The rhythm of rapport: interpersonal synchrony and social perception

Similar documents
Cognitive consequences of novelty and familiarity : how mere exposure influences level of construal Förster, Jens

The Influence of Facial Attractiveness on Imitation Leeuwen, Matthijs L. van; Veling, Harm; Baaren, Rick B. van; Dijksterhuis, Ap

Are experimental economists prone to framing effects? A natural field experiment Gächter, Simon; Orzen, Henrik; Renner, Elke; Starmer, Chris

The effect of the refusal avoidance training experiment on final disposition codes in the German ESS-2 Schnell, Rainer; Trappmann, Mark

Sad, thus true: negativity bias in judgments of truth Hilbig, Benjamin E.

On the goal-dependency of unconscious thought Bos, Maarten W.; Dijksterhuis, Ap; Baaren, Rick B. van

Media violence and the self: the impact of personalized gaming characters in aggressive video games on aggressive behavior

Group dynamic processes in groups

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

A comparison between factor analysis and smallest space analysis of the comprehensive scoring system of the Rorschach Cohen, Arie

When and how communicated guilt affects contributions in public good dilemmas Wubben, Maarten J.J.; Cremer, David de; Dijk, Eric van

Get Angry, Get Out: The Interpersonal Effects of Anger Communication in Multiparty Negotiation

Hofmann, Wilhelm; Friese, Malte; Roefs, Anne Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

'Automatic' evaluation? Strategic effects on affective priming Teige-Mocigemba, Sarah; Klauer, Karl Christoph

Embodied perception with others bodies in mind : stereotype priming influence on the perception of spatial environment Chambon, Michel

Rezension: Timothy A. Brown, 2006: Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research Schmidt, Peter

How Love and Lust Change People s Perception of Partners and Relationships

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Self-Reference Effect and the Group-Reference Effect in the Recall of Shared and Unshared Information in Nominal Groups and Interacting Groups

Is Compensation Enough? Relational Concerns in Responding to Unintended Inequity

Cognitive abilities and behavioral biases Oechssler, Jörg; Roider, Andreas; Schmitz, Patrick W.

Riding the O Train: Comparing the Effects of Ostracism and Verbal Dispute on Targets and Sources Zadro, Lisa; Williams, Kipling D.

Analyzing quota sample data and the peer-review process

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

A twin study into the genetic and environmental influences on academic performance in science in 9-year-old boys and girls

Verbal and nonverbal behaviour as a basis for credibility attribution: the impact of task involvement and cognitive capacity

The issue of fuzzy concepts in test construction and possible remedies

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Changing explicit and implicit attitudes: the case of self-esteem Grumm, Mandy; Nestler, Steffen; Collani, Gernot von

The German Socio-Economic Panel (SOEP) as reference data set Siedler, Thomas; Schupp, Jürgen; Spiess, Katharina; Wagner, Gert G.

Daniël Lakens Eindhoven University of Technology. Mariëlle Stel Utrecht University

Gender Differences in the Relational and Collective Bases for Trust Maddux, William W.; Brewer, Marilynn B.

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

How Dissociated Are Implicit and Explicit Racial Attitudes? A Bogus Pipeline Approach Nier, Jason A.

Prospects for Group Processes and Intergroup Relations Research: A Review of 70 Years' Progress

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Surface-Level Diversity and Decision-Making in Groups: When Does Deep-Level Similarity Help?

The Effect of Group Decision Making on Cooperation in Social Dilemmas

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Autism spectrum disorders in children and adolescents with Moebius sequence

Incorporating eye tracking into cognitive interviewing to pretest survey questions

The politics of opinion assignment: a conditional logit model with varying choice set Gschwend, Thomas; King, Chad M.

Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination

Socioeconomic inequalities in cancer incidence and mortality - a spatial analysis in Bremen, Germany

Running head: DYNAMICAL SOCIAL SYNCHRONY 2. Measuring the Dynamics of Interactional Synchrony. R. C. Schmidt and Samantha Morr

Investigating Strategy Discovery and Coordination in a Novel Virtual Sheep Herding Game among Dyads

Modulation of facial mimicry by attitudes Likowski, Katja U.; Mühlberger, Andreas; Seibt, Beate; Pauli, Paul; Weyers, Peter

Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

"How much do you like your name?" An implicit measure of global self-esteem

Period Basin of Entrainment for Unintentional Visual Coordination

Popular evolutionary psychology in the UK: an unusual case of science in the media? Cassidy, Angela

Speaking for Others: The Pros and Cons of Group Advocates using Collective Language Hornsey, Matthew J.; Blackwood, Leda; O brien, Anne

Modulation of individual auditorymotor coordination dynamics through interpersonal visual coupling

Contact, Perspective Taking, and Anxiety as Predictors of Stereotype Endorsement, Explicit Attitudes, and Implicit Attitudes

The aaersion to lying Lundquist, Tobias; Ellingsen, Tore; Gribbe, Erik; Johannesson, Magnus

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

econstor Make Your Publications Visible.

Undesired self-image congruence in a lowinvolvement

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

What makes revenge satisfactory: seeing the offender suffer or delivering a message?

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Exploring the Meaningfulness and Relevance of Subjective Well-being for India Neff, Daniel

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Direct evidence on income comparisons and their welfare effects Senik, Claudia

Synchrony and the Social Tuning of Compassion

Impact of drug discount contracts on pharmacies and on patients' drug supply Gröber-Grätz, Dagmar; Gulich, Markus

Tropa, Mihai Alternative pedagogical strategies

The answer is blowing in the wind: weather effects on personality ratings

How Newly Acquainted Dyads Develop Shared Stereotypic Impressions through Conversation

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Scandals and the public's trust in politicians: assimilation and contrast effects

Utrecht University. Master Social Psychology THESIS. The effects of interpersonal synchrony on experienced self-agency. Cynthia Overeem,

Parental rating of sleep in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder Hvolby, Allan; Jørgensen, Jan; Bilenberg, Niels

Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Enabling healthy choices: is ICT the highway to health improvement?

Strik Lievers, Luisa; Curt, Florence; Wallier, Jenny; Perdereau, Fabienne; Rein, Zoé; Jeammet, Philippe; Godart, Nathalie

Motricity and Social Synchronization in ASD

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Public culture and public understanding of genetics: a focus group study

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

Zur Verfügung gestellt in Kooperation mit / provided in cooperation with:

Attitudinal and socio-structural determinants of cervical cancer screening and HPV vaccination uptake: a quantitative multivariate analysis

Income, Relational Goods and Happiness Becchetti, Leonardo; Trovato, Giovanni; Londono Bedoya, David Andres

Using a multilevel structural equation modeling approach to explain cross-cultural measurement noninvariance

Translating Answers to Open-ended Survey Questions in Cross-cultural Research: A Case Study on the Interplay between Translation, Coding, and Analysis

The Benefits of Synchronized Genuine Smiles in Face-to-Face Service Encounters

Accepted Manuscript. Personality and Personal Control. Simine Vazire, John M. Doris

Early Signs of Autism

Ignoring the Non-ignorables? Missingness and Missing Positions König, Thomas; Finke, Daniel; Daimer, Stephanie

MEDIATED CHAMELEONS: AN INTEGRATION OF NONCONSCIOUS BEHAVIORAL MIMICRY AND THE PARALLEL PROCESS MODEL OF NONVERBAL COMMUNICATION. Keturi D.

Pueblo chico, infierno grande Castro-Vazquez, Genaro; Tarui, Masayoshi Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article

