CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION BELIEFS AMONG SCHOOL STUDENTS IN PAKISTAN

Similar documents
Causal Attribution Beliefs of Success and Failure: A Perspective from Pakistan

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

WHY? A QUESTION BEHIND EVERY OUTCOME FINALLY ANSWERED

Why Do You Study? Complex Answers to a Simple Question. Mimi Bong. Department of Education Brain & Motivation Research Institute Korea University

International Core Journal of Engineering Vol.3 No ISSN:

Gender Differences in Locus of Control

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE COLLEGE AMONG TEACHERS IN COIMBATORE

Locus of Control in Relation to Academic Achievement of College Students in Meghalaya

ELEMENTARY TEACHERS SCIENCE SELF EFFICACY BELIEFS IN THE EAST AZERBAIJAN PROVINCE OF IRAN

An in-depth analysis of the relationship between age and attribution in EFL contexts

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSION

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model

Pearson Education Limited Edinburgh Gate Harlow Essex CM20 2JE England and Associated Companies throughout the world

Keywords: positive attribution, negative attribution, English learning, gender. Introduction

Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers

Self-Concept By Saul McLeod 2008

WHAT IS SELF? MODULE-IV OBJECTIVES 16.1 CONCEPT OF SELF. What is Self? Self and Personality. Notes

ISSN X Journal of Educational and Social Research Vol. 2 (8) October 2012

MALE AND FEMALE LEADERSHIP SIMILARITIES AND DIFFERENCES

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

Between intrinsic and extrinsic motivation: Examination of reasons for academic study based on the theory of internalization

The Controllability Beliefs Scale used with carers of people with intellectual disabilities: psychometric propertiesjir_

The Relationship between Happiness and Academic Achievements

Self-Handicapping Variables and Students' Performance

The Effects of Positivity on Health. Abstract. The purpose of this research is to investigate the benefits of positive thinking and how it can

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Perceived Control and Failure Preoccupation in Academic Computing: Effects on Cognitions, Emotions, and Achievement Outcomes

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Measure of Social Presence

Can trait Emotional Intelligence. predict differences in attainment and progress. in secondary school?

Title: The Relationship between Locus of Control and Academic Level and Sex of Secondary School Students

CHAPTER 3 METHODOLOGY, DATA COLLECTION AND DATA ANALYSIS

ANALYSIS OF PROCRASTINATION BEHAVIOUR AMONG TEACHERS: A NON-PARAMETRIC APPROACH

Emotion in Education: A Timely and Thorough Compilation

Motivation CURRENT MOTIVATION CONSTRUCTS

Well-Being of Secondary School Teachers In Relation To Their Organisational Climate

The Relationship of Mental Pressure with Optimism and Academic Achievement Motivation among Second Grade Male High School Students

Resilient Optimism R

References. Note: Image credits are in the slide notes

Locus of Control among University teachers: an Empirical Study

LEARNING. Learning. Type of Learning Experiences Related Factors

Everything DiSC Manual

Supplementary material. Cross-Cultural Differences and Similarities in Proneness to Shame: An Adaptationist and Ecological Approach

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Are dimensions of psycho-social well-being different among Latvian and Romanian University students?

Personality. Chapter 13

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site

Gender Differences in Prosocial Behaviour

ATTRIBUTION THEORY. Attribution - to explain by indicating a cause

Perceived parental attachment and achievement motivation

Study the Rate of Practice of Religious Beliefs and Duties in Undergraduate Students of Hormozgan University

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 191 ( 2015 ) WCES 2014

Causal attributions for success or failure by passing and failing students in college algebra

Graeme George. Villanova College, Brisbane RPforSchools.net

A General Framework for Personality Psychology

The Multifactor Leadership Questionnaire (MLQ) measures a broad range of leadership types from passive leaders, to leaders who give contingent rewards

Mental Health and Adjustment of Prospective Secondary Education Teachers

International Journal of Recent Advances in Multidisciplinary Research Vol. 02, Issue 12, pp , December, 2015

MOTIVATION OF FEMALE SECONDARY STUDENTS TOWARDS PARTICIPATION IN PHYSICAL ACTIVITIES IN HONG KONG

Multiple Act criterion:

The Relationship between Intrinsic Motivation and Happiness with Academic Achievement in High School Students

The Influence of Hedonic versus Utilitarian Consumption Goals on the Compromise Effect. Abstract

Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale. Interpretive Report. Paul L. Hewitt, Ph.D. & Gordon L. Flett, Ph.D.

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Social Presence Measure

Investigating the Appraisal Patterns of Regret and Disappointment 1

Students Personality Development Paradigm: Public versus Private

WITRIANI SOEGIJARDJO 1 WISMANINGSIH SUDRADJAT SUDARMO WIYONO FACULTY OF PSYCHOLOGY UNIVERSITAS PADJADJARAN BANDUNG INDONESIA 2007

A Study on Emotional Intelligence among Teachers with Special reference to Erode District

Leading with Emotional Intelligence. Courtney Holladay, PhD Executive Director

The Cambridge Framework for Life Competencies. Emotional Development. Introductory Guide for Teachers and Educational Managers

A Meaning-Centered Approach to Positive Education. Paul T. P. Wong

A Study of Life Satisfaction and Optimism in Relation to Psychological Well-Being Among Working and Non Working Women

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE QUESTIONNAIRE

relationships (i.e., attribution schemal). The I-E Scale Peterborough, Ontario, Canada ROTTER S (1968) Internal-External Locus of Control (I-E) Scale

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

Funnelling Used to describe a process of narrowing down of focus within a literature review. So, the writer begins with a broad discussion providing b

MEASUREMENT, SCALING AND SAMPLING. Variables

INDIAN JOURNAL OF PHYSICAL EDUCATION, SPORTS AND APPLIED SCIENCE, VOL.7, NO.4,October, 2017

COGNITIVE STYLE AND BUSINESS POSTGRADUATES IN TURKEY: PRELIMINARY FINDINGS

Emotional Intelligence: A Historical Overview. and strategies for the purpose of improving policymakers perceptions and behavior,

JOB INVOLMENT AMONG LIBRARY PROFESSIONALS WORKING IN ACADEMIC INSTITUTIONS IN CHENNAI

Bouncing back from setbacks

THE IMPACT OF STRESS LEVEL AND THEIR COPING STRATEGIES: A COMPARATIVE STUDY ON FEMALE COLLEGE TEACHERS AND FEMALE HOME MAKERS

Self Concept and Values of Male and Female Employees Working in Banking Sector

The Power to Change Your Life: Ten Keys to Resilient Living Robert Brooks, Ph.D.

