Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis'

Similar documents
The effect of decision frame and decision justification on risky choice

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

How People Estimate Effect Sizes: The Role of Means and Standard Deviations

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 2010, 111, 3, Perceptual and Motor Skills 2010 KAZUO MORI HIDEKO MORI

The Effects of Voice Pitch on Perceptions of Attractiveness: Do You Sound Hot or Not?

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information on Consumer Elaboration and Brand Attitude

The Effects of Product Attribute s New Information and Target Compatibility on Consumer Elaboration

An Implicit Assessment of the Effect of Artificial Cheek Raising: When Your Face Smiles, the World Looks Nicer

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

Several studies have researched the effects of framing on opinion formation and decision

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

The Game Prisoners Really Play: Preference Elicitation and the Impact of Communication

Matching Versus Mismatching Attitude Functions: Implications for Scrutiny of Persuasive Messages

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

Cognitive dissonance in children: Justification of effort or contrast?

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Social Presence Measure

INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES, COGNITIVE ABILITIES, AND THE INTERPRETATION OF AUDITORY GRAPHS. Bruce N. Walker and Lisa M. Mauney

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Measure of Social Presence

Sawtooth Software. The Number of Levels Effect in Conjoint: Where Does It Come From and Can It Be Eliminated? RESEARCH PAPER SERIES

What is Beautiful is Good Online: Physical Attractiveness, Social Interactions and Perceived Social Desirability on Facebook Abstract

Test-Taking Strategies and Task-based Assessment: The Case of Iranian EFL Learners

Black 1 White 5 Black

Supplemental Data: Capuchin Monkeys Are Sensitive to Others Welfare. Venkat R. Lakshminarayanan and Laurie R. Santos

DIFFERENCES IN THE ECONOMIC DECISIONS OF MEN AND WOMEN: EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE*

Experimental Testing of Intrinsic Preferences for NonInstrumental Information

The Effects of Expertise and Physical Attractiveness upon Opinion Agreement and Liking*

PSY The Psychology Major: Academic and Professional Issues. Module 8: Critical Thinking. Study Guide Notes

The Influence of Framing Effects and Regret on Health Decision-Making

Framing the frame: How task goals determine the likelihood and direction of framing effects

EXPERIMENTAL ECONOMICS INTRODUCTION. Ernesto Reuben

ATTITUDE CHANGE FROM AN IMPLIED THREAT TO ATTITUDINAL FREEDOM 1

Highlighting Effect: The Function of Rebuttals in Written Argument

RUNNING HEAD: RAPID EVALUATION OF BIG FIVE 1

Acceptance of Help as a Function of Similarity of the Potential Helper and Opportunity To Repay'

Selecting Research Participants. Conducting Experiments, Survey Construction and Data Collection. Practical Considerations of Research

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

Bridging the Gap: Predictors of Willingness to Engage in an Intercultural Interaction

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

Veronika Grimm, Friederike Mengel. Let me sleep on it: Delay reduces rejection rates in Ultimatum Games RM/10/017

COOPERATION 1. How Economic Rewards Affect Cooperation Reconsidered. Dan R. Schley and John H. Kagel. The Ohio State University

SHORT REPORT Facial features influence the categorization of female sexual orientation

An Integrated Approach to Consumers Processing of Health Marketing Communication Messages

Review Questions for Exam 2 Math 263

Gender Difference in Emotional and Behavioral Responses of Being Rendered Invisible

The Effects of Involvement and Information Processing on Latitudes of Acceptance: An Elaboration Likelihood Perspective

Resistance to Persuasion and Attitude Certainty: The Moderating Role of Elaboration

Effect of Importance of Freedom and Attraction to Group Members on Influence Produced by Group Pressure

Chapter 3-Attitude Change - Objectives. Chapter 3 Outline -Attitude Change

Effects of Sequential Context on Judgments and Decisions in the Prisoner s Dilemma Game

GOLDSMITHS Research Online Article (refereed)

Lecture Notes Module 2

Attention, Frame Condition and Decision Making Under Risk:

2 Critical thinking guidelines

Thinking and Intelligence

Effects of self-complexity on mood-incongruent recall 1

I love it or I hate it? The positivity effect in stated preferences for agent evaluation

Impression order effects as a function of the personal relevance of the object of description*

Cognitive dissonance: effects of perceived choice on attitude change

Product Assortment and Individual Decision Processes

Desirability Bias: Do Desires Influence Expectations? It Depends on How You Ask.

