SOCIAL STRUCTURE, SOCIALIZATION AND THE SELF: SOME HISTORICAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL REFLECTIONS.

Similar documents
The four chapters in Part I set the stage. Chapter 1 moves from the implicit common sense theories of everyday life to explicit theories that are

MARK SCHEME for the May/June 2011 question paper for the guidance of teachers 9699 SOCIOLOGY

On the Comparison of Systems Theory and Neoinstitutionalism 1

INTRODUCTION THE PSYCHOLOGY OF MODERN LEFTISM

CHAPTER-5. Family Disorganization & Woman Desertion by Socioeconomic Background

Cannabis Changes. Understanding Dynamics of Use and Dependence N. Liebregts

SOC-SOCIOLOGY (SOC) SOC-SOCIOLOGY (SOC) 1

Chapter One. The Sociological Point of View

Test Bank. Chapter 2: The Cultural Context. Multiple Choice

Opener. Look at opener and answer questions as papers are being passed out. Keep these (questions on tests arise from worksheets/ etc)

Socialization Peter L. Berger

Individual and Society: Sociological Social Psychology Lizabeth A. Crawford & Katerine B. Novak Table of Contents

PSYCHOLOGY CONTENT STANDARDS

Social Studies 4 8 (118)

THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES IN SOCIOLOGY F U N C T I O N A L I S M, C O N F L I C T T H E O R Y A N D S Y M B O L I C I N T E R A C T I O N A L I S M

Motivation in Historical Perspective. Chapter 2

Introduction to Criminology Theme 1

Holism in health and caring. - A nursing perspective.

SOCIAL STRATIFICATION AND SOCIAL VALUES

1. The benefit of sociology according to Bob Brym is that it can help create

Dimensions of Health and Illness: Toward an Integrated Model

PYSC 224 Introduction to Experimental Psychology

CHAPTER 1 Criminology and the Sociological Perspective

Ian Rory Owen. Psychotherapy and Phenomenology: On Freud, Husserl and Heidegger. Lincoln, NE: iuniverse, Inc., 2006.

Sociology Matters. Culture and Socialization. Culture and Socialization. What is Culture? What is Culture? 9/5/2012

Altruism & the New (non- American) Social Psychology. Prosocial behaviour is. Altruism is. Week 10

The Personal Profile System 2800 Series Research Report

Introduction to Sociology:

Between Micro and Macro: Individual and Social Structure, Content and Form in Georg Simmel

Instructor's Manual for Gregg Barak s Integrating Criminologies. Prepared by Paul Leighton (Boston: Allyn & Bacon, 1997) * CHAPTER 1

Using the sociological perspective changes how we perceive the surrounding world and ourselves. Peter Berger

Henry Matisse, La danse, The Great Watershed

Social Change in the 21st Century

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

COWLEY COLLEGE & Area Vocational Technical School

BACHELOR S DEGREE IN SOCIAL WORK. YEAR 1 (60 ETCS) Fundamentals of Public and Private Law Sociology. Practicum I Introduction to Statistics

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE COLLEGE AMONG TEACHERS IN COIMBATORE

Angell Society, Detroit (June 11, 1991) Good evening Anne and I are delighted to be here tonight to honor and thank this very special group of

CROSS CULTURAL COMMUNICATION

This article, the last in a 4-part series on philosophical problems

Introducing Reflexivity.

MARK SCHEME for the May/June 2006 question paper 0495 SOCIOLOGY

Introduction to Social Psychology p. 1 Introduction p. 2 What Is Social Psychology? p. 3 A Formal Definition p. 3 Core Concerns of Social Psychology

THEORY OF POPULATION CHANGE: R. A. EASTERLIN AND THE AMERICAN FERTILITY SWING

Personal identity is cultural identity. Culture is a powerful organizer of

Audience research US Empirical traditions of audience research Effects research Bobo dolls and short term behavioural effects Results

The New Golden Rule by Amitai Etzioni

Contents. Part I Setting the Stage... 1

What is the importance of Durkheim s Suicide to Sociology? How relevant are his views about the individual and sociaty today?

ILLINOIS LICENSURE TESTING SYSTEM

Review of Interspecies Ethics. Cynthia Willett Columbia Univ. Press 2014 p. 232, pbk. Thomas Randall University of Western Ontario

Cultural Models and Implicit Theories of Self and World: A View from (Cross-)Cultural Psychology!

Field 052: Social Studies Psychology Assessment Blueprint

Using the sociological perspective changes how we perceive the surrounding world and ourselves. Peter Berger

Writing in an Academic Style Module: Introduction

LIFE NARRATION AND SOCIAL WORK. Kirsi Nousiainen

UNIT 1. THE DIGNITY OF THE PERSON

The Sociological Perspective

Social Freedom: Life Satisfaction:

Key Concepts in. Sociology

Towards a biocultural approach of dissociative consciousness

Symbolic Interactionism

HOW TO IDENTIFY A RESEARCH QUESTION? How to Extract a Question from a Topic that Interests You?