PSYCHOLOGY (413) Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D.

Final Exam: PSYC 300. Multiple Choice Items (1 point each)

The Grateful Disposition: Links to Patterns of Attribution for Positive Events. Sharon L. Brion. Michael E. McCullough. Southern Methodist University

The role of theory in construction management: a call for debate

SECONDARY SCHOOL STUDENTS TEST ANXIETY AND ACHIEVEMENT IN ENGLISH

Public Attitudes toward Nuclear Power

Understanding of Sexual Harassment at the Workplace

Psycho Socio Variations in Male and Female Student Teachers with Internal and External Behavioural Control

Bahria Journal of Professional Psychology, January 2014, Vol-13, 1, 44-63

Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and Autonomy

The Youth Experience Survey 2.0: Instrument Revisions and Validity Testing* David M. Hansen 1 University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign

Feelings. Subjective experience Phenomenological awareness Cognitive interpretation. Sense of purpose

Aspects of emotion. Motivation & Emotion. Aspects of emotion. Review of previous lecture: Perennial questions about emotion

KOM 5113: Communication Research Methods (First Face-2-Face Meeting)

Principles of Sociology

Transcription:

CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION BELIEFS AMONG SCHOOL STUDENTS IN PAKISTAN Muhammad Faisal Farid, PhD Scholar (Corresponding author) Institute of Education and Research, University of the Punjab, Lahore, Pakistan Abstract Prof. Dr. Hafiz Muhammad Iqbal, Institute of Education and Research, University of the Punjab, Lahore, Pakistan A self-reporting instrument was constructed to measure causal explanations of the educational outcomes in the secondary student population. The instrument measured 8 types of beliefs: ability, effort, luck, task difficulty, strategy, interest, family influence and teacher influence. Secondary school students from large, public schools completed the instrument. The sample comprised of 396 students from selected districts of Punjab. Students confirmed all causes as potential causes of their success and failure. Parents and teachers were more a cause of success than failure. There was significant difference in causal attributional pattern of male and female students. Keywords: Causal attributions, success, failure, academic achievement 1. Introduction Causal attributions are the justifications provided by us to the events taking place in our lives especially when outcomes are unsatisfactory to our hopes. We give self justifications to maintain our ego and esteem levels by providing these explanations. In almost all settings and spheres of life this process works in the same way but when it comes to competitive world, this process becomes more critical. In the field of education, where success and failure are two possible outcomes for any learner the conceptualization of causes of success and failure becomes most important matter as he/she has to attribute it with some possible factors like ability, effort or environment. Weiner (1976) provided four factors to explain these attributions like ability, effort, task difficulty or luck. People mostly attribute success to their own effort or ability and failure is linked to luck or some other environmental factors (Hunter & Barker, 1987). Causal attribution has been studied in various countries among students of different levels for measuring their beliefs about successes and failures. It has a rich legacy that started with four prominent causes and is still a growing empirical construct for researchers (Boruchovitch, 2004; Forsyth, Story, Kelley & McMillan, 2009; Gipps & Tunstall 1998; Hui, 2001; Hovemyr, 1998; Lei, 2009; Nenty, 2010). Weiner s (1976) attributional model of achievement motivation and emotion dominated research in the field of social and educational psychology (McAuley, Duncan, Russell, 1992). For Weiner (2008), attribution inquiry is still strong enough to attract attention of the researchers as students still react when they hear about their grades in a classroom test or in a paper. Explaining motivation from an attribution perspective, Weiner (2005) describes that in achievement contexts the process of motivation begins with the exam outcome. If outcome is positive, student is happy and if outcome is negative then his frustration and sadness leads him to causal ascriptions. Examining emotional diversity in the classroom, Weiner (2007) enlist anxiety as a self-directed emotion. This is most concerning emotion for educational psychologists. Appraisals generates emotions like envy, scorn, admiration, anger, gratitude, guilt, indignation, jealously, regret, shame and sympathy. He suggests broadening the study of emotions in achievement contexts as emotions have their social context and functions too that need to be addressed. COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 411