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

Athletic Identity and Life Roles of Division I and Division III Collegiate Athletes

Influence of Agent Type and Task Ambiguity on Conformity in Social Decision Making

Middlesex University Research Repository: an open access repository of Middlesex University research

Sinlultaneous vs sequential discriminations of Markov-generated stimuli 1

Information Presentation Constraints and the Adaptive Decision Maker Hypothesis

Gut Feelings: Short Cuts To Better Decision Making Gerd Gigerenzer

PSYCHOLOGY : JUDGMENT AND DECISION MAKING

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

Happiness Versus Sadness as a Determinant of Thought Confidence in Persuasion: A Self-Validation Analysis

Examples of Feedback Comments: How to use them to improve your report writing. Example 1: Compare and contrast

The Heart Wants What It Wants: Effects of Desirability and Body Part Salience on Distance Perceptions

CONFIDENCE VIA CORRECTION. ESMT Working Paper THE EFFECT OF JUDGMENT CORRECTION ON CONSUMER CONFIDENCE

REPETITION, ORDER OF PRESENTATION, AND TIMING OF ARGUMENTS AND MEASURES AS DETERMINANTS OF OPINION CHANGE l

PSYC2600 Lecture One Attitudes

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

The Relationship between Consumer Elaboration and Decision Making

STOCHASTIC CHOICE HEURISTICS *

Thinking about product attributes: Investigating the role of unconscious valence processing in attribute framing

Supplementary Material. Perceived Agreeableness. We also conducted exploratory analyses on potential

Anxiety Studies Division Annual Newsletter

Cooperation in Risky Environments: Decisions from Experience in a Stochastic Social Dilemma

Supporting Information

The notion that people sometimes base judgments on. Ease of Retrieval Effects in Persuasion: A Self-Validation Analysis

It is Whether You Win or Lose: The Importance of the Overall Probabilities of Winning or Losing in Risky Choice

How would you evaluate an individual who argues that

Assignment 2: Experimental Design

Gender Differences Associated With Memory Recall. By Lee Morgan Gunn. Oxford May 2014

ENVIRONMENTAL VALUES EXECUTIVE SUMMARY

Healthy choices in context: How contextual cues can influence the persuasiveness of framed health messages

SOURCE CREDIBILITY AND THE EFFECTIVENESS OF FIREWISE INFORMATION

Introduction to Training Beginning Athletes. Beginning Athletes 19/04/2018. Fundamental Movement Skills for Beginning Athletes. Training Progressions

Non-smokers rights movement begins Per capita cigarette consumption First medical reports linking smoking and cancer.

Deception Detection Accuracy Using Verbal or Nonverbal Cues

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Decisions based on verbal probabilities: Decision bias or decision by belief sampling?

Effect of Choice Set on Valuation of Risky Prospects

Comparative Ignorance and the Ellsberg Paradox

Automatic activation of attribute knowledge in heuristic inference from memory

Transcription:

Japanese Psychological Rescarch 1994, Vol.36, No. 1. 41-48 Special lssue: Decision Making Personal involvement, task complexity, and the decision making process: An information search analysis' KAZUHISA TAKEMURA' Institute of Socio-Economic Planning, University of Tsukuba, Tsukuba, Ibaraki 305 The purpose of this study was to examine the influence of personal involvement and task complexity on the decision making process. Seventy-four female university students were randomly assigned to one cell of a 2 (personal involvement: low or high) x 3 (task comlexity: 2, 6, or 10 alternatives) design. The main results were as follows, (I) The subjects spent a greater amount of time reaching decisions in which they had greater involvement. (2) The subjects inquired about a greater amount of information during the decision task in which they had greater involvement. (3) The subjects more often returned to the information that they had already inquired about during the decision task in which they had greater involvement. (4) An interaction effect between involvement and task complexity was observed. That is, the effect of involvement on decision making process was largest when task complexity was moderate. Key words: personal involvement, task complexity, decision making, choice process, information search, decision strategies. Investigating psychological factors influencing decision making process is one of the main problems in the research on human decision making. Despite growing evidence that personal involvement can play an important role in the way people interpret and evaluate social information (Borgida & Howard-Pitney, 1983; Chaiken, 1980; Harkness, DeBono. & Borgida, 1985; Petty & Cacioppo, I990; Petty, Cacioppo, & Goldman, 1981), little work has been done on the effect of personal involvement on individual decision making. This study assessed the influence of personal involvement on the decision making process on the decision were varied. tasks where the task complexities Recent work on social cognition suggests that involved subjects pay more attention to the content of information presented, process this information more systematically, and are less likely to rely solely on simple heuristics (Borgida & Howard-Pitney, 1983; Chaiken, 1980; Harkness et ' The earlier version of this paper was presented at 24th International Congress of Psychology, Sydney, August 1988. ' The author is grateful to Professor S. Kojima of Aichigakuin University and Professor 0. Takagi of Kansai University for their helpful advice during the study. al.. 1985; Petty & Cacioppo, 1990; Cacioppo & Goldman, 1981). However, no previous research has demonstrated this link between involvement and the decision making process. For example, although Harkness et al. (1985) identified the judgmental strategies by using covariation judgment tasks, they did not trace the decision making process. In contrast, the present study used a process tracing technique which should provide valuable insights into the decision making process which led a subject to exhibit a particular choice. The process tracing technique involves presenting subjects with decision tasks where they have to inquire about information about the alternatives available. Examination of the information search pattern should provide a method for discriminating between alternative models of decision making in terms of the information processing behavior assumed to underlie the various models of decision strategies (Payne, 1976; Payne, Braunstein, & Carroll, 1978; Takemura & Takagi, 1985, 1987). The studies using the process tracing technique showed that people used a variety of decision strategies for making choices and the selection of these strategies was largely contingent on task complexity (Abelson & Levi, 1985; Olshaysky, 1979; Payne, 1976, 1982; Payne & Braunstein, 1978; Takemura, 1988, 1993). In most

