relationships (i.e., attribution schemal). The I-E Scale Peterborough, Ontario, Canada ROTTER S (1968) Internal-External Locus of Control (I-E) Scale

Similar documents
Perception of Female and Male Success in the United States and Third World Nations I

ATTRIBUTIONAL STYLE, ACADEMIC SELF- EFFICACY, AND ATTEMPS TO INFLUENCE THE STUDY ENVIRONMENT

Title: The Relationship between Locus of Control and Academic Level and Sex of Secondary School Students

Pearson Education Limited Edinburgh Gate Harlow Essex CM20 2JE England and Associated Companies throughout the world

The Controllability Beliefs Scale used with carers of people with intellectual disabilities: psychometric propertiesjir_

THEORIES IN SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY PSY834, Fall 2018 Thursdays, 9:10-12:00 210A Berkey Hall

Personality and Individual Differences

Attributions and Performance: An Empirical Test of Kukla's Theory

Locus of Control: Influence of Internality, Externality- Others, Externality-Chance among Management Students

Locus of Control in Relation to Academic Achievement of College Students in Meghalaya

AAPT Summer 2004, Sacramento, CA

EFFECTS OF SEX, ATTENTIONAL FOCUS ON ATTRIBUTION FOR SUCCESS AND FAILURE OF HIGH AND LOW ACHIEVERS

A scale to measure locus of control of behaviour

Gender Differences in Locus of Control

Impact of Academic Self Efficacy and Locus of Control on Academic Achievement of high school students

Academic Locus of Control of High and Low Achieving Students

CHAPTER II. STUDIES ON ATTRIBUTION THEORY s LITERATURE SEARCH. This chapter surveys the literature on attribution theory

CHILDREN'S PERCEIVED CONTINGENCIES OF TEACHER REINFORCEMENTS, PERCEPTIONS OF COMPETENCE, AND ACADEMIC PERFORMANCE DISSERTATION

The relationship between locus of control and academic achievement and the role of gender

Attributions in the Sports Pages

WHY? A QUESTION BEHIND EVERY OUTCOME FINALLY ANSWERED

An in-depth analysis of the relationship between age and attribution in EFL contexts

CAUSAL ATTRIBUTION BELIEFS AMONG SCHOOL STUDENTS IN PAKISTAN

Causal Attribution Beliefs of Success and Failure: A Perspective from Pakistan

Meta-Analysis Procedures. Search procedures. Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies were searched from

On the Use of the Causal Dimension Scale in a Field Setting: A Test With Confirmatory Factor Analysis in Success and Failure Situations

A General Framework for Personality Psychology

Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and Autonomy

CHAPTER-II DEVELOPMENT AND DESCRIPTION OF TOOLS

FUNCTIONAL CONSISTENCY IN THE FACE OF TOPOGRAPHICAL CHANGE IN ARTICULATED THOUGHTS Kennon Kashima

CHAPTER- III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Supporting Information

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Children s Assessment of Participation and Enjoyment & Activities of Children (CAPE)

Significance Tests Based on Differences Donald W. Zimmerman, Carleton University

An Investigation of Factors Influencing Causal Attributions in Managerial Decision Making

The Effect of Perceived Difficulty and Locus of Control on Task Performance

An International Multi-Disciplinary Journal, Ethiopia Vol. 4 (1) January, 2010

Locus of Control and Psychological Well-Being: Separating the Measurement of Internal and External Constructs -- A Pilot Study

Post-Hoc Rationalism in Science. Eric Luis Uhlmann. HEC Paris. (Commentary on Jones and Love, "Bayesian Fundamentalism or Enlightenment?

Measuring Empathy: Reliability and Validity of Empathy Quotient

LANGUAGE TEST RELIABILITY On defining reliability Sources of unreliability Methods of estimating reliability Standard error of measurement Factors

Type of Education, Locus of Control and Optimistic - Pessimistic. Attitude among Undergraduate College Students

Factors affecting the classes of causal explanations by industrial safety specialists

Intrinsic Motivation and Social Constraints: A Qualitative Meta-analysis of Experimental Research Utilizing Creative Activities in the Visual Arts

Self-Efficacy in the Prediction of Academic Performance and Perceived Career Options

The Effects of Locus of Control and Method of Goal Setting on Task Motivation

University of Groningen. Mastering (with) a handicap Kunnen, Elske

2 - CORRELATION BETWEEN HARDINESS AND THE SENSE OF COHERENCE

ZIVA KUNDA. Updated July University of Waterloo Waterloo, Ontario Canada N2L 3G1 (519) ext

AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) Association for Information Systems. Brad Prince

The Grateful Disposition: Links to Patterns of Attribution for Positive Events. Sharon L. Brion. Michael E. McCullough. Southern Methodist University

Predictive Value of Locus of Control on Academic Performance Of Senior Secondary School Students in Sokoto Metropolis, Sokoto, Nigeria.