Various methods are employed by researchers to assess the attributions of students. Proudfoot, Corr, Guest and Gray (2001) explain various methods to measure causal attributions and attributional style. They divided these under four broad categories. First category contains various scales that require subjects to choose from a list of given attributions like ability, effort, luck etc. In the second set of strategies, people freely describe the causes of specific out come and it s up to researcher to rate attributions. Third is content analytical procedure, which is used to map out causal attributions from already existing written materials like diaries, articles etc. The fourth method asks respondents to rate their personal or hypothetical attributions. The development of causal dimension scale (Russell, 1982) was one of the valid and reliable measures which provided researchers an open-ended causal attribution for achievement. Russell (1992) removed the short comings in his CDS and provided CDS II. We find three major methodologies to measure causal attribution beliefs. Vispoel & Austin (1995) listed them as situational, dispositional and critical incident methodologies. Explaining the short comings of situational and dispositional studies they argue that these types fail to assess real-life responses of the individual. While in critical incident methodology, respondents evaluate real events of success and failure. Interviews, open-ended questionnaires, forced-choice items questionnaires and rating scales are mostly used techniques in measuring attributions. In early works of Weiner (1976) we find him favoring traditional attributes of ability, effort, task difficulty and luck in academic related situations. While in his later works he described relationship between attributions, self-esteem and achievement. The attributional responses are categorized on the basis of bipolar dimensions (Vispoel & Austin, 1995) which are already suggested by Weiner (1976) and Russell (1982) as internal and external. We find three kinds of information that explain success and failure, i.e. locus, stability and controllability (Weiner, 1985). Locus is the location of a cause that whether it is internal or external. Internal factors may include ability or effort of a learner. According to Weiner (1976, 1985) ability and effort produces more impressive change as compared to luck or task difficulty. Pride comes out after success or victory; and humiliation or shame is the outcome after defeat or failure in any competitive task. There is a link between the attributions we make about success and failure and future behavior. Future expectations are based on stability of the cause. Some causes are stable over time while others are not (Bar-Tal, 1978). For example, luck is not stable and is subject to change over the period of time. Research on high achievers revealed that successful people make use of massive effort to be successful and they attribute their success to internal factors. The third dimension of attribution theory is controllability of the cause i.e. whether the cause is under control of the person or not. There are certain causes like effort and attention, which are under control of an individual and he can be held responsible for them. Whereas, some other causes like ability and luck are out of one s control and he cannot be answerable for them. Successful students believe in self-motivation, perseverance and effort. Their positive mindsets help them achieve success. Another quality of them is their approach towards mistakes. They take mistakes as learning opportunities that motivate them to keep on working rather than feel defeated and dejected (Goldstein & Brooks, 2007). Attribution research explains that different attribution patterns exist between successful students and unsuccessful students. We should not only understand the causes that are provided by students but also take notice of the process through which they pass during it. There exists a relationship between achievement and attribution style of the students. Cortes-Suarez & Sandiford (2008) describes passing and failing students attributions in college algebra through experimental design. By using CDS II, they found the attributes of passing and failing students. Attributional perspective has its roots in constructivism that propagates that individuals bring their own meanings to world and these meanings are personal to them. As a child whatever we learn, we are busy in constructing new knowledge. Our interpretations of our experiences influence the specific things we learn from those experiences (Ormrod, 1998) and that may be reflected in our subsequent behaviours and actions. This boosts motivation for future learning. Causal attribution beliefs differ between age groups, cultures and depend on whether the causal target is one s own self or someone else. The variation between them depends upon the situation. In Pakistan, research on this important aspect of learning is almost non-existing and there has been no instrument to measure causal attributions among students. The purpose of this study was to devise a questionnaire for secondary school students in Pakistan to measure the perceived causes of their success and failure. Data were collected from some schools of district Mianwali and district Bahawalnagar both from urban high schools and rural high schools. COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 412

2. Method & procedure 2.1Sample The sample comprised of 396 students from government sector schools of both urban and rural locale. These students were selected conveniently during the personal visit to these schools. There were 224 female students and 172 male students in the sample from two districts i.e. district Mianwali (159) and district Bahawalnagar (237). There were 108 arts group students and 288 science group students. The demographic variables information part of the questionnaire explained that students belonged to families having varied socio-economic status. 2.2 Development of the Instrument In the light of literature review, the instrument was based on taxonomy of 8 types of beliefs to investigate students attributions for success and failure in English and mathematics. These were ability, effort, task difficulty, luck, strategy, interest, family influence and teacher influence. The draft questionnaire was shared with Weiner, Russell and Forsyth (eminent researchers in the field of attribution theory) and they proposed changes in the questionnaire. Keeping in view their suggested changes, the questionnaire was finalized. We made all wording parallel and identical in the questionnaire. Our set of attribution measures was adequate to measure the basic theoretical causes of academic performances as identified in classic theories of attributions. We provided same number of items for each cause and for each outcome and for each subject (mathematics and English). We also phrased the same for each cause for mathematics and English. We grouped all causes and enlisted them under one stem like when I perform well/bad in mathematics/english in school is due to..in this way we had two situations each for mathematics and English as one for success and second for failure. The final questionnaire had two identical forms, i.e. one having success situations (success in mathematics and success in English) and other having failure situations (failure in mathematics and failure in English). 2.2.1 Reliability Statistics The CABS questionnaire has two identical forms with one having success attributions and other having failure attributions. Table 2 explains the reliability statistics of the questionnaire with its subscales. Table 2 shows that failure attribution form has a greater reliability α than success attribution form. Over all reliability co-efficient α is 0.823 of the questionnaire. 2.3 Procedure The head teachers of selected schools were contacted and after getting their consent, researcher visited respective classes to collect data from students in the presence of local teachers. Researcher explained the questionnaire to students and satisfied their quires before filling the causal attribution beliefs questionnaire (CABS). The students were given success situation form first. When they filled-in the form, it was collected back and failure situation form was distributed among them. A school period of 35 minutes was utilized in all this process. 3. Results Table 3 explains attribution scale means of the success scale and failure scale. Means are measured across the subject area i.e. mathematics and English According to rank order (based on total means), the students success attributions were arranged in descending order (from highest to lowest) as teacher influence (4.08), effort (3.99), strategy (3.97), interest (3.995), parent influence (3.90), ability (3.775), luck (3.63) and task difficulty (3.39). Similarly, descending rank order for failure attributions included effort (3.395), strategy (3.29), interest (3.275), task difficulty (3.155), luck (2.89), teacher influence (2.835), ability (2.805) and parent influence (2.795). These findings suggest that participants confirmed all attributions as perceived causes of their success and failure. The mean of success scale clearly describes that all attributions have greater mean score than 3 which suggest that these are possible causes of success of students. As far as failure scale is concerned, there is mixed response. The participants did not accept the same causes (as attributed for their success) being the possible cause of their failure. So, they were reluctant to attribute all causes as perceived causes of their failure. We studied attributions subject wise to get better idea about perceived causes of success and failure (table 4). Similar patterns of success attributions were found between male students and female students. Both male and female students attributed teacher influence, strategy, effort and interest as important causes of their success in mathematics. Interesting findings occurred in success attributions of English, where female students attributed teacher influence, effort, parent s influence, strategy and interest as leading causes of their success. Male COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 413