Transcription:

www.ssoar.info The rhythm of rapport: interpersonal synchrony and social perception Miles, Lynden K.; Nind, Louise K.; Macrae, C. Neil Postprint / Postprint Zeitschriftenartikel / journal article Empfohlene Zitierung / Suggested Citation: Miles, L. K., Nind, L. K., & Macrae, C. N. (2009). The rhythm of rapport: interpersonal synchrony and social perception. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45(3), 585 589. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.002 Nutzungsbedingungen: Dieser Text wird unter dem "PEER Licence Agreement zur Verfügung" gestellt. Nähere Auskünfte zum PEER-Projekt finden Sie hier: http://www.peerproject.eu Gewährt wird ein nicht exklusives, nicht übertragbares, persönliches und beschränktes Recht auf Nutzung dieses Dokuments. Dieses Dokument ist ausschließlich für den persönlichen, nicht-kommerziellen Gebrauch bestimmt. Auf sämtlichen Kopien dieses Dokuments müssen alle Urheberrechtshinweise und sonstigen Hinweise auf gesetzlichen Schutz beibehalten werden. Sie dürfen dieses Dokument nicht in irgendeiner Weise abändern, noch dürfen Sie dieses Dokument für öffentliche oder kommerzielle Zwecke vervielfältigen, öffentlich ausstellen, aufführen, vertreiben oder anderweitig nutzen. Mit der Verwendung dieses Dokuments erkennen Sie die Nutzungsbedingungen an. Terms of use: This document is made available under the "PEER Licence Agreement ". For more Information regarding the PEER-project see: http://www.peerproject.eu This document is solely intended for your personal, non-commercial use.all of the copies of this documents must retain all copyright information and other information regarding legal protection. You are not allowed to alter this document in any way, to copy it for public or commercial purposes, to exhibit the document in public, to perform, distribute or otherwise use the document in public. By using this particular document, you accept the above-stated conditions of use. Diese Version ist zitierbar unter / This version is citable under: http://nbn-resolving.de/urn:nbn:de:0168-ssoar-284822

Accepted Manuscript The Rhythm of Rapport: Interpersonal Synchrony and Social Perception Lynden K. Miles, Louise K. Nind, C. Neil Macrae PII: S0022-1031(09)00032-8 DOI: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.002 Reference: YJESP 2208 To appear in: Journal of Experimental Social Psychology Received Date: 19 November 2008 Revised Date: 22 December 2008 Accepted Date: 1 February 2009 Please cite this article as: Miles, L.K., Nind, L.K., Macrae, C.N., The Rhythm of Rapport: Interpersonal Synchrony and Social Perception, Journal of Experimental Social Psychology (2009), doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2009.02.002 This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Interpersonal Synchrony 1 Running Head: Interpersonal Synchrony The Rhythm of Rapport: Interpersonal Synchrony and Social Perception Lynden K. Miles, Louise K. Nind and C. Neil Macrae University of Aberdeen Word count: 2480 Keywords: Interpersonal synchrony, behavioral coordination, rapport, social perception Address correspondence to: Lynden Miles School of Psychology University of Aberdeen King s College Aberdeen AB24 2UB Scotland, UK. Email: lynden.miles@abdn.ac.uk

Interpersonal Synchrony 2 Abstract The temporal coordination of behavior during dyadic interactions is a foundation for effective social exchange with synchronized actions enhancing perceptions of rapport and interpersonal connectedness. What has yet to be established, however, are the precise characteristics of behavioral coordination that give rise to such effects. Informed by a dynamical systems approach, the current investigation considered whether judgments of rapport are influenced by the mode of interpersonal synchrony. In two experiments, participants rated the degree of rapport manifest by a simulated pair of walkers exhibiting various configurations of synchronized strides. The results revealed that the highest levels of rapport were associated with the most stable forms of interpersonal coordination (i.e., in-phase and anti-phase synchrony), regardless of whether coordination between the walkers was conveyed via the presentation of visual (Expt. 1a) or auditory (Expt. 1b) cues. These findings underscore the importance of interpersonal coordination to core aspects of social perception.