Advocating Internal Locus: The appeal of James Frey

NORM OF INTERNALITY AND EVALUATION OF PROFESSIONAL WORTH IN AN ORGANIZATIONAL CONTEXT. Astrid MIGNON. Patrick MOLLARET.

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

The Impact of Culture on Consumers Perceptions of Service Recovery Efforts

AI04, AAPT Summer 2003 Meeting. Vince H. Kuo. Patricia Heller, Ken Heller. Physics Education Research Group

Selective Attention and Locus of Control in Learning Disabled and Normal Children

Chapter 4: Understanding Others

Academic Satisfaction and Its Relationship to Internal Locus of Control among Students of Najran University

The Effect of Locus of Control and Stability on Academic Achievement in Eighth Grade Students From North Central West Virginia: A Comparative Approach

Lecture 4: Research Approaches

Depression and Attributional Style: Interpretations of Important Personal Events

THEORETICAL ASSUMPTIONS AND PSYCHOMETRIC CHARACTERISTICS OF THE SENSE OF PERSONAL CONTROL AT WORK QUESTIONNAIRE

Reliability and Validity checks S-005

Factors Affecting Children's Attitudes Toward Health Care and Responses to Stressful Medical Procedures 1

An Empirical Study on Causal Relationships between Perceived Enjoyment and Perceived Ease of Use

A Study of Health Locus of Control among Female High and Low Performers University Athletes

Epistemological Beliefs and Leadership Style among School Principals

Examining the Psychometric Properties of The McQuaig Occupational Test

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation

Analyze the Impact of Control Source on Explicit and Implicit Memory

Perceived Likelihood as a Measure of Optimism and Pessimism: Support for the Future Events Scale

Explanatory Attributions of Anxiety and Recovery in a Study of Kava

ISC- GRADE XI HUMANITIES ( ) PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 2- Methods of Psychology

LOCUS OF CONTROL AND REACTION TO ILLNESS: A STUDY OF PATIENTS WITH CHRONIC RENAL FAILURE

Locus of Control and Its Impact on Self-Efficacy of University Graduates

Assessing the Affective, Behavioral, and Cognitive Dimensions of Stuttering in Young Children

Achievement in Science as Predictors of Students Scientific Attitude

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

Lecture Week 3 Quality of Measurement Instruments; Introduction SPSS

2008 Ohio State University. Campus Climate Study. Prepared by. Student Life Research and Assessment

N1/ Afo, 9. For the Degree of MASTER OF MUSIC. Richard S. Williams, B. M. Denton, Texas December, 1992

ATTRIBUTION THEORY. Attribution - to explain by indicating a cause

Ch. 11 Measurement. Paul I-Hai Lin, Professor A Core Course for M.S. Technology Purdue University Fort Wayne Campus

Family Expectations, Self-Esteem, and Academic Achievement among African American College Students

CHAPTER 7: Attribution theory, confidence and self-efficacy. Practice questions at - text book pages 124 to 125 QUESTIONS AND ANSWERS.

2 Types of psychological tests and their validity, precision and standards

Attribution theory in the organizational sciences: A case of unrealized potential

Schunk, D. H. (1984). Self-efficacy perspective on achievement behavior. Educational Psychologist, 19,

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

Help! My Brain is Out of Control! Impact from Irrelevant and Misleading Information in Software Effort Estimation. BEKK-seminar 25.1.

d =.20 which means females earn 2/10 a standard deviation more than males

Archive of SID. (GSE-10) GSE-10

Attitude Measurement

CONTENT ANALYSIS OF COGNITIVE BIAS: DEVELOPMENT OF A STANDARDIZED MEASURE Heather M. Hartman-Hall David A. F. Haaga

0457 GLOBAL PERSPECTIVES

Who Is Rational? Studies of Individual Differences in Reasoning

Transcription:

EDUCATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENT 1978, 38 INITIAL VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY OF THE TRENT ATTRIBUTION PROFILE (TAP) AS A MEASURE OF ATTRIBUTION SCHEMA AND LOCUS OF CONTROL PAUL T. P. WONG University of California, Los Angeles DEREK A. WATTERS and CATHERINE F. SPROULE Trent University Peterborough, Ontario, Canada Success and failure outcomes in three achievement-oriented common-life situations were simulated in the Trent Attribution Profile (TAP) with both self and others-orientations. The TAP provides measures of locus of control and stability dimensions which can be further partitioned into individual attribution elements (i.e., ability, effort, task difficulty, and luck) under four different conditions (selfsuccess, other-success, self-failure, other-failure). The Internality dimension of the TAP was correlated significantly with Rotter s I-E Scale. The test-retest reliability coefficient was significant. Traditional success-failure and self-other biases were also obtained. ROTTER S (1968) Internal-External Locus of Control (I-E) Scale measures individual differences in generalized beliefs regarding behavior-outcome relationships (i.e., attribution schemal). The I-E Scale consists of forced-choice items, each of which offers an internal and an Requests for reprints should be addressed to: Paul T. P. Wong, Department of Psychology, Trent University, Peterborough, Ontario, K9J 7B8, Canada. Copies of the TAP and scoring sheets can also be obtained from Wong. Research was supported by a Canada Council Leave Fellowship awarded to the first author. 1 Attribution schema, in the present paper, refer to generalized beliefs regarding causal attributions about common life events as different from specific causal attributions about performance in a specific, concrete situation. Copyright 1978 by Educational and Psychological Measurement 1129

1130 EDUCATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENT external alternative. This scale thus yields only one global score of Internality. According to Weiner (e.g., Weiner, Frieze, Kukla, Reed, Rest, and Rosenbaum, 1971), causal attributions, at least in achievement-oriented contexts, involve both the locus of control (internal versus external) and the stability (stable versus unstable) dimensions. The internal source of attribution consists of ability and effort; the external source, task difficulty and luck. Ability and effort constitute stable attribution elements, whereas effort and luck constitute unstable elements. Weiner s model, which has been supported by a number of studies, has proven to be very useful in unravelling causal attribution processes (e.g., Frieze, 1973). However, Rotter s I-E Scale does not allow the partitioning of attribution schema into the locus of control and stability dimensions; nor does it permit the assessment of relative importance of individual attribution elements. There is also some evidence that individual differences in causal attributions may be limited to certain conditions. For example, Davis and Davis (1972) reported that the difference in causal attributions between individuals classified as Internals and Externals as measured by the I-E Scale occurred primarily in a failure condition. Unfortunately, the I-E Scale does not provide separate scores of internality under success and failure conditions. Recent research on causal attribution has demonstrated various attribution biases or errors (Ross, 1977). For example, success-failure biases take the form of stronger internal attributions following success than following failure, and self-other biases typically refer to stronger dispositional attributions for others but a stronger situational attribution for self. These biases have been demonstrated mainly in betweensubjects studies. In no study, however, have all the biases within the same subject been shown. It remains an interesting question whether individuals who differ in attribution schema also differ in attribution biases. In view of the previously described developments in research on causal attributions, it seems advantageous to develop a questionnaire to measure individual differences in attribution schema based on Weiner s model. The questionnaire should be constructed in such a way that it would provide a profile of attribution schema in different conditions (self-success, other-success, self-failure, and other-failure); that the attribution can be partitioned into the locus of control and stability dimensions; and that attribution biases can be derived. Thus the purpose of this investigation was to provide information regarding the validity and reliability of selected dimensions of a newly devised measure entitled Trent Attribution Profile (TAP).

PAUL T. P. WONG ET AL. 1131 Test Development Method The following critera were used to generate questionnaire items: (1) both success and failure outcomes should be related to common life situations; (2) these situations should be achievement-related but not limited to academic situations; (3) other-orientation should be presented in general terms, e.g., &dquo;others,&dquo; &dquo;a person,&dquo; or an &dquo;individual.&dquo; On the basis of these criteria, seven judges (including two psychology professors and five students in a third-year social psychology course) chose three common life situations: academic, social, and financial. In each situation, the two different outcomes (success and failure) that were presented in two different orientations (self and others) yielded a total of 12 different conditions. The Trent Attribution Profile (TAP) consists of 12 questionnaire items representing these 12 conditions. Each item is followed by four possible explanations based on four attribution elements. For example, the item on academic failure for others is presented this way: &dquo;when people fail school, it is because of (a) lack of academic skills, (b) bad breaks, (c) lack of effort; and (d) harsh judgements by the teachers.&dquo; In the case of academic success for self, the item is phrased this way: &dquo;my good marks in school were due to (a) easy marking by the teacher, (b) hard work on my part, (c) good luck, (d) my academic skills.&dquo; Respondents were asked to rate the importance of each of the four explanations for each item on a 5-point scale, with 1 meaning not at all important, 3 undecided, and 5 very important. In scoring the TAP, ratings in the three different life situations are summed to reflect generalized beliefs regarding causal attributions. The TAP thus provides a profile of attribution schema in four different conditions: (a) self-success, (b) other-success, (c) self-failure, and (d) other-failure. Discrepancies between different conditions indicate the direction and magnitude of attribution biases. For example, successfailure bias may be indicated by higher internal attributions but lower external attributions in success conditions as compared to failure conditions. Subjects. In Study 1, the subjects consisted of 30 males and 30 females, with a mean age of 25.2. In each sex, half of the subjects were Trent University students, and the other half primarily consisted of public school teachers and office workers. In Study 2, the subjects were 21 male and 15 female Trent University students, with a mean age of 22.1. Procedure. In Study 1, the subjects were approached individually and given the I-E Scale and TAP to complete. In Study 2, the subjects