students quoted strategy, interest, teacher influence, effort and parent s influence as foremost causes of their success. Table 4 also describes failure attributions of students. Male students quoted interest, strategy, effort, task difficulty, parent s influence and luck as main causes of their failure in mathematics. Female students cited effort, interest, task difficulty, strategy, luck and ability as foremost causes of their failure in mathematics. It was quite interesting to report the failure attributions in English. Male students described their failure attributions in following rank order; interest, strategy, effort, task difficulty, luck, parent s influence, teacher influence and ability. Whereas, female students reported effort, strategy, task difficulty, interest, ability, teacher influence, luck and parent s influence as causes of their failure in English. We used independent sample t-test to measure significant difference in causal attributions of students on the basis of gender. Independent sample t-test was conducted to find out the mean difference in failure attributions of male & female students in mathematics. Surprisingly, male students endorsed some items more than others like strategy, interest, luck, task difficulty and parent s influence. The mean scores of male students on these attributes surpass mean scores of female students (table 5). Table 5 shows that there is statistically significant difference in students failure attributions in mathematics regarding strategy, interest, luck, parent s influence and teacher s influence. Significant difference in scores for strategy on males (M=3.48, SD=1.09) and female (M=3.11, SD=1.43); t (394) =2.973, p=0.003 *. Statistically significant difference in scores for interest on males (M=3.54, SD=1.19) and female (M=3.24, SD=1.35); t (394) =2.334, p=0.022 *. Similarly, significant difference in scores for luck on males (M=3.13, SD=1.38) and female (M=2.70, SD=1.40); t (394) =2.995, p=0.003 * ; significant difference in scores for parent s influence on males (M=3.24, SD=1.37) and female (M=2.46, SD=1.45); t (394) =5.393, p=0.000 *, and significant difference in scores for strategy on teacher influence (M=2.96, SD=1.30) and female (M=2.65, SD=1.56); t (394) =2.146, p=0.032 *. The remaining three failure attributions i.e. ability, effort and task difficulty have no statistically mean difference. Independent sample t-test was conducted to find out the mean difference in failure attributions of male & female students in English. Male students mean score is greater than female students in all failure attributions and except for ability attribution, it is greater than 3. This suggests that male students strongly approved every cause as a potential cause of their failure in English (table 6). Table 6 shows that there is statistically significant difference in students failure attributions in English regarding strategy, interest, luck and parent s influence. The other four failure attributions i.e. ability, effort, task difficulty and teacher s influence show no significant difference. Statistically, significant difference in scores for strategy on males (M=3.56, SD=1.05) and female (M=3.13, SD=1.44); t (394) =3.463, p=0.001 *. Similarly, significant difference in scores for interest on males (M=3.66, SD=1.18) and female (M=2.82, SD=1.44); t (394) =6.397, p=0.000 *. Statically significant difference in scores for luck on males (M=3.16, SD=1.38) and female (M=2.69, SD=1.33); t (394) =3.418, p=0.001 * ; and significant difference in scores for parent s influence on males (M=3.04, SD=1.32) and female (M=2.62, SD=1.49); t (394) =2.996, p=0.003 *. Independent sample t-test was conducted to find out the mean difference in success attributions of male & female students in mathematics. Surprisingly, female students mean score is greater than male students in every success attributions. Both male and female students strongly agreed upon every cause as a potential cause of their success in mathematics (table 7). Table 7 explains that only luck attribution has statistically significant difference in success attributions in mathematics. There is no significant mean difference in remaining seven attributions i.e. ability, effort, strategy, interest, task difficulty, parent s influence and teacher influence. Significant difference in scores for luck on males (M=3.51, SD=1.39) and female (M=3.79, SD=1.18); t (394) =2.097, p=0.037 *. Independent sample t-test was conducted to find out the mean difference in success attributions of male & female students in English. A quite similar pattern of success attributions was observed in English. Female students mean score was greater than male students in every success attributions. Both male and female students strongly recognized every cause as a potential cause of their success in English (table 8). Statistically significant difference was observed in luck attribution in mathematics while task difficulty and parent s influence have significant difference in English. Table 8 shows that there is statistically significant difference in students success attributions in English regarding task difficulty and parent s influence. Significant difference in scores for task difficulty on males (M=3.36, SD=1.31) and female (M=3.66, SD=1.36); t (394) =2.154, p=0.032 *. Similar significant difference in scores for parent s influence on males (M=3.75, SD=1.23) and female (M=4.08, SD=1.07); t (394) =2.775, p=0.006 *. The remaining six success attributions in English show no statically significant difference. COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 414