Interpersonal Synchrony 3 The Rhythm of Rapport: Interpersonal Synchrony and Social Perception When two things keep happening simultaneously for an extended period of time, the synchrony is probably not an accident. (Strogatz, 2003, p. 2). Performing actions that are similar to, and coordinated with, those of an interaction partner elicits feelings of connectedness and interpersonal rapport (Bernieri, 1988; Bernieri, Davis, Rosenthal, & Knee, 1994; Cappella, 1997; Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; Lakin & Chatrand, 2003; Tickle-Degnen & Rosenthal, 1990). This relationship between coordinated behavior and social perception has been demonstrated in a variety of settings (e.g., parent-child bonding, intimate relationships, teacher-student interactions see Isabella, Belsky & van Eye, 1989; Julien, Brault, Chartrand & Bégin, 2000; LaFrance, 1979), such that the temporal organization of action is acknowledged to be a critical determinant of successful social exchange. To date, the social implications of behavioral coordination have been documented following both the mimicry of discrete bodily movements (e.g., foot shaking, face touching see Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; van Baaren, Maddux, Chartrand, de Bouter, & van Knippenberg, 2003) and the synchronization of more continuous sequences of action (e.g., postural movements, facial expressions, gestures see Bernieri, 1988; Cappella, 1997). Research in this domain has been driven by two primary objectives. First, to establish the emergence of mimicry by aggregating instances of to-be-imitated behavior (e.g., face touching) during social interactions (e.g., Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Second, to examine the relationship between coordinated behavior and perceptions of social connectedness (e.g., Bernieri et al., 1994). While this work has repeatedly demonstrated a positive association between coordination and rapport (see Chartrand, Maddux & Lakin, 2005), it has not yet considered whether variations in the dynamics of synchronized actions modulate the nature of this relationship (Semin & Cacioppo, 2008). That is, does the manner in which behavior is coordinated impact social perception?

Interpersonal Synchrony 4 Related work in the areas of dynamical systems and human movement science may inform this issue. Dynamical approaches to interpersonal coordination (see Schmidt & Richardson, 2008) consider the nature and degree of synchrony between two (or more) coupled oscillators, such as the recurrent movements of interacting individuals (e.g., the limb movements of two people walking side-by-side). This research has provided valuable evidence for spontaneous and unintentional behavioural entrainment across a range of activities, including walking (e.g., van Ulzen, Lamoth, Daffertshofer, Semin & Beek, 2008; Zivotofsky & Hausdorff, 2007), limb movements (e.g., Issartel, Marin & Cadopi, 2007; Schmidt, Carello & Turvey, 1990), postural sway (e.g., Shockley, Santana & Fowler, 2003), the swinging of handheld pendulums (e.g., Richardson, Marsh & Schmidt, 2005), and the motion of rocking chairs (e.g., Richardson, Marsh, Isenhower, Goodman & Schmidt, 2007). Of relevance to the current inquiry, when the movements of individuals (i.e., coupled oscillators) become entrained, two dominant modes of synchrony emerge (Haken, Kelso & Bunz, 1985; Schmidt & Richardson, 2008): in-phase coordination (i.e., a 0 relative phase relationship, 1 whereby the actions of each individual are at equivalent points of the movement cycle); and antiphase coordination (i.e., a 180 relative phase relationship, whereby each individual s actions are at opposite points of the movement cycle). While the actions of interacting individuals routinely move through the various intermediary stages of the relative phase relationship, over time they typically settle in a state of in-phase or anti-phase coordination (Schmidt et al., 1990). This effect arises because in-phase and anti-phase coordination reflect the globally stable attractor states for coupled oscillators (Haken et al., 1985), and indeed interpersonal coordination (Schmidt et al., 1990). Thus, when individuals coordinate or synchronize their actions, two pertinent observations can be made. First, social-cognitive research has revealed that aspects of the interaction notably judgments of social connectedness and rapport are enhanced when behavior is coordinated (e.g., Bernieri, 1988; Chartrand & Bargh, 1999). Second, work from a dynamical systems perspective has 1 In-phase coordination can also be defined as a 360 relative phase relationship.