SFHPT24 Undertake an assessment for family and systemic therapy

SOC 101/Introduction to Sociology 1 course unit SOC 170/Topics in Sociology 1 course unit SOC 205/Introduction to Social Work 1 course unit

NCERT Solutions Class 11 Psychology. Chapter - The Bases of Human Behaviour

CALL FOR PAPERS: THE 8th MIDTERM CONFERENCE ON EMOTIONS, EDINBURGH, 2018

Identity Development. Kate C. McLean Western Washington University. Moin Syed University of Minnesota

INTRODUCTION IDENTITY OF THE INDIVIDUAL IDENTITY OF THE COLLECTIVE IDENTITY OF PLACE AIM. University of Pretoria

#70 Carl Jung: What are the Archetypes?

A-LEVEL General Studies B

PLOs for Social Science & Humanities Division - Active Only

LEN 227: Introduction to Corrections Syllabus 3 lecture hours / 3 credits CATALOG DESCRIPTION

Unconscious motivation

THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK

Check List: B.A in Sociology

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing.

Chapter 10 Human Population and Human Behavior. Devon Rood SOS 513

May 19, 2014 August 9, 2014 UNDERGRADUATE COURSES

society. The social perspective is a way of looking at society. It sees society as something over and above the very people who are in that society.

Groups, norms, and conformity. What s it about?

Autonomous Family Groups and Networks: A Promising Development

Emotion, History Notes

Attitudes of Family Heads towards Endogamy Marriage in the UAE society* Dr. Ahmad Falah Alomosh Dr. Yousef Hasan Abu Laila Faculty of Arts and

AFFECTS, MOODS, EMOTIONS, AND BELONGING

School Philosophy. Introduction

2017 Sociology. Higher. Finalised Marking Instructions

Fairfield Public Schools Social Studies Curriculum Sociology Grades 11-12

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS SOCIOLOGY Spring 2018

Promoting Sexual Health: The Public Health Challenge

GCSE EXAMINERS' REPORTS

Richard Nisbett is a Distinguished University Professor at the University of

GOVERNOR LIVINGSTON HIGH SCHOOL SOCIAL STUDIES DEPARTMENT SOCIOLOGY #SSS0002

MCDSARE: 2017 International Multidisciplinary Scientific Conference on the Dialogue between Sciences & Arts, Religion & Education

Historical genealogy of the concept of "risk"

Sr.Secondary: Sociology Sociology 331

Unconscious motivation

Unconscious motivation

SHORT NOTES I or we : Family socialization values in a national probability sample in Taiwan

The Deaf of Bulgaria The Bulgarian Sign Language Community

Transcription:

SOCIAL STRUCTURE, SOCIALIZATION AND THE SELF: SOME HISTORICAL AND CROSS-CULTURAL REFLECTIONS. Victor Pereira da Rosa - University of Ottawa Paul Lamy - University of Ottawa Ab s t r a c t Norbert Elias (1998; 2000) argument that in European societies, from the period of medieval times until the mid-20th century, the self became more individuated and self-regulation more internalized as these societies became more socially differentiated and complex is critically analyzed. His perspective is applied beyond these initial parameters to the emergent globalized world in which we now live. Re s u m o Norbert Elias (1998; 2000) postula que, nas sociedades europeias, da Idade Média até meados do Século XX, o self se individualizou progressivamente à medida que estas sociedades se tornaram mais complexas. Este trabalho tenta aplicar esta perspectiva ao mundo contemporâneo no contexto da globalização. It is the purpose of this paper to look at some of the relations between social structure and the self from a historical and cross-cultural perspective.at birth, any baby enters a world already socially structured and with norms and values which will, for better or worse, shape his or her personality and its social expression. It boils down to a question of cultural adaptation to the group one must live in and its environment. The individual acquires not only the knowledge and skills necessary for living in that group but also a consciousness and a concept of self. Symbolic communication is essential to these processes and they can vary across time and space. The languages spoken by hunting and gathering bands often lack the grammatical first person. These could certainly constitute examples of communitarian as opposed to individualistic cognitive systems. This, of course, in no way means that socialization is less intensive in such social groups and indeed the opposite could possibly be the case. A common distinction made by anthropologists and cross-cultural social psychologists is between collectivistic and individualistic societies. In collectivistic societies, it has been argued, the sense of self is more interdependent with others than in individualistic societies, in which the sense of self is more independent and autonomous (cf. Triandis, 1995). Hunting and gathering as well as agrarian societies usually rank higher on indicators of collectivism than do industrial or postindustrial ones. The most differentiated and socially complex societies are also the most individualized. This raises the issue of whether there is an evolutionary trend for consciousness and the concept of self to become more individuated as societies become more differentiated and complex. Norbert Elias argues that this is indeed the case, at least as regards European society. In his study of changes in European society from medieval times to the mid 20th century, he found that as social functions become more and more differentiated: more and more people must attune their conduct to that of others, the web of actions must be organized more and more strictly and accurately, if each individual action is to fulfill its social function. The individual is compelled to regulate his conduct in an increasingly differentiated, more even and stable manner (Elias, 1998, p. 52).