1132 EDUCATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENT were given the TAP twice, with an average inter-test interval of four weeks. Validity Data Results In Study 1, the total Internality rating of TAP (combining ability and effort ratings) was correlated significantly with the Internality score of the I-E Scale (Pearson s r.51, p <.001). The I-E Scale = was also correlated significantly with ability and effort ratings (rs.37 and =.49, ps <.01 and <.001, respectively). The attribution differences between internals and externals as defined by a median split in the I-E Scale are shown in Table 1. Consistent with the literature (Davis and Davis, 1972), internals and externals differed primarily in the failure condition. It is also informative to note that effort and task difficulty attributions are most sensitive in differentiating between internals and externals. (More detailed profiles of internals and externals involving further differentiation into self-success, other-success, self-failure, and other-failure are not presented in this paper, because they do not yield additional information in this particular sample.) Differences in attribution biases between internals and externals were also calculated on the basis of the discrepancy scores. Results showed that externals did have a greater success-failure bias than did TABLE I Differences in Attributions between Intervals and Externals as Defined by Rotter s I-E Scale. p <.05... p <.01.

PAUL T. P. WONG ET AL. 1133 internals in task difficulty attribution (t 2.32, = p <.05). In other words, the fact that externals did exhibit a stronger task difficulty attribution than did internals in failure resulted in a larger successfailure discrepancy score. One can also detect this difference by inspecting Table 1. Evidence for the construct validity of the TAP was further enhanced by the findings of traditional attribution biases within the same subjects. When dependent t-tests were computed on the differences in attribution ratings by the same subjects, it was found that success did produce higher internal ratings than did failure, that higher dispositional ratings (i.e., ability) were given to others than to self, but that higher situational attributions (i.e., task) were given to self than to others. These biases were obtained in the first study as well as in both the test and retest of the Study 2 (p <.01 in all cases). 2 Reliability The test-retest reliability coefficients are presented in Table 2. In all four attribution elements the reliability coefficients of total rating scores in the TAP were of the same magnitude as those associated with the I-E Scale (i.e., Rotter, 1966). Dependent t-tests failed to reveal any significant difference between test and retest in all attribution ratings in the TAP. Discussion The significant correlations between the I-E Scale and the internal attribution elements in the TAP furnish promising evidence for the validity of the TAP as an instrument to measure locus of control. There are several advantages of using the TAP: (1) it provides separate measures of internality (ability and effort), externality (task difficulty and luck), stability (ability and task difficulty), and unstability (effort and luck); (2) it yields a profile of attribution ratings under four different conditions (self-success, other-success, self-failure, other-failure) ; and (3) it allows a determination of attribution biases. Given that the I-E Scale has been shown to be related to a number of personality and performance variables (Lefcourt, 1976), it is now possible to determine which attribution elements under which conditions in the TAP really account for the variances. Since all three common life situations simulated in the TAP are achievement-oriented, the TAP may be more predictive of achievement behavior than 2 Detailed tables of dependent t-values are available upon request.

1134 EDUCATIONAL AND PSYCHOLOGICAL MEASUREMENT TABLE 2 Test-Retest Reliability Coefficients (Pearson rs).p::s.05... p <.01.... p <.001. is the I-E Scale which actually contains subscales related to just world belief and political unresponsiveness. (See Lefcourt ( 1976). ) REFERENCES Davis, W. L. and Davis, D. E. Internal-external control and attribution of responsibility for success and failure. Journal of Personality, 1972, 40, 123-136. Frieze, I. Studies of information processing and the attributional process. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles, 1973. Lefcourt, H. M. Locus of control: Current trends in theory and research. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 1976. Ross, L. The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In L. Berkowitz (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology, Vol. 10. New York: Academic Press, 1977 (pp. 174-220). Rotter, J. B. Generalized expectancies for internal versus external control of reinforcement. Psychological Monographs, 1966, 80, No. 1 (Whole No. 609). Weiner, B., Frieze, I., Kukla, A., Reed, L., Rest, S., and Rosenbaum, R. M. Perceiving the causes of success and failure. In E. E. Jones, D. E. Kanouse, H. H. Kelley, R. E. Nisbett, S. Valins, and B. Weiner (Eds.), Attribution: Perceiving the causes of. behavior M orristown, N. J.: General Learning Press, 1971.