Interpersonal Synchrony 5 shown that two modes of interpersonal coordination predominate, in-phase and anti-phase synchrony (Haken et al., 1985; Schmidt & Richardson, 2008). The integration of these related yet disparate literatures gives rise to an interesting question. Does the mode of interpersonal coordination impact people s evaluations of a dyadic interaction? To explore this issue, we assessed the level of rapport associated with a simulated pair of walkers displaying different modes of behavioral synchrony. Coordination mode was manipulated by varying the relative phase relationship between the strides of the walkers and the phase-relevant information was conveyed to participants either visually (Expt. 1a) or auditorily (Expt. 1b). Method Participants Sixty-six participants (31 females) ranging in age from 18-36 years took part in the research in exchange for course credit or 5. Thirty-six participants (19 females) completed Experiment 1a and 30 participants (12 females) completed Experiment 1b. Stimulus Materials Experiment 1a (Visual Synchrony): A simple 2-dimensional video animation of a walking figure was created by combining, in sequence, 11 static images of a stick figure at equally spaced degrees of stride length, ranging from full stride to feet together and back again to full stride. Target videos were constructed by duplicating the figure so it appeared that two stick characters were walking side-by-side. The phase relationship between the strides of the figures was varied by shifting the starting point in the animation sequence of one figure relative to the other. A separate video was created for each of the combinations of the starting positions of the one figure relative to the other (i.e., ranging from both figures starting at full stride, to one starting at full stride and the other at feet together, back to both starting at full stride ) resulting in 22 video clips. These clips represented regularly spaced increments in the relative phase relationship between the two

Interpersonal Synchrony 6 figures strides ranging from 0 to 360. The duration of each clip was 10 seconds and comprised 15 repetitions of the stride sequence for the two figures. Experiment 1b (Auditory Synchrony): A stereo audio recording was made of the footsteps of an adult male walking in hard-soled shoes on a firm surface at a comfortable pace (i.e., average inter-stride interval of 650 ms). To create the impression of two individuals walking together (i.e., two sets of footsteps), one channel of the recording was time-shifted to create a delay between channels in terms of the onset of each step. The relative phase relationship between the footsteps was manipulated by varying the amount of delay (50 600 ms, in 50 ms increments) for each channel giving a total of 24 audio clips. As for the video stimuli, these clips represented regularly spaced increments in the relative phase relationship between the two sets of footsteps ranging from 0 to 360. The clips comprised 19 footsteps in each channel and ranged in duration from 11.4 12 seconds. Procedure Participants were invited to take part in a study concerning perceptions of walking styles and were tested individually. Following their random assignment to one of the tasks, participants were informed that they would be making ratings of interpersonal rapport from either video (i.e., Expt. 1a) or audio recordings (i.e., Expt. 1b). Participants in Expt. 1a were told that they would be viewing stick figures that represented the movements of two interacting individuals. Participants in Expt. 1b were instructed that they would be listening to audio recordings of the footsteps of two individuals walking side-by-side. Following each video or audio clip, participants gave their ratings of rapport on a 9-point rating scale (anchored, 1 = no rapport at all; 9 = a high level of rapport). The video/audio clips were presented in a unique random order for each participant. Upon completion of the tasks, participants were debriefed and dismissed.

Interpersonal Synchrony 7 Results To preserve the structure of the phase manipulations, for the purpose of analysis the data sets were ordered linearly according to the relative phase relationship between walkers strides/footsteps. Thus, the first and last levels of the phase manipulation corresponded to in-phase coordination (i.e., a 0 /360 relative phase relationship). The intermediate levels of the phase manipulation corresponded to an incremental linear transition of the phase relationship, such that the middle values represented anti-phase coordination (i.e., a 180 relative phase relationship). Experiment 1a (Video Walkers) Mean ratings of rapport (see Figure 1, top panel) were subjected to a single factor (Relative Phase Level: 1-22) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). This yielded a significant effect on participants ratings [F(21, 735) = 52.76, p <.001, p rep > 0.99, η 2 p = 0.60]. Withinparticipants contrast tests were employed to examine this effect more closely. These revealed that the pattern of change in ratings of rapport fit a quartic 2 function [F (1, 35) = 209.49, p < 0.0001, p rep > 0.99, η 2 p = 0.86]. Participants perceived the level of interpersonal rapport to be highest when stride patterns reflected either in-phase or anti-phase coordination, and lowest at the mid-points of the transitions between in-phase and anti-phase movements (i.e., around 90 and 270 relative phase). Experiment 1b (Audio Walkers) As in Expt. 1a, mean ratings of rapport (see Figure 1, bottom panel) were submitted to a single factor (Relative Phase Level: 1-24) repeated measures ANOVA. This yielded a significant effect on participants ratings [F (23, 667) = 22.10, p < 0.001, p rep > 0.99, η 2 p = 0.43]. Withinparticipants contrast tests revealed that the effect of the phase manipulation on ratings of rapport fit 2 Quartic (or biquadratic) functions indicate three points of inflection (i.e., changes of direction) in a curve fitted to the data as can be seen in the W shaped curves in both panels of Figure 1.