Data were collected on success attributions and failure attributions from same participants, so within sample (paired sample) t-test was conducted to compare subject-wise significant difference in causal attributions of students. A paired sample t-test was conducted to compare success attributions and failure attributions of male students in mathematics. Table 9 explains that there exists significant difference in all attributions. Success attributions have greater mean scores than failure attributions except for task difficulty where mean score of failure attribution is greater than mean score of success attribution (table 9). Male students strongly endorsed every success attribution as all success attributions have means scores greater than 3. While they rated effort, strategy, interest, luck, task difficulty and parent s influence as potential causes of failure (means scores of these six causes were greater than 3). Significant difference in scores for ability as success attribution (M=3.59, SD=1.377) and ability as failure attribution (M=2.94, SD=1.296); t (171) =-5.505, p=0.000 **. Similar significant difference in scores for effort as success attribution (M=3.97, SD=0.920) and effort as failure attribution (M=3.43, SD=1.266); t (171) =-5.138, p=0.000 ** ; significant difference in scores for strategy as success attribution (M=3.99, SD=1.005) and strategy as failure attribution (M=3.48, SD=1.094); t (171) =-5.231, p=0.000 **. Reported significant difference in scores for interest as success attribution (M=3.91, SD=1.164) and interest as failure attribution (M=3.54, SD=1.191); t (171) =-2.930, p=0.004 **. Significant difference in scores for luck as success attribution (M=3.51, SD=1.391) and luck as failure attribution (M=3.13, SD=1.384); t (171) =-2.519, p=0.013 *. Similar significant difference in scores for task difficulty as success attribution (M=3.12, SD=1.303) and task difficulty as failure attribution (M=3.31, SD=1.255); t (171) =1.572, p=0.018 *. In the same manner, significant difference in scores for parent s influence as success attribution (M=3.80, SD=1.166) and parent s influence as failure attribution (M=3.23, SD=1.379); t (171) =-3.989, p=0.000 ** ; and significant difference in scores for teacher s influence as success attribution (M=4.06, SD=0.980) and teacher s influence as failure attribution (M=2.96, SD=1.301); t (171) =-9.074, p=0.000 **. A paired sample t-test was conducted to compare success attributions and failure attributions of male students in English. Table 10 explains that there is no significant difference in task difficulty attributions for success and failure in English. The remaining seven attributions show significant difference in mean scores. Male students strongly endorsed every success attribution as possible cause of their success in English (M>3). Surprisingly, except for ability, they rated effort, strategy, interest, luck, task difficulty, parent s influence and teacher s influence (M>3) as perceived causes of their failure in English. We found an identical pattern of success and failure attributions in male students in mathematics and English. Interesting failure attribution pattern emerged in mathematics and English. Along with six common attributions, they added teacher s influence as potential cause of their failure in English. Significant difference in scores for ability as success attribution (M=3.71, SD=1.291) and ability as failure attribution (M=2.86, SD=1.390); t (171) =-6.585, p=0.000 **. Reported significant difference in scores for effort as success attribution (M=3.88, SD=1.158) and effort as failure attribution (M=3.37, SD=1.299); t (171) =- 4.188, p=0.000 **. Similar significant difference in scores for strategy as success attribution (M=3.98, SD=0.958) and strategy as failure attribution (M=3.56, SD=1.054); t (171) =-4.118, p=0.000 **. Significant difference in scores for interest as success attribution (M=3.97, SD=1.061) and interest as failure attribution (M=3.66, SD=1.189); t (171) =-2.652, p=0.009 **. Table 10 describes significant difference in scores for luck as success attribution (M=3.47, SD=1.290) and luck as failure attribution (M=3.16, SD=1.383); t (171) =-2.290, p=0.023 *. Similar significant difference in scores for parent s influence as success attribution (M=3.75, SD=1.237) and parent s influence as failure attribution (M=3.04, SD=1.323); t (171) =-5.348, p=0.000 **. In the same manner, significant difference in scores for teacher s influence as success attribution (M=3.97, SD=1.189) and teacher s influence as failure attribution (M=3.02, SD=1.317); t (171) =-7.265, p=0.000 **. A paired sample t-test was conducted to compare success attributions and failure attributions of female students in mathematics. Table 11 describes that except for task difficulty attribution there is statistically significant difference in remaining seven attributions of success and failure in mathematics. Female students strongly endorsed every success attribution (M>3). For female students effort, strategy, interest and task difficulty were the possible causes of their failure in mathematics (M>3). Reported significant difference in scores for ability as success attribution (M=3.82, SD=1.080) and ability as failure attribution (M=2.68, SD=1.375); t (223) =-9.913, p=0.000 **. Similar significant difference in scores for effort as success attribution (M=4.01, SD=0.970) and effort as failure attribution (M=3.49, SD=1.267); t (223) =-5.058, p=0.000 **. Statistical significant difference in scores for strategy as success attribution (M=3.99, SD=0.930) and strategy as failure attribution (M=3.11, SD=1.427); t (223) =-8.268, p=0.000 **. In the same manner, significant difference in scores for interest as COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 415

