 60000 True OR = 3.0 Hospitalization All cases are hospitalized Hospitalized Population Smoker 60 No Non-Smoker 40

Biased Control Selection Source Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.5) No Smoker 60 30000 Non-Smoker 40 60000 True OR = 3.0 Hospitalization Hospitalized Population Among the possible controls (i.e. the source population) smokers are more likely to be hospitalized than non smokers (1.8% vs. 0.6%). No Smoker 60 540 Non-Smoker 40 360 Biased Control Selection Source Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.5) No Smoker 60 30000 Non-Smoker 40 60000 True OR = 3.0 Hospitalization Hospitalized Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 1.5) No Smoker 60 540 Non-Smoker 40 360 Among the possible controls (i.e. the source population) smokers are more likely to be hospitalized than non smokers (1.8% vs. 0.6%).

Biased Control Selection Source Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.5) No Smoker 60 30000 Non-Smoker 40 60000 True OR = 3.0 Hospitalization Hospitalized Population sample controls for study No Smoker 60 540 240 Non-Smoker 40 360 160 Among the possible controls (i.e. the source population) smokers are more likely to be hospitalized than non smokers (1.8% vs. 0.6%). Biased Control Selection Source Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.5) No Smoker 60 30000 Non-Smoker 40 60000 True OR = 3.0 Hospitalization Study Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 1.5) No Smoker 60 240 Non-Smoker 40 160 Among the possible controls (i.e. the source population) smokers are more likely to be hospitalized than non smokers (1.8% vs. 0.6%).

Biased Control Selection Source Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.5) No Smoker 60 30000 Non-Smoker 40 60000 True OR = 3.0 Hospitalization Among the possible controls (i.e. the source population) smokers are more likely to be hospitalized than non smokers (1.8% vs. 0.6%). Study Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 1.5) No Smoker 60 240 Non-Smoker 40 160 Study OR = 1.0 Biased Control Selection Source Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 0.5) No Smoker 60 30000 Non-Smoker 40 60000 True OR = 3.0 Hospitalization Exposure distribution in study controls exposure distribution in source population controls Study Population (Exposure odds in non-cases = 1.5) No Smoker 60 240 Non-Smoker 40 160 Study OR = 1.0

Selection Bias in Case Control Studies In the example, the selection process for the controls sampled from hospitalized patients instead of randomly sampled from the non-cases in the source population changed the distribution of the exposure of interest (smoking) in the control patients of the study from the true distribution in the source population. Solution Population-based sampling of controls Selection Bias in Case Control Studies With disease Without disease Population base Exposed Study population (sampled from hospitalized patients) Not exposed

Example II Prevalent User Bias Those who develop outcomes stop taking the drug (depletion of susceptibles) Prevalent users tend to be healthy adherers and those that benefit from treatment In sum, inclusion of prevalent users will distort the study population (oversampling of subjects / person time at low risk) and result in underestimation of harms and overestimation of benefits Solution New user design Selection Bias Prevalent User Bias With disease Without disease Population base Exposed Study population Not exposed

Information Bias Information Bias Often referred to as measurement bias Occurs due to poor measurement (classification) of study variables (exposure, outcome, confounders) Particularly problematic when using secondary data Primary data: collected for research purposes Secondary data: collected for clinical, administrative, or payment purposes Distinguish two basic types of information bias Non-differential -Misclassification between groups is approximately equal Differential - Amount of misclassification differs between groups

Misclassification of Exposure Binary, non-differential Bias towards the null 20% of exposed subjects classified as unexposed (used OTC version of the drug) 10% of unexposed subjects classified as exposed (non-compliers) Truth AE+ AE- Exp+ 20 10 Exp- 80 90 Non-differential misclassification of exposure AE+ AE- Exp+ 20 4 10 2 Exp- 80 8 90 9 Observation AE+ AE- Exp+ 24 17 Exp- 76 83 True OR = 2.25 (20x90)/(80x10) Estimated OR = 1.54 (24x83)/(76x17) Misclassification of Exposure Binary, differential Direction of bias is unpredictable Truth AE+ AE- Exp+ 20 10 Exp- 80 90 True OR = 2.25 (20x90)/(80x10) Differential exposure misclassification I (e.g., recall bias) AE+ AE- Exp+ 20 10 3 Exp- 80 90 (0%) Differential exposure misclassification II AE+ AE- Exp+ 20 10 Exp- 80 90 (30%) (0%) 9 (10%) Observation I AE+ AE- Exp+ 20 7 Exp- 80 93 Observation II AE+ AE- Exp+ 20 19 Exp- 80 81 Estimated OR = 3.32 (20x93)/(80x7) Bias away from null Estimated OR = 1.07 (20x81)/(80x19) Bias towards null Exposure not binary Direction of bias is unpredictable

Misclassification of Outcome Analogous to misclassification of exposure Detection bias (differential misclassification of the outcome) often occurs when diagnostic procedures necessary for outcome classification are influenced by exposure Notable special case: Incomplete sensitivity in outcome ascertainment does not cause bias in risk ratio if specificity is 100% Truth AE+ AE- Exp+ 80 920 1000 Exp- 40 960 1000 True RR = 2.0 (80/1000)/(40/1000) AE+ AE- Exp+ 80 920 1000 40 (50%) Exp- 40 960 1000 20 (50%) Non-differential misclassification of the outcome. Specificity = 100% Observation AE+ AE- Exp+ 40 960 1000 Exp- 20 980 1000 Estimated RR = 2.0 (40/1000)/(20/1000) Misclassification of Confounders Adjustment with a binary non-differentially misclassified confounder reduces bias and produces a partially adjusted effect estimate that falls between the crude and true effect residual confounding Greenland and Robins, AJE 1985 Residual confounding decreases with increasing sensitivity and specificity of the misclassified confounder Savitz and Baron, AJE 1986 Necessary assumption (likely to hold in most applications in epidemiology) Effect of the confounder on the outcome is in the same direction among the treated and the untreated (i.e., there is no qualitative interaction between the treatment and the confounder) Ogburn and VanderWeele, Epidemiology 2012

Addressing Misclassification Prospective studies with primary data collection Ensure accurate measurement (instruments, procedures, quality control, etc) Studies that rely on secondary data Use validated measures for exposure, outcome, and confounding factors Rule out recall and detection biases Protopathic Bias

Protopathic Bias First symptoms of the outcome of interest are the reasonfor the prescriptionof the drug under study and the outcome. Reverse causation Prescribed estrogens for uterine bleeding time Diagnosis of endometrial cancer No disease Prodrome Fully symptomatic disease Summary

Best remedy for bias is prevention! Frame your question in a way that minimizes biases Design your study well to minimize biases Carefully analyze your results to control for confounding Understand the potential effects of residual bias and confounding on the risk estimate. Perform sensitivity analyses. What is the magnitude of the bias? In which direction is the estimate pulled? 1 2.5 Confounding Selection Bias attrition Information Bias Randomization Randomization Maximize protocol adherence/minimize Intent-to-treat analysis Primary data collection Blinding RCTs are explicitly designed to minimize bias. That does not mean that all RCTs do a good job with it!

Confounding Restriction(e.g., patients with hypertension; certain age range; free of contraindications) Choice of comparator (non-use vs. active treatment; same vs. different class/indication); e.g., healthy user effect! Statistical control (multivariate models, propensity scores, etc) Selection Bias New user design Population-based data / sampling Measurement Bias Use validated measures Rule out differential misclassification(recall & detection bias) If there are significant biases that can t be addressed, don t do the study! Thank you for your attention! Contact: tgerhard@rci.rutgers.edu