42 K. Takemura cases, the task complexity has been manipulated through variations in the number of alternatives in the choice set. Previous studies indicate that decision strategies were sensitive to the number of alternatives, and that information acquisition patterns of the strategies became more attribute based as the number of alternatives (degree of task complexity) increased (Payne, 1976; Payne & Braunstein, 1978; Takemura, 1993). As Branscome and Cohen (1991) suggested, task complexity would be an important condition influencing involvement effects on decision making. That is, the interaction effect between involvement and task complexity would be expected. Thus, the present study was also designed to examine the joint effects of involvement and task complexity which was manipulated by three variations in the number of alternatives (two, six, and ten alternative conditions). In the present study, "outcome dependency" which was provided by Berscheid, Graziano, Monson, and Dermer (1976) was utilized as the way of manipulating the degree of involvement in the decision task. In the high-involvement condition, before the decision task, each subject was asked to meet the person whom she chose as a dating partner. On the other hand, in the low-involvement condition, each subject was not asked to meet the person. This manipulation would he expected to control some perceived degree of psychological control over their outcomes (e.g., the amount of fun, the level of boredom, degree to which they feel defensive, and the sexual opportunities ). As a result, meeting-condition subjects should experience greater involvement than should non-meeting-condition subjects. On the basis of the two major conditions, the following hypotheses were proposed: 1. Subjects in the high-involvement condition should spend a greater amount of time during the decision task than subjects in the low-involvement condition. 2. Subjects should inquire about a greater amount of information and the ratio of inquired information to the total available information should be higher during the decision task in which they have greater involvement. 3. Subjects should more likely to return information they had already inquired about in which they have greater involvement. 4. Subjects in the high-involvement condition should more often use an alternative-wise information search than subjects in the lowinvolvement condition. On the other hand, subjects in the low-involvement condition should more likely use an attribute-wise information search than subjects in the high- involvement condition. 5. An interaction effect between involvement and task complexity should be observed. That is, the effect of involvement should be largest when the degree of task complexity for processing information. is adequate In the present study, the six-alternative condition should create the largest effect of involvement, because six is the closest number of the Magical Number Seven (Miller, 1956), which indicates the maximum capacity of human information processing. Design Method A 2 x 3 experimental design was used. Our experiment involved two major involvement conditions: meeting condition (high-involvement condition) and non-meeting condition (low- involvement condition). These two major conditions were crossed with three conditions of task complexity: a two-alternative condition, a six-alternative condition, and a ten alternative condition. Subjects Seventy-four female Doshisha University undergraduates who enrolled in the introductory psychology course participated in the experiment. They were recruited on a voluntary basis and were given extra course credit for their participation. These subjects were randomly assigned to one of the six conditions that we created by crossing the two involvement conditions with the three conditions of task complexity (number of alternatives). Stimuli A decision task for a subject involved a number of alternatives, either two, six, and ten. The stimuli were information matrices representing different potential dating partners. Each row of the matrix represented one attribute of potential dating partners, and each column was labeled with a letter representing a person (e.g., Person A). One

Involvement, task complexity, and the decision making 43 example of the material used in this study is shown in Figure I. Each cell of the matrix represents the actual informational value for the person (alternative (-attribute combination and was labeled with a letter and a numeral. For example. "A2" represents "height of person A". Attributes were chosen on the basis of an exploratory study involving 98 undergraduates. The ten attributes most frequently listed as important in selecting a dating partner were used in the present study. These were ( I) facial attractiveness, (2) height, (3) body form, (4) sympathy, (5) cheerfulness, (6) decisive character, (7) firm attitude, (8) punctuality. (9) hobby, and (101 possibility of acceptance. With the exception of "hobby", there were five values for each attribute: well above average, above average, average, below average, well below average. For the hobby attribute, the following categories were used: Plays musical instruments, plays sports, studies, and does nothing. The values of the alternatives of the various attributes were chosen such that no alternative would completely dominate the other remaining attributes. The values of each attribute were selected so that each alternative would have both good and poor qualities, a priori. Procedure Each subject was run individually through an experimental session. The subjects were told that they would be presented with a number of alternatives to choose from and a certain amount of information about each alternative. They were told that Figure I. An example of an information matrix representing different potential dating partners. each alternative represented a potential dating partner and that they should choose the person they would prefer as a dating partner for themselves on the basis of the information provided about each person. In the high-involvement condition, the subjects were given these instructions: "This is a dating study. If you agree to participate, you will meet, at least one time, the person from our dating pool that you have chosen during the experiment. Our interest is in how judgments change, how our feelings change about people from the time we first hear about a person to the time when we actually meet the person. That is the basic idea behind this study. If you agree, please sign the consent form." As a result, all the subjects in this condition signed it. On the other hand, in the low-involvement condition, the subjects were not asked to meet the person they chose during the experiment. Subjects were instructed to ask the experimenter one piece of information at a time. They were told they were free to ask as little or as much information as they wanted to or felt was necessary to make the decision. No time constraints were placed on the subject. They were instructed to work at their own pace and that they should have plenty of time to finish. Responding to the subjects' request for information, the experimenter sequentially gave each subject information about each value of the cell from a part of the experiment room, which was screened off from subject's view by a screen. Sessions were timed and taperecorded. Debriefing Because of the deceptive nature of the involvement manipulation, an extensive and carefully designed debriefing session concluded the procedure. Care was taken to create a friendly and informal atmosphere in which the aims and rationale for the study were fully explained. All subjects understood and accepted the rationale of the procedure, and we found no evidence of any residual negative effects. Analysis All analyses of variance reported below were performed using a 2 x 3 design. The independent variables were: two levels of involvement (low and high levels), the number of alternatives (two, six, and ten). In all analyses, the two factors were