212 Schooler (1998, p. 46) in an analysis of how history and social structure affected individualism in Japan, concluded that there is substantial evidence that the level of individualism in Japan has risen in response to such socio-environmental changes as increased environmental complexity (1998, p. 46). Elias, in The Civilizing Process, presents a fascinating and penetrating analysis of how self-consciousness, inhibition, and affect modulation changed over time as Europe moved from feudalism to the present era of post-industrialism. Not only social complexity but also the centralization of state authority and its administrative apparatus were considered by Elias to be very important variables impacting on the way in which affect and its modulation change over time in response to changes in social structure. The direction of change in Europe he describes as the process of civilization ; central to this process has been increasing control of the expression of the drives -- such as, for example, the sex drive, which: measured in terms of the standards of the men of successive upper classes, control has grown ever stricter. The drive has been slowly but progressively suppressed from the public life of society. The reserve that must be exercised in speaking of it has also increased. And this restraint, like all others, is enforced less and less by direct physical force. It is cultivated in individuals from an early age as habitual self-restraint by the structure of social life, by the pressure of social institutions in general, and by certain executive organs of society, above all, the family (2000, p. 158). More control, restraint, and concealment of drives and impulses of all kinds becomes required, as societies become more structurally complex and differentiated and these inhibitions become increasingly a part of the self. What Elias is getting at may perhaps be understood from his portrayal of affect modulation in the preceding feudal period: a moment ago they were joking, now they mock each other, one word leads to another, and suddenly in the midst of laughter they find themselves in the fiercest feud. Much of what appears contradictory to us the intensity of their piety, the violence of their fear of hell, their guilt feelings, the penitence, the immense outbursts of joy and gaiety, the sudden flaring and the uncontrollable force of their hatred and belligerence -- all these, like the rapid changes of mood, are in reality symptoms of one and the same structuring of the emotional life (Elias, 2000, p. 168). The picture is one of low modulation of affect, the importance of external rather than internal constraint and reliance on fear and guilt rather than shame and embarrassment. By the civilizing process, Elias (1998, p. 49), then, is essentially referring to the rationalization of human instinctual and affective life, in a sense that is reminiscent of Max Weber s use of this term. The internalization of inhibitions charged with socio-genetic shame and embarrassment become central to the socialization process in European society, according to Elias. This occurred as Europe became more socially differentiated, complex, and politically and administratively centralized. One way in which affects associated with the drives came to be more modulated and controlled was through the emergence of a far more clearly defined split between an intimate and public sphere, and between private and public behaviour than had existed during the feudal period. He refers to a growing division of behaviour into what is and is not publicly permitted, the psychic structure of people is also transformed (2000, p.160). This transformation is such that social rules of conduct become a constituent element of...individual selves (2000, p. 160); in other words individuals carry society within themselves and in all situations (even when alone). From another starting point Triandis (1989; 1995) offers a somewhat different view of the impact of increasing social complexity and the relationships between an individual and his society. He distinguishes between three types of society according to their cul-