success attribution (M=4.01, SD=0.951) and interest as failure attribution (M=3.24, SD=1.1357); t (223) =- 6.784, p=0.000 **. In table 11, significant difference in scores for luck as success attribution (M=3.79, SD=1.180) and luck as failure attribution (M=2.70, SD=1.404); t (223) =-9.413, p=0.000 **. Reported significant difference in scores for parent s influence as success attribution (M=3.91, SD=1.215) and parent s influence as failure attribution (M=2.45, SD=1.457); t (223) =-11.710, p=0.000 **. Similar significant difference in scores for teacher s influence as success attribution (M=4.08, SD=1.096) and teacher s influence as failure attribution (M=2.65, SD=1.559); t (223) =-10.486, p=0.000 **. A paired sample t-test was conducted to compare success attributions and failure attributions of female students in English. Table 12 explains that there exists significant difference in all attributions. Female students endorsed every success item. For them, every factor is a potential cause of success (M>3). However, they endorsed some items more than others. Surprisingly, they rated effort and strategy (M>3) as potential causes of their failure in English. Reported significant difference in scores for ability as success attribution (M=3.92, SD=1.135) and ability as failure attribution (M=2.77, SD=1.347); t (223) =-9.980, p=0.000 **. Similar significant difference in scores for effort as success attribution (M=4.08, SD=0.985) and effort as failure attribution (M=3.28, SD=1.308); t (223) =-8.035, p=0.000 **. In the same manner, significant difference in scores for strategy as success attribution (M=3.94, SD=1.067) and strategy as failure attribution (M=3.12, SD=1.441); t (223) =- 7.223, p=0.000 **. Statistically significant difference in scores for interest as success attribution (M=3.93, SD=1.002) and interest as failure attribution (M=2.82, SD=1.443); t (223) =-9.625, p=0.000 **. Similar significant difference in scores for luck as success attribution (M=3.68, SD=1.144) and luck as failure attribution (M=2.69, SD=1.338); t (223) =-8.900, p=0.000 **. Reported significant difference in scores for task difficulty as success attribution (M=3.66, SD=1.369) and task difficulty as failure attribution (M=2.93, SD=1.563); t (223) =-5.726, p=0.000 *. In the similar manner, significant difference in scores for parent s influence as success attribution (M=4.08, SD=1.074) and parent s influence as failure attribution (M=2.62, SD=1.498); t (223) =-12.132, p=0.000 **. Similarly, significant difference in scores for teacher s influence as success attribution (M=4.17, SD=1.059) and teacher s influence as failure attribution (M=2.77, SD=1.595); t (223) =-10.468, p=0.000 **. 4. Discussion When students receive results from their teacher after a class test or an examination they give reaction. Good or bad feelings do emerge. Research studies do not agree on a single set of attributions rather they provide space to extract traditional as well as non traditional attributional categories (Nenty, 2010: Weiner, 2010: Forsyth, Story, Kelley & McMillan, 2009: Vispoel & Austin, 1995: Bar-Tal, 1978). Causes are of many types but importantly good causes increases the chances of success whereas bad causes increase the possibility of failure (Forsyth, Story, Kelley & McMillan, 2009). In this study, we provided with a list of attributions to students to check their perception about success and failure in the school subjects of Mathematics and English. We found similar patterns of success and failure attributions. Students documented their success attributions by quoting teacher influence, parent s influence, effort and strategy as prime causes of their success. This tells the importance of teacher and family in student s life. The students are still willing to give due credit to their teachers and parents/family in country like Pakistan where social realities are changing. Collectively, these students backed every item. For them, every factor is a potential cause of both success and failure. However, they endorsed some items more than others. The respondents endorsed all attributions for success but were reluctant to endorse all given attributions for their failure. Significant mean difference emerged in failure attributions of male students and female students in mathematics and English. Results are in consonance with Sweeney, Moreland & Gruber (1982). They found gender differences in performance attributions. A critical incident method was employed by Vispoel & Austin (1995) to study same list of attributions in junior high school students. We used a small questionnaire to study only 8 causal attributions for success and failure. More causal attributions can be studied by expanding the list of attributions. The sample size was also small. The limited sample can be increased and more students can be included for further investigation. Students from private sector can also be added to study their causal attribution beliefs about success and failure. COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 416

Acknowledgement We would like to thank B. Weiner, D. Russell and D. Forsyth for their guidance and support in finalizing research instrument used in this study. References Bar-Tal, D. (1978). Attributional analysis of achievement-related behavior. Review of Educational Research, 48, 259-271. Doi: 10.3102/00346543048002259. Boruchovitch, E. (2004). A study of causal attributions for success and failure in mathematics among Brazilian students. International Journal of Psychology, 38(1), 53-60. Cortes-Suarez, G., & Sandiford, J, R. (2008). Causal attributions for success or failure of students in college algebra. Community College Journal of Research and Practice, 32, 325-346. Doi: 10.1080/10668920701884414. Forsyth, D, R., Story, P, A., Kelley, K, N., &McMillan, J, H. (2009). What causes failures and success? Students perceptions of their academic outcomes. Social Psychology Education, 12: 157-174. doi: 10.1007/s11218-008-9078-7. Gipps, C., & Tunstall, P. (1998). Effort, ability and the teacher: young children s explanations for success and failure. Oxford Review of Education, 24(2), 149-165. Goldstein, S. & Brooks, R. B. (2007). Understanding and managing children s classroom behaviour (2 nd ed.). New Jersey: Wiley & Sons, Inc. Hovemyr, M. (1998). The attribution of success and failure as related to different patterns of religious orientation. International Journal of the Psychology of Religion, 8(2), 107-124. doi:10.1207/s15327582ijpr0802_4 Hui, E. K. P. (2001). Hong Kong students and teachers beliefs on students concerns and their causal explanation. Educational Research, 43(3), 279-284. doi: 10.1080/00131880110081044. Hunter, M. & Barker, G. (1987). If at first : attribution theory in the classroom, Educational Leadership, 50-53. Lei, C. (2009). On the causal attribution of academic achievement in college students. Asian Social Science, 5(8), 87-96. McAuley, E., Duncan, T. E., & Russell, D. W. (1992). Measuring causal attributions: The revised causal dimension scale (CDSII). Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 18(5), 555-573. Nenty, H, J. (2010). Analysis of some factors that influence causal attribution of mathematics performance among secondary school students in Lesotho. Journal of Social Science, 22(2), 93-99. Ormrod, J. E. (1998). Educational psychology: developing learners (2 nd ed.). New Jersey: Merrill/Prentice-Hall, Inc. Proudfoot, J, G., Corr, P. J., Guest, D. E., & Gray, J.A. (2001). The development and evaluation of a scale to measure occupational attributional style in the financial service sector. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 259-270. Russell, D. (1982). The causal dimension scale: A measure of how individuals perceive causes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1248-1257. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.42.6.1137. Sweeney, P. D., Moreland, R, L. & Gruber, K. L. (1982). Gender differences in performance attributions: students explanations for personal success or failure. Sex Roles, 8(4), 359-373. Vispoel, W.P., & Austin, J. R. (1995). Success and failure in junior high school: A critical incident approach to understanding students attributional beliefs. American Educational Research Journal, 32, 377-412. doi: 10.3102/00028312032002377. Weiner, B. (1976). An attributional approach for educational psychology. Review of Research in Education, 4, 179-209. doi: 10.3102/0091732X004001179. Weiner, B. (1985). An attributional theory of achievement motivation and emotion. Psychological Review, 92(4), 548-573. COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 417