44 K. Takemura treated as between-group factors. In order to meet the formal requirements for the analyses of variances, raw proportions were subjected to an arcsine transformation. Decision Time Results and Discussion Figure 2 presents the mean time required to make the decision for each condition. A two-way analysis of variance (that is, involvement and number of alternatives) was conducted. The main effect of involvement was highly significant, F(1, 66)=13.31, p<. 001. As predicted in Hypothesis I, subjects in the high-involvement condition spent a greater amount of time at the decision task than subjects in the low-involvement condition. Furthermore, the main effect of the number of alternatives was highly significant, Ft2, 66)= 9.48, p<. 001. The multiple comparison test for differences between the number of alternatives revealed that subjects in the six and ten alternative conditions spent a greater amount of time during the decision task than subjects in the twoalternative condition. Although all the main effects were significant, the interaction was not significant. Amount of Information Searched The search data for each subject was primarily organized in terms of the amount of available information inquired about. Figure 3 presents the mean pieces of information examined in the decision making process for each condition. A two- way analysis of variance was conducted. The main effect of involvement was significant, F(1, 66)=6.85, p<. 05. As predicted in Hypothesis 2, subjects in the high-involvement condition inquired about a greater amount of information during the decision task than subjects in the lowinvolvement condition, Furthermore, the main effect of the number of alternatives was highly significant, F(2, 66)=14.84, p<. 001. The multiple comparison test for differences between the number of alternatives revealed that subjects in the six and ten alternative conditions inquired about a greater amount of information during the decision task than subjects in the two-alternative condition, In addition, the interaction was marginally significant, F(2, 66)=2.98, p<. 06. The interaction implied that involvement had a strong effect on the amount of information inquired only in the six-alternative condition. This result was suggested by Hypothesis 5. Ratio of Information Used Figure 4 presents the mean percentage of available information inquired about in the decision making process for each condition. This measure was computed in order to examine the relative number of pieces of information inquired during the decision task with different number of alternatives. conducted. A two-way analysis of variance was The main effect of involvement was significant, R I, 66)=8.50, p<. 01. As predicted in Hypothesis 2, the high-involvement condition produced a greater ratio of information used than the low-involvement condition. Furthermore, the main Figure 2. Mean time required to make a decision for each codition. Figure 3. Mean amout of information inquired about for each condition.

Involvement, task complexity, and the decision making 45 effect of the number of alternatives was highly significant, F(2, 66)=8. 56, p<. 001. The multiple comparison test for differences between the number of alternatives revealed that the number of alternatives condition which encouraged the use of the greatest ratio of the information used to information available was the two-alternative condition. This result is compatible with those reported by Payne (1976). In addition, the interaction was significant, F(2, 66)=3.39, p<. 05. The interaction revealed that involvement had a strong effect on the ratio of information used only in the six-alternative condition. This result supported Hypothesis 5. Rechecking Frequencies Figure 5 presents the mean rechecking fre- Figure 4. Mean ratio of information inquired about for each condition. quencies, that is, amount of reconsidered information for each condition. A two-way analysis of variance was conducted. The main effect of involvement was significant, 66)=6.18, p<. 05. As predicted in Hypothesis 3, subjects more often returned to information they had already inquired about in which they had greater involvement. Furthermore, the main effect of the number of alternatives was significant, F(2, 66)=5. 70, p<. 01. The multiple comparison test for differences between the number of alternatives revealed that subjects in the six and ten alternative conditions reconsidered a greater amount of information during the decision task than subjects in the two-alternative condition. In addition, the interaction was marginally significant, F(2, 66)=2.65, p<. 08. The interaction revealed that involvement had a strong effect on the rechecking frequency only in the six-alternative condition. This was suggested by Hypothesis 5. Ratio of Rechecking In order to examine the ratio of rechecking during the decision task with different numbers of alternatives, the relative number of pieces of information inquired about more than once (That is, number of pieces of information picked at leasttwice divided by the total number of pieces investigated) was computed, arcsine-transformed and served as a dependent measure in the analysis. Figure 6 presents mean the percentage of rechecking for each condition. A two-way analysis of variance was conducted. The main effect of each Figure 5. Mean frequencies of rechecking information for condition. Figure 6. Mean ratio of rechecking information for each condition.