tural complexity: extremely simple societies, such as the Australian Aborigines; societies in the middle range of cultural complexity, such as the Roman or Chinese empires, and highly complex cultures such as the industrial and post-industrial societies of today. In the middle range of cultural complexity, control exercised by such entities as nuclear and extensive families, clan, city, and state is higher than in more complex modern societies. This suggests that the effects of political and administrative centralization may not always be such as to increase individualization and to lead to internal as opposed to external constraint as the predominant form of both self and social control. 213 As European society became increasingly differentiated and complex, a more clearly defined split between the private and public dimensions of social life occurred. Elias sees this split as facilitating the modulation of affect associated with basic human drives, and a major reason why this split developed was to make this process more manageable. However, there is also the possibility that the emergence of more sharply defined public and private domains in many highly complex societies may reflect the need and necessity to give meaning and comprehension to central life activities. Da Rosa and Lamy (1999, p. 56) have pointed out that as social complexity increases individuals are left with a more fragmentary grasp of broader cultural patterns of meaning. This may produce what Calhoun (1991, p. 96) has referred to as an increasing split between everyday life and large-scale systemic integration. Further, as social complexity augments, participation in public space may become less rewarding in personal terms. Concerning North America, Maher (1992, p.131) remarks: Neighbourhoods, as places to find friendship, identity, and support, have generally disappeared from the American stage and parenting is now a lonely undertaking. More generally, the expanded and more clearly delimited private domain of social life has become more complex, differentiated, and less subject, in many respects, to external constraint.. Braithwaite (1993, p. 6) argues that Elias greatly exaggerated the extent to which affect modulation through the internalization of inhibitions charged with socio-genetic shame became democratized and that this theory remains one of more or less unilinear evolution. The argument that the affect structure of the upper-classes in European society diffused down to the middle and lower classes is complicated by the fact that there has been continued interpenetration of upper-class and middle class manners and morals. Equally problematic is that affect control in the United States was shaped predominantly by the middle-class. The working-class and young, working class males in particular, are not characterized by the same degree of internalization and affect control as the middle- and upper-classes (Braithwaite, 1993). Much solid cross-national research has been conducted which has found that in both Europe and North America, working-class men value conformity and obedience (that is, external constraint) more than those from other social classes and that these values are reflected in the way in which they socialize their children; at least part of the reason that conformity and obedience are valued is that these reflect the requirements of working-class jobs, which lack the autonomy and self-direction of jobs further up the occupational scale. There is, then, a relationship between the social complexity inherent in occupations and social positions and the extent to which affect modulation is related to internalization as opposed to external constraint. Braithwaite (1993) also points to the role of an important variable that is largely ignored by Elias urbanization; in large cities, Braithwaite argues, a higher degree of internalization and affect modulation are required in order for them to be able to function. Both urbanization and industrialization are accompanied by increasing social differentiation

214 and both are increasingly powerful forces in global perspective. By mid-century almost two-thirds of the world s population is projected to live in urban areas and working in national economies which are becoming increasingly globally interdependent with occupational structures trending toward the industrial and postindustrial. All of this is part of part of the larger process of modernization which is becoming worldwide.. Huntington (1996, p. 126) views the rupture of traditional bonds and social relations in societies undergoing modernization as resulting in widespread anomie and identity problems at the individual level because of individuation. In other words, as the collective dimensions of social life erode, self identity becomes more problematic, sources of social support may weaken or decline, and external constraint becomes far more circumscribed as a mechanism of social control in a highly complex and differentiated social world. While Huntington (1996) predicts reaction and the resurgence of indigenous cultures, Berger (2002, p. 9) predicts a cultural earthquake : All sectors of the emerging global culture enhance the independence of the individual over against tradition and collectivity. Individuation must be seen as a social and psychological process, manifested empirically in the behaviour and consciousness of people regardless of the ideas they may hold about this. Extrapolating from Elias (1998; 2000) argument, one dimension of the cultural earthquake predicted by Berger (2002) might be the outcome of the ways in which different cultures, in response to increasing structural differentiation, social complexity, and individuation, demarcate the public and private dimensions of social life and gear socialization to internalization as opposed to external constraint.

Re f e re n c e s Berger, P. L. (2002). The Cultural Dynamics of Globalization. In: Berger, P. L. and Huntington, S.P. (eds.). Many Globalizations: Cultural Diversity in the Contemporary World. Oxford, U.K., Oxford University Press, pp. 1-16 Braithwaite, J. (1993). Shame and Modernity. In: The British Journal of Criminology, Vol. 33, No. 1, pp. 1-18. Da Rosa, V. and Lamy, P. (1999) From Tribalism to Globalization: Some Problems and Challenges. In: Antropológicas, Vol. 3, pp. 55-61. Davis, J. (Ed). (2000). Identity and Social Change. London, Transaction Publishers. Elias, N. (1998). On Civilization, Power, and Knowledge; Selected Writings. Chicago, University of Chicago Press. Elias, N. (2000). The Civilizing Process; Sociogenetic and Psychogenetic Investigations. (Revised Edition). Oxford, Blackwell. Huntington, S. P. (1996). The Clash of Civilizations and the Remaking of World Order. New York, Simon and Schuster. Inglehart, R. (1997). Modernization and Postmodernization: Cultural, Economic and Political Change in 43 Societies. Princeton, N.J, Princeton University Press. Maher, T. (1992). The Withering of Community Life and the Growth of Emotion Disorders. In: Journal of Sociology and Social Welfare, pp. 125-146. Schooler, C. (1998). History, Social Structure and Individualism; A Cross-Cultural Perspective on Japan. In: International Journal of Comparative Sociology, Vol. 39, No. 1, pp. 2-51. Triandis, H. (1995). Individualism and Collectivism. Boulder, Colorado, Westview Press. Triandis, H. (1989). The Self and Social Behavior in Differing Cultural Contexts. In: Psychological Review, Vol. 96, No. 3, pp. 506-520. Tuan, Yi-Fu (1982). Segmented Worlds and Self; Group Life and Individual Consciousness. Minneapolis, University of Minnesota Press. 215