Weiner, B. (2005). Motivation from an attribution perspective and the social psychology of perceived competence. In Elliot, A. J & Dweck, C. S (Eds.), Handbook of competence and motivation. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Weiner, B. (2007). Examining emotional diversity in the classroom: an attribution theorist considers the moral emotions. In Schutz, P. A & Pekrun, R (Eds.), Emotions in education. California, Academic Press. Weiner, B. (2008). Reflections on the history of attribution theory and research- people, personalities, publications, problems. Social Psychology, 39 (3), 151-156. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335.39.3.151. Weiner, B. (2010). The development of an attribution-based theory of motivation: A history of ideas. Educational Psychologist, 45 (1), 28-36.doi: 10.1080/0046152090343359 COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 418

Tables Table 1 Distribution of Participants by Gender and District District Gender Total Mianwali Female 22 Male 137 Bahawalnagar Female 202 Male 35 Total 396 Table2 Reliability Statistics of the Instrument N of Items Cronbach s Alpha (α) 16 (Success Attributions) 0.801 16 (Failure Attributions) 0.820 32 (Total) 0.823 Table 3 Attribution Scale Means and Standard Deviation by Subject Area and Outcome (Success & Failure) Success scale Mathematics English Total * M SD M SD M SD Ability 3.72 1.18 3.83 1.21 3.775 1.105 Effort 3.99 0.94 3.99 1.06 3.99 1.00 Strategy 3.99 0.96 3.95 1.02 3.97 0.99 Interest 3.97 1.05 3.94 1.02 3.955 1.035 Luck 3.67 1.28 3.59 1.21 3.63 1.245 Task difficulty 3.25 1.41 3.53 1.35 3.39 1.38 Parent influence 3.86 1.19 3.94 1.15 3.90 1.17 Teacher influence 4.08 1.04 4.08 1.12 4.08 1.08 Failure scale Ability 2.80 1.38 2.81 1.36 2.805 1.37 Effort 3.47 1.26 3.32 1.27 3.395 1.265 Strategy 3.27 1.31 3.31 1.30 3.29 1.305 Interest 3.37 1.29 3.18 1.40 3.275 1.345 Luck 2.89 1.41 2.89 1.37 2.89 1.39 Task difficulty 3.26 1.40 3.05 1.47 3.155 1.435 Parent influence 2.79 1.47 2.80 1.43 2.795 1.45 Teacher influence 2.79 1.45 2.88 1.48 2.835 1.465 Total * represents the mean attribution scale score across subject areas i.e. mathematics & English COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 419

Table 4 Means and Standard Deviations by Outcome (Success, Failure) in Subject Area of the Students by Gender Success Scale Mathematics English Male Female Male Female M SD M SD M SD M SD Ability 3.59 1.29 3.82 1.08 3.71 1.29 3.92 1.14 Effort 3.97 0.92 4.01 0.97 3.88 1.15 4.08 0.98 Strategy 3.99 1.01 3.99 0.93 3.98 0.95 3.94 1.06 Interest 3.91 1.16 4.01 0.95 3.97 1.06 3.93 1.01 Luck 3.52 1.39 3.79 1.18 3.47 1.29 3.68 1.14 Task difficulty 3.12 1.30 3.35 1.47 3.36 1.31 3.66 1.36 Parent influence 3.80 1.16 3.91 1.21 3.75 1.23 4.08 1.07 Teacher influence 4.06 0.98 4.08 1.09 3.97 1.18 4.17 1.05 Failure Scale Ability 2.95 1.37 2.68 1.37 2.86 1.39 2.77 1.34 Effort 3.44 1.26 3.49 1.26 3.37 1.23 3.28 1.31 Strategy 3.48 1.09 3.11 1.42 3.56 1.05 3.12 1.44 Interest 3.54 1.19 3.24 1.35 3.66 1.18 2.82 1.44 Luck 3.13 1.38 2.71 1.40 3.16 1.38 2.69 1.33 Task difficulty 3.32 1.25 3.21 1.51 3.21 1.34 2.93 1.56 Parent influence 3.24 1.38 2.45 1.46 3.04 1.32 2.62 1.49 Teacher influence 2.96 1.30 2.65 1.56 3.02 1.31 2.77 1.59 Table 5 Comparison of Failure Attributions of Male & Female Students in Mathematics Attributions Gender N M SD df t P Ability Female 224 2.69 1.37 394 1.864 0.063 Male 172 2.94 1.37 Effort Female 224 3.49 1.27 394 0.428 0.669 Male 172 3.44 1.26 Strategy Female 224 3.11 1.43 394 2.973 0.003 * Male 172 3.48 1.09 Interest Female 224 3.24 1.35 394 2.334 0.022 * Male 172 3.54 1.19 Luck Female 224 2.70 1.40 394 2.995 0.003 * Male 172 3.13 1.38 Task difficulty Female 224 3.21 1.51 394 0.758 0.449 Male 172 3.32 1.25 Parent s influence Female 224 2.46 1.45 394 5.393 0.000 * Male 172 3.24 1.37 Teacher s influence Female 224 2.65 1.56 394 2.146 0.032 * *p<0.05 Male 172 2.96 1.30 COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 420