46 K Takernura involvement was significant, F(I, 66)=8, 55, p<. 01. As predicted in Hypothesis 3, the highinvolvement condition produced a greater ratio of rechecking than the low-involvement condition. Furthermore, the main effect of the number of alternatives was marginally significant, I; (2, 66)=2.47, p<. 10. The multiple comparison test for differences between the number of alternatives suggested that the ten-alternative condition encouraged the use of the greatest proportion of rechecking. In addition, the interaction was marginally significant, F(2, 66)=2.52, p<. 09. The interaction in dicated that involvement had a strong effect on the ratio of rechecking only in the six-alternatives condition. Hypothesis 5. Patterns of information Search This was suggested by The search data for each subject was organized in terms of the patterns of information search. According to the method provided by Payne (1976), the pattern of search was determined, in part, by examining the alternative and attribute associated with the nth + I piece of information inquired about by a subject as a function at the alternative and attribute associated with the nth piece of information searched. If the nth + I piece of information searched was within the same alternative but involved a different attribute, then that constituted an instance of the pattern of alternative-wise processing. On the other hand, if the nth + I piece of information searched was neither the same attribute, but a different alternative, then that constituted an instance of the pattern of attributewise processing. A measure of alternative processing was given by the number of alternative single step transitions divided by the amount of information searched, and a measure of attribute-wise processing was given by the number of attributive single step transitions divided by the amount of information searched. First, a two-way analysis of variance for the measure of alternative processing was conducted. The main effect of involvement was not significant. This result does not support Hypothesis 4. However, the main effect of the number of alternatives was significant, F(2, 66)= 3.29, p<. 05. The multiple comparison test for differences between the number of alternatives revealed that subjects in the six-alternative condition more often used the alternative-wise processing strategy during the decision task than subjects in the two-alternative condition. Second, a twoway analysis of variance for the measure of alternative-wise processing was conducted. All the main effects were not significant. not support Hypothesis 4. General Discussion This result does The main purpose of the present study was to examine the effects of personal involvement and task complexity on decision making process, using a method of monitoring information search. First, as predicted in Hypothesis I and Hypothesis 2, subjects in the high-involvement condition spent a greater amount of time inquiring about the information and inquired about more of the information during the decision task than subjects in the lowinvolvement condition. These results suggested that decision makers who experienced higher levels of involvement use more complex and compensatory decision strategies. The significant interaction between involvement and task complexity indicated that involvement had a strong effect on the amount of information inquired about only in the six-alternative condition. This result, which was consistent with Hypothesis.5, might be due to limited information capacity of humans proposed by Miller (1956) and Simon (1955, 1957). In the two-alternative condition, there were small differences between the high and low involvement condition, because the task with two alternatives required less information processing activity. In the six-alternative condition, there were large differences between the high and low involvement condition, because the task with six alternatives required more information processing activity within the limits of processing capacity. In the ten-alternative condition, there were small differences between high and low involvement condition, because the task with six alternatives required more information processing activity beyond the limits of processing capacity. Second, as predicted in Hypothesis 3, the high-involvement condition produced a greater frequency and ratio of rechecked information than the low-involvement condition. This result suggested that involved subjects pay more attention to