Interpersonal Synchrony 8 a quartic 2 function [F (1, 29) = 118.53, p < 0.0001, p rep > 0.99, η 2 p = 0.80]. Replicating the video walkers, interpersonal rapport was highest when footsteps conveyed in-phase or anti-phase coordination. Again, the mid-points of the transitions between in-phase and anti-phase coordination (i.e., 90 and 270 relative phase) were perceived to reflect the lowest levels of rapport. Discussion A relationship between behavioral coordination and interpersonal connectedness has been revealed in previous research. For the most part, this work has operationalized coordination very broadly, either in terms of mimicry (e.g., Chartrand & Bargh, 1999; van Baaren et al., 2003) or synchrony (e.g., Bernieri et al., 1994; Capella, 1997) during dyadic interactions. As a result, little consideration has been given to the precise nature of the coordination observed and how this may impact the perceived quality of social exchanges. Addressing this issue, we explored whether the characteristics of coordinated action, specifically the mode of interpersonal synchrony modulates third-party judgments of rapport. The results were revealing. Across two experiments, in-phase and anti-phase synchrony were consistently judged to reflect the highest levels of interpersonal rapport. Moreover, transitions between in-phase and anti-phase synchrony were accompanied by strikingly consistent gradations in perceived rapport, such that when the relative phase relationship was arranged linearly (i.e., from 0 to 360 ) ratings of rapport revealed a strong curvilinear (i.e., quartic) pattern (see Figure 1). Importantly, perceiver s ratings of rapport were lowest when relative phase approximated 90 and 270 (i.e., the points most distant from in-phase and anti-phase), states which represent potentially unstable modes of coordination. This relationship was evident when phaserelevant information was presented visually (Expt. 1a) or auditorily (Expt. 1b), thereby demonstrating the generality of the observed effect. This association between the relative phase of coordination and rapport corresponds with the stability of in-phase and anti-phase synchrony and the relative instability of other modes of coordination within natural self-organizing systems (Haken et al., 1985; Strogatz, 2003). It would

Interpersonal Synchrony 9 appear that stability in the dynamics of an interpersonal coupling creates the appearance of a social connection or synergy between interacting individuals (Marsh, Richardson, Baron, & Schmidt, 2006) which, in turn, enhances judgments of rapport. That is, stable coordination generates a spatiotemporal context (i.e., common ground) that can be used to judge the qualitative aspects of social interaction. In this respect, deviations in critical aspects of the relative phase relationship (e.g., remaining coordinated but in unstable modes) are sufficient to disrupt interactional synergy and undermine perceptions of rapport. What this suggests is that perceivers naïve theories regarding the quality of interpersonal interactions appear to correspond almost exactly with the dynamical properties that support and constrain the entrainment of coupled oscillators within real-world physical systems (Haken et al., 1985; Strogatz, 2003). The current findings demonstrate that the manner in which behavior is coordinated is an important determinant of the perception of interpersonal connectedness. However, although the simulated dyads we employed provide a means to isolate synchronous movement from other potentially confounding variables (e.g., sex, emotion, social status), it remains to be seen whether our findings are applicable to the perception of actual, real-world interactions. While previous research has shown that perceptions of psychological attributes based on low-level kinematic information are consistent regardless of whether the movements are represented as videos of actual individuals or as animations of nonhuman entities (e.g., Morewedge, Preston & Wegner, 2007), it is likely that a host of social and contextual factors (e.g., goals, motives, target characteristics, task setting) play a significant role in shaping people s judgments. One useful task for future research will be to explore this issue. In addition, work should also consider whether qualitative variations in the synchronicity of behavior influence judgments of rapport when the perceiver is a member of the social unit in question. Given that third-party perceptions of rapport are closely aligned with interactants reports of the occurrence of rapport (e.g., Bernieri, Gillis, Davis & Grahe, 1996; Cappella, 1997), it is probable that first-person experiences of interpersonal connectedness during an interaction will also be influenced by the nature of behavioral coordination.