Table 6 Comparison of Failure Attributions of Male & Female Students in English Attributions Gender N M SD df t p Ability Female 224 2.77 1.34 394 0.604 0.546 Male 172 2.86 1.39 Effort Female 224 3.28 1.30 394 0.713 0.476 Male 172 3.37 1.22 Strategy Female 224 3.13 1.44 394 3.463 0.001 * Male 172 3.56 1.05 Interest Female 224 2.82 1.44 394 6.397 0.000 * Male 172 3.66 1.18 Luck Female 224 2.69 1.33 394 3.418 0.001 * Male 172 3.16 1.38 Task difficulty Female 224 2.93 1.56 394 1.897 0.059 Male 172 3.21 1.34 Parent s influence Female 224 2.62 1.49 394 2.996 0.003 * Male 172 3.04 1.32 Teacher s influence Female 224 2.77 1.59 394 1.753 0.080 *p<0.05 Male 172 3.02 1.31 Table 7 Comparison of Success Attributions of Male & Female Students in Mathematics Attributions Gender N M SD df t p Ability Female 224 3.83 1.08 394 1.902 0.058 Male 172 3.59 1.29 Effort Female 224 4.01 0.97 394 0.488 0.626 Male 172 3.97 0.92 Strategy Female 224 3.99 0.93 394 0.014 0.989 Male 172 3.99 1.01 Interest Female 224 4.01 0.95 394 0.962 0.337 Male 172 3.91 1.16 Luck Female 224 3.79 1.18 394 2.097 0.037 * Male 172 3.51 1.39 Task difficulty Female 224 3.35 1.47 394 1.678 0.094 Male 172 3.12 1.30 Parent s influence Female 224 3.91 1.21 394 0.884 0.377 Male 172 3.81 1.16 Teacher s influence Female 224 4.08 1.09 394 0.238 0.812 *p<0.05 Male 172 4.06 0.98 COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 421

Table 8 Comparison of Success Attributions of Male & Female Students in English Attributions Gender N M SD df t p Ability Female 224 3.92 1.13 394 1.681 0.094 Male 172 3.71 1.29 Effort Female 224 4.08 0.98 394 1.784 0.075 Male 172 3.88 1.15 Strategy Female 224 3.94 1.06 394 0.398 0.691 Male 172 3.98 0.95 Interest Female 224 3.93 1.00 394 0.363 0.717 Male 172 3.97 1.06 Luck Female 224 3.68 1.14 394 1.691 0.092 Male 172 3.47 1.29 Task difficulty Female 224 3.66 1.36 394 2.154 0.032 * Male 172 3.36 1.31 Parent s influence Female 224 4.08 1.07 394 2.775 0.006 * Male 172 3.75 1.23 Teacher s influence Female 224 4.17 1.05 394 1.742 0.082 *p<0.05 Male 172 3.97 1.18 Table 9 Comparison of Success and Failure Attributions of Male Students in Mathematics Attributions Outcomes M SD df t p Ability Failure 2.94 1.296 171-5.405 0.000 ** Success 3.59 1.377 Effort Failure 3.43 1.266 171-5.138 0.000 ** Success 3.97 0.920 Strategy Failure 3.48 1.094 171-5.231 0.000 ** Success 3.99 1.005 Interest Failure 3.54 1.191 171-2.930 0.004 ** Success 3.91 1.164 Luck Failure 3.13 1.384 171-2.519 0.013 * Success 3.51 1.391 Task difficulty Failure 3.31 1.255 171 1.572 0.018 * Success 3.12 1.303 Parent s influence Failure 3.23 1.379 171-3.989 0.000 ** Success 3.80 1.166 Teacher s influence Failure 2.96 1.301 171-9.074 0.000 ** Success 4.06 0.980 N=172 ** P<0.01 * P<0.05 COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 422

Table 10 Comparison of Success and Failure Attributions of Male Students in English Attributions Outcomes M SD df t P Ability Failure 2.86 1.390 171-6.585 0.000 ** Success 3.71 1.291 Effort Failure 3.37 1.229 171-4.188 0.000 ** Success 3.88 1.158 Strategy Failure 3.56 1.054 171-4.118 0.000** Success 3.98 0.958 Interest Failure 3.66 1.189 171-2.652 0.009** Success 3.97 1.061 Luck Failure 3.16 1.383 171-2.290 0.023 * Success 3.47 1.290 Task difficulty Failure 3.21 1.344 171-1.099 0.273 Success 3.36 1.319 Parent s influence Failure 3.04 1.323 171-5.348 0.000 ** Success 3.75 1.237 Teacher s influence Failure 3.02 1.317 171-7.265 0.000** Success 3.97 1.189 N=172 ** P<0.01 * P<0.05 Table 11 Comparison of Success and Failure Attributions of Female Students in Mathematics Attributions Outcomes M SD df t p Ability Failure 2.68 1.375 223-9.913 0.000 ** Success 3.82 1.080 Effort Failure 3.49 1.267 223-5.058 0.000** Success 4.01 0.970 Strategy Failure 3.11 1.427 223-8.268 0.000** Success 3.99 0.930 Interest Failure 3.24 1.357 223-6.784 0.000** Success 4.01 0.951 Luck Failure 2.70 1.404 223-9.413 0.000** Success 3.79 1.180 Task difficulty Failure 3.21 1.511 223-1.257 0.210 Success 3.35 1.478 Parent s influence Failure 2.45 1.457 223-11.710 0.000** Success 3.91 1.215 Teacher s influence Failure 2.65 1.559 223-10.486 0.000** Success 4.08 1.096 N=224 ** P<0.01 * P<0.05 COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 423

Table 12 Comparison of Success and Failure Attributions of Female Students in English Attributions Outcomes M SD df t p Ability Failure 2.77 1.347 223-9.980 0.000** Success 3.92 1.135 Effort Failure 3.28 1.308 223-8.035 0.000** Success 4.08 0.985 Strategy Failure 3.12 1.441 223-7.223 0.000** Success 3.94 1.067 Interest Failure 2.82 1.443 223-9.625 0.000** Success 3.93 1.002 Luck Failure 2.69 1.338 223-8.900 0.000** Success 3.68 1.144 Task difficulty Failure 2.93 1.563 223-5.726 0.000** Success 3.66 1.369 Parent s influence Failure 2.62 1.498 223-12.132 0.000** Success 4.08 1.074 Teacher s influence Failure 2.77 1.595 223-10.468 0.000** Success 4.17 1.059 N=224 ** P<0.01 * P<0.05 COPY RIGHT 2012 Institute of Interdisciplinary Business Research 424