Involvement, task complexity, and the decision making 47 the content of information presented, process this information more systematically, and are less likely to rely on simple heuristics. This finding is compatible with those reported on attitude and judgment research (Borgida & Howard-Pitney, 1983; Chaiken, 1980; Harkness et al., 1985; Petty & Cacioppo, 1990; Petty, Cacioppo, & Goldman, 19811. Furthermore, the marginally significant interaction between involvement and number of alternatives indicated that involvement had the strongest effect on frequency and ratio of rechecked information in the six-alternative condition. Although this interaction effect was only marginally significant, the tendency supported Hypothesis 5. Third, the results of the information search pattern did not support Hypothesis 4, that subjects in the high-involvement condition should use alternative-wise search more likely and attributewise search less likely than subjects in the lowinvolvement condition. This is incompatible with the finding by Harkness et al. (1985) that the selection of judgmental strategies were largely contingent on personal involvement. The result of the present study suggested that the direction of information search was not influenced by involvement. Last, in the present study, the subjects were presented only ten attributes of potential dating partners in stead of using attributes naturally. More studies that investigate the effects of involvement on decision making are needed. Some of these will require liberation from laboratory conditions in which only a relatively limited set of decision problems have been used. Despite these limitations, this study provides data that point to the importance of involvement and task complexity on the decision making process in providing a more complete account of individual decision making process. References Abelson, R. P., & Levi, A. 1985 Decision making and decision theory. In G. Lindzey & E. Aronson (Eds. ), The handbook of social psychology. 3rd ed. Vol.1. New York: Random House. Pp.231-309. Berscheid, E., Granziano,W., Monson, T., & Dermer, M. 1976 Outcome dependency: Attention, nunhut ion, and attraction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 34, 978-989. Borgida, E., & Howard-Pitney, B. 1983 Personal involvement and the robustness of perceptual salience effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 560-570. Branscombe, N. R., & Cohen, B. M. 1991 Motivation and complexity levels as determinants of heuristic use in social judgment. In J. P. Forgas (Ed.), Emotion and social judgment. Oxford: Pergamon Press. Pp.145-160. Chaiken, S. 1980 Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 752-766. Harkness, A. R., DeBono, K. G., & Borgida, U. 1985 Personal involvement and strategies for making contingency judgments: A stakes in the dating game makes a difference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 23-32. Miller, G. A. 1956 The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychological Review, 63, 81-97. Olshaysky, R. W. 1979 Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: A Replication and extension. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 24, 300-316. Payne, J. W. 1976 Task complexity and contingent processing in decision making: An information search and protocol analysis. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 16, 366-387. Payne, j. W. 1982 Contingent decision behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 92, 382-402. Payne, J. W., & Braunstein, M. L. 1978 Risky choice: An examination of information a quisition behavior. Memory & Cognition, 5, 554-561. Payne, J. W., Braunstein, M. L, & Carroll, J. S. 1978 Exploring pre-decisional behavior: An alternative approach to decision research. Organizational Behavior and Human Performance, 22, 17-44. Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. 1990 Involvement and persuasion: Tradition versus integration. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 367-374. Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Goldman, R. 1981 Personal involvement as a determinant argument based persuation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 847-855. Simon, H. A. 1955 A behavioral model of rational choice. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69, 99-118. Simon, H. A. 1957 Administrative behavior: A study

K. Takernura of decision-making process in administrative organization. 2nd ed. New York: Macmillan. Takemura, K. 1988 The effects of decision strategies on decision making process: A case of consumer behavior. Japanese Journal of Psychology, 59, 83-90. In Japanese with English abstract) Takemura, K. 1993 Protocol analysis of multistage decision strategies. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 77,459-469. Takemura, K., & Takagi, 0. 1985 Analysis of decision making process underlying prosocial behavior. Research in Social Psychology, 10, 35-44. (In Japanese with English abstract) Takemura, K., & Takagi, 0. 1987 Information search strategies and inner states in decision making process: A case of donating behavior. Japanese Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 26, 105-114. (In Japanese with English abstract) (Received Nov.6, 1993; accepted Jan. 8, 1994)