Interpersonal Synchrony 10 Of additional theoretical importance is the extent to which the beneficial effects of behavioral synchrony extend to the more cognitive aspects of social interaction. Aside from providing a foundation for enhanced rapport and effective social exchange (Semin, 2007; Wilson & Knoblich, 2005), we suspect that interpersonal synchrony may afford other cognitive advantages to interacting individuals. Wilson and Knoblich (2005), for example, have suggested that coordinated action provides a basis for perceptual emulation, a process through which self-perception can inform the understanding of other social agents and facilitate observational learning when self/other actions overlap. Supporting this claim, behavioral coordination has been shown to facilitate action planning and anticipatory control when individuals perform a joint tracking task (Knoblich & Jordan, 2003). Additionally, Wilson (2001) has proposed that coordinating one s actions with another may facilitate what she termed information flow, the dynamics of knowledge transmission during a social exchange. Through savings in working memory, coordinated behavior may enable attentional resources to be directed toward conspecifics, thereby enhancing the person-perception process. Recent findings provide preliminary support for this viewpoint. In a task designed to tap the incidental acquisition of person knowledge during a social exchange, Macrae, Duffy, Miles and Lawrence (2008) demonstrated that memory for an interaction partner s appearance and utterances were enhanced following instances of synchronized action. Thus, moving in perfect harmony not only facilitates the processes vital to the foundations of effective interaction, so too it modulates core aspects of person perception and understanding.

Interpersonal Synchrony 11 Acknowledgements This research was supported by a grant from The British Academy (SG-48408) awarded to LKM and CNM. LKM was supported by a Research Councils of the UK Academic Fellowship and CNM was supported by a Royal Society-Wolfson Fellowship. Thanks to Brad Miles for recording the footsteps used in Expt. 1b.

Interpersonal Synchrony 12 References Berneiri, F. J. (1988). Coordinated movement and rapport in teacher-student interactions. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 12, 120-138. Berneiri, F. J., Davis, J. M., Rosenthal, R., & Knee, C. R. (1994). Interactional synchrony and rapport: Measuring synchrony in displays devoid of sound and facial affect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 303-311. Berneiri, F. J., Gillis, J. S., Davis, J. M., & Grahe, J. E. (1996). Dyad rapport and the accuracy of its judgment across situations: A lens model analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 110-129. Cappella, J. N. (1997). Behavioral and judged coordination in adult informal social interactions: Vocal and kinesic indicators. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 119-131. Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893-910. Chartrand, T.L., Maddux, W., & Lakin, J. (2005). Beyond the perception-behavior link: The ubiquitous utility and motivational moderators of nonconscious mimicry. In R. Hassin, J. Uleman, & J.A. Bargh (Eds.), Unintended thought II: The new unconscious (pp. 334-361). New York: Oxford University Press. Haken, H., Kelso. J. A. S., & Bunz, H. (1985). A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand movements. Biological Cybernetics, 51, 347-356. Isabella, R. A., Belsky, J., & van Eye, A. (1989). Origins of mother-infant attachment: An examination of interactional synchrony during the infant s first year. Developmental Psychology, 25, 12-21. Issartel, J., Marin, L., & Cadopi, M. (2007). Unintended interpersonal coordination: Can we march to the beat of our own drum? Neuroscience Letters, 411, 174-179. Julien, D., Brault, M., Chartrand, É., & Bégin, J. (2000). Immediacy behaviors and synchrony in satisfied and dissatisfied couples. Canadian Journal of Behavioral Science, 32, 84-90.

