Emotion, Stress, and Cardiovascular Response: An Experimental Test of Models of Positive and Negative Affect

Similar documents
Can Stress Make You Sick?

2016 Sara Patricia Bolen ALL RIGHTS RESERVED

The Open Psychology Journal

EFFECTS OF SEX AND DISPOSITION ON CARDIOVASCULAR REACTIVITY AND RECOVERY THESIS

Moodscope: Mood management through self-tracking and peer support

Beyond Extinction: Erasing human fear responses and preventing the return of fear. Merel Kindt, Marieke Soeter & Bram Vervliet

Loss and the Organization of Affect

An investigation into the acute effect of exercise on physiological and psychological responses to musical performance

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals

Personality and physiological reactions to acute psychological stress in a large cohort of middle aged men and women

Sikha Naik Mark Vosvick, Ph.D, Chwee-Lye Chng, Ph.D, and John Ridings, A.A. Center for Psychosocial Health

Tourists' Emotions: A Longitudinal Study

University of Padova, Padua, Italy, and HARVEST Study Group, Italy

Negative affect words prime beer consumption in young drinkers

Positive emotion inducement modulates cardiovascular responses caused by mental work

COrvfJ\;fUNICA TION APPREHENSION AND ACCUJ\;fULA TED COrvfj\tfUNICA TION STATE ANXIETY EXPERIENCES: A RESEARCH NOTE

Special Lecture 10/28/2012

SelfRelax: mobile phone application for relaxation Evaluation study. Juho Merilahti

Workplace Mental Health & Wellbeing Programs An overview

P A N A N X I E T Y C

Social Skills and Perceived Maternal Acceptance-Rejection in Relation to Depression in Infertile Women

Stress & Health. } This section covers: The definition of stress Measuring stress

Motivation, Conflict, Emotion. Abdul-Monaf Al-Jadiry, MD; FRCPsych Professor of Psychiatry

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

Physiological Responses in the Anticipation of an Auditory. and Visual Stimulus

Metacognitive therapy for generalized anxiety disorder: An open trial

The challenge of representing emotional colouring. Roddy Cowie

Balancing the Challenges of Mental Health Claims in Insurance

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 1998, 26, Cambridge University Press. Printed in the United Kingdom

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

The Use of Alternative Therapies to Lower Blood Pressure in Older African Americans

Affective pictures and emotion analysis of facial expressions with local binary pattern operator: Preliminary results

BackBeat Cardiac Neuromodulation Therapy (CNT) for Immediate, Substantial and Sustained Lowering of Blood Pressure. Daniel Burkhoff MD PhD

MENTAL TOUGHNESS: IS IT THE KEY TO ACADEMIC SUCCESS?

Family-centered Stress Management for Childhood Cancer: A Multimodal Intervention for Children Newly Diagnosed with Cancer and their Families

Psychology Session 13 Stress and Health

Social support and loneliness in college students: Effects on pulse pressure reactivity to acute stress

Application Package Mental Health First Aid First Nations Co-facilitator Training Course

Avoidant Coping Moderates the Association between Anxiety and Physical Functioning in Patients with Chronic Heart Failure

Robert C. Whitaker, MD, MPH Professor of Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Pediatrics Temple University Philadelphia, PA

A Comparative Study of Psychological Hardiness and Coping Strategies in Female Athlete and Non-Athlete Students

Flow in computer-human environment

Adjustment to Retirement: The Moderating Role of Attachment. Dikla Segel, Peter Bamberger

ASSOCIATIONS BETWEEN SELF-RATED HEALTH AND PERSONALITY

Serial Subtraction Performance in the Cycling Study

Health Psychology. What is Stress? Stress: Theories & Models. Stress: Theories & Models

Personality and Individual Differences

Stress Reactivity and Vulnerability to Depressed Mood in College Students

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

Mental Health and Stress Management

Why (an overview of) twin research?

The Effect of Choice-Making Opportunities during Activity Schedules on Task Engagement of Adults with Autism

Emotions and Moods. Robbins & Judge Organizational Behavior 13th Edition. Bob Stretch Southwestern College

Mindfulness as a Mediator of Psychological Wellbeing in a Stress Reduction Intervention for Cancer Patients - a randomized study

Exploring a Counterintuitive Finding with Methodological Implications

Emotional memory: from affective relevance to arousal

Phase 3 investigation of aprocitentan for resistant hypertension management. Investor Webcast June 2018

David M. Fresco, Ph.D.

Extraversion and Neuroticism in Contact Athletes, No Contact Athletes and Non-athletes: A Research Note

Towards Human Affect Modeling: A Comparative Analysis of Discrete Affect and Valence-Arousal Labeling

Lecture Overview. Stress & Health. Psychological Stress. Stress. Stress. Effects of Stress. Perceived Control. Physiological Thriving.

Affect, Motivation, & Virtual Reality

Moralization Through Moral Shock: Exploring Emotional Antecedents to Moral Conviction. Table of Contents

Validity of the Perceived Health Competence Scale in a UK primary care setting.

Relaxation and Stress Management Through Meditation. Dennis Abreu AM LCSW

Validation of the OMRON 705 IT blood pressure measuring device according to the International Protocol of the European Society of Hypertension

Psychology Your Life

Interpretations of Ambiguous Social Situations and Cardiovascular Responses in Adolescents

Emotional Intelligence (EI)

Running head: RELIGIOUS STRENGTH AND PTG 1

Comorbidity With Substance Abuse P a g e 1

University of Huddersfield Repository

ATTENTION. The Art (and Habit) Of. Paying Attention 5/4/2018

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory

Chapter 11 summary definitief ineke brands.indd :57:59

International Journal of Research and Review E-ISSN: ; P-ISSN:

Facebook Therapy? Why Do People Share Self-Relevant Content Online? Eva Buechel. University of Miami. Jonah Berger. University of Pennsylvania

Effects of Biofeedback-Aided Relaxation on the Psychological Stress Symptoms of College Students

Chapter 37 Effects of Acupuncture on Anxiety Levels and Prefrontal Cortex Activity Measured by Near-Infrared Spectroscopy: A Pilot Study

Building Resilience The role of challenge and threat appraisals

School Climate and Indicators of Emotional Distress

Heart Age and Cardiovascular Risk

Delivering self-efficacy: I can do it!