Interpersonal Synchrony 13 Knoblich, G., & Jordan, J. S. (2003). Action coordination in groups and individuals: Learning anticipatory control. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 29, 1006-1016. LaFrance, M. (1979). Nonverbal synchrony and rapport: Analysis by the cross-lag panel technique. Social Psychology Quarterly, 42, 66-70. Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334-338. Macrae, C. N., Duffy, O. K., Miles, L. K., & Lawrence, J. (2008). A case of hand waving: Action synchrony and person perception. Cognition, 109, 152-156. Marsh, K. L., Richardson, M. J., Baron, R. M., & Schmidt, R. C. (2006). Contrasting approaches to perceiving and acting with others. Ecological Psychology, 18, 1-38. Morewedge, C. K., Preston, J., & Wegner, D. M. (2007). Timescale bias in the attribution of mind. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 1-11. Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., Isenhower, R. W., Goodman, J. R. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2007). Rocking together: Dynamics of intentional and unintentional interpersonal coordination. Human Movement Science, 26, 867-891. Richardson, M. J., Marsh, K. L., & Schmidt, R. C. (2005). Effects of visual and verbal interaction on unintentional coordination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31, 62-79. Schmidt, R. C., & O Brien, B. (1997). Evaluating the dynamics of unintended interpersonal coordination. Ecological Psychology, 9, 189-206. Schmidt, R. C., Carello, C., & Turvey, M. T. (1990). Phase transitions and critical fluctuations in visual coordination of rhythmic movements between people. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 227-247.

Interpersonal Synchrony 14 Schmidt, R. C., & Richardson, M. J. (2008). Dynamics of interpersonal coordination. In A. Fuchs & V. K. Jirsa (Eds.), Coordination: Neural, behavioral and social dynamics (pp. 281-307). Berlin: Springer-Verlag. Semin, G. R. (2007). Grounding communication: Synchrony. In A. Kruglanski & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles (2 nd ed., pp. 630-649). New York: Guilford Publications. Semin, G. R. & Cacioppo, J. T. (2008). Grounding social cognition: Synchronization, entrainment and coordination. In G. R. Semin & E. R. Smith (Eds.), Embodied grounding: Social, cognitive, affective, and neuroscientific approaches (pp. 119-147). New York: Cambridge University Press. Shockley, K., Santana, M. V., & Fowler, C. A. (2003). Mutual interpersonal constraints are involved in cooperative conversation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 29, 326-332. Strogatz, S. (2003). Sync: The emerging science of spontaneous order. New York: Hyperion. Tickle-Dengen, L., & Rosenthal, R. (1990). The nature of rapport and its nonverbal correlates. Psychological Inquiry, 1, 285-293. van Baaren, R. B., Maddux, W. W., Chartrand, T. L., De Bouter, C., & Van Knippenberg, A. (2003). It takes two to mimic: Behavioral consequences of self-construals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 84, 1093-1102. van Ulzen, N. R., Lamoth, C. J. C., Daffertshofer, A., Semin, G. R., & Beek, P. J. (2008). Characteristics of instructed and uninstructed interpersonal coordination while walking side-byside. Neuroscience Letters, 432, 88-93. Wilson, M. (2001). Perceiving imitatible stimuli: Consequences of isomorphism between input and output. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 543-553. Wilson, M. & Knoblich, G. (2005). The case for motor involvement in perceiving conspecifics. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 460-473.

Interpersonal Synchrony 15 Zivotofsky, A. Z., & Hausdorff, J. M. (2007). The sensory feedback mechanisms enabling couples to walk synchronously: An initial investigation. Journal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation, 4, 28.

Interpersonal Synchrony 16 Experiment 1a (Video) 9 8 7 Mean Ratings Mean of Rapport (1 / = Coordination No Rapport at Ratings All, 9 = (1 A = High None, Level 9 = A of lot) Rapport) 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 In-phase Anti-phase In-phase (0 ) (180 ) (360 ) Relative phase relationship between strides Experiment 1b (Audio) 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 In-phase Anti-phase In-phase (0 ) (180 ) (360 ) Relative phase relationship between footsteps Figure 1: Mean ratings of rapport as a function of relative phase for E1a (top panel) and E1b (bottom panel). Error bars indicate 1 standard error of the mean.