Methods for Computing Missing Item Response in Psychometric Scale Construction

Trait Negative Affect Relates to Prior-Week Symptoms, But Not to Reports of Illness Episodes, Illness Symptoms, and Care Seeking Among Older Persons

Catheter-Based Renal Sympathetic Denervation in the Management of Resistant Hypertension

Teacher stress: A comparison between casual and permanent primary school teachers with a special focus on coping

How to Approach Someone Having a Mental Health Challenge

Mean Arterial Pressure Classification: A Better Tool for Statistical Interpretation of Blood Pressure Related Risk Covariates

AP Psychology Guided Reading Unit 8B Emotions, Stress, and Health

Defining emotion: a clinical perspective

The Impact of Autonomic Neuropathy on Left Ventricular Function in Normotensive Type 1 Diabetic Patients: a Tissue Doppler Echocardiographic Study

Emotional Intelligence and NLP for better project people Lysa

Check a Pulse! When to Question SpO 2, NIBP & EtCO 2 Readings

Individual Differences in the Stress and Workload of Sustained Attention. James L. Szalma University of Central Florida Orlando, FL.

the rest of our research. For be any correlation between outcome and the strength of the relationship according to the coach.

Introduction to SOCIAL STYLE sm

INSTRUCTION MANUAL Instructions for Patient Health Questionnaire (PHQ) and GAD-7 Measures

Psychological Stress and Disease: Implications for the Gulf Oil Spill. Sheldon Cohen, PhD Carnegie Mellon University

Transcription:

Int. J. Behav. Med. (2010) 17:189 194 DOI 10.1007/s12529-009-9063-3 Emotion, Stress, and Cardiovascular Response: An Experimental Test of Models of Positive and Negative Affect Haulie Dowd & Alex Zautra & Michael Hogan Published online: 18 September 2009 # International Society of Behavioral Medicine 2009 Abstract Background The nature of the relationship between positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA) has been a topic of debate for some time. In particular, there are gaps in our knowledge of the independent effects of PA and NA on health under stress. Purpose The study examined the effects of a laboratoryinduced stressor on the experience of PA and NA, and the effects of affect on cardiovascular (CV) reactivity and recovery. Method A sample of 56 female college students was randomly assigned to a public speaking (stress) task or a silent reading (control) task. Pre- and posttask PA and NA were measured using the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS Watson J Pers Soc Psychol 54:1,063-1,070, 1988). Baseline, task, and posttask cardiovascular measures were also recorded. Results The results indicated that PA and NA responded differently to the stressor and contributed independently to the prediction of both CV reactivity and recovery. Of particular interest was the finding that higher levels of both PA and NA predicted greater CV recovery. Conclusion Results are discussed in light of the debate concerning the (in)dependence of positive and negative emotions and the importance of understanding the dynamics of emotions, stress, and health. H. Dowd : M. Hogan (*) Department of Psychology, NUI, Galway, Ireland e-mail: michael.hogan@nuigalway.ie A. Zautra Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, USA Keywords Stress. Affect. Cardiovascular reactivity. Health Introduction For some time now there has been a debate on the nature of the relationship between positive affects (PA) and negative affects (NA). Some argue for a bipolar model with PA and NA inversely correlated (e.g., [1]). Others argue for a bivariate model with independent but covarying PA and NA (e.g., [2]). Most researchers investigating the relative merits of bipolar versus bivariate models of affects and other psychological states have constructed and tested measurement models with which to gage the relative independence of positive and negative experience. Another approach, and the one adopted here, has been to examine whether distinguishing between positive and negative states is useful in understanding how stressful events influence behavior. In this study, we examine the merits of a bivariate versus bipolar approach to the prediction of affective and cardiovascular (CV) responses to a laboratory stressor. Increasingly, researchers have pointed to the beneficial effects of positive emotion on health and longevity (see [3]) independent of negative emotions. Fredrickson and Levenson [4] found direct evidence that positive emotion can quicken cardiovascular recovery. More generally, the dynamic model of affect (DMA; 10 proposes that PA and NA often function independently in shaping health outcomes, and that stress may alter the way PA and NA function. Indeed, evidence from several quarters points to the conclusion that positive events and emotions have their greatest influence on negative emotions and CV health when the person is under stress (see [5] for a review). In a meta-analysis on the effects of laboratory induced stressors, Feldman et al. [6] found an association

190 Int. J. Behav. Med. (2010) 17:189 194 between an increase in negative emotion and CV reactivity. The amount of variance accounted for was small and ranged between 2% and 12%. However, this research did not examine positive emotions. Maier, Waldstein, and Synowski [7] found that PA before a stressor, but not NA was associated with more CV reactivity during a stressor task. Somewhat typical of studies of this kind, Maier et al. [7] did not examine CV recovery. Consequently, there are gaps in our knowledge about the independent effects of positive and negative effect on CV reactivity and recovery during a stress episode. A good test of the idea that positive emotions have their greatest influence on CV health when the person is under stress is to examine the effects of PA and NA on CV reactivity and recovery in a group exposed to stress and group not exposed to stress. The current experiment examined changes in both affect and cardiovascular responding in the context of a public speaking task (experimental stress group) and a silent reading task (control group). We predicted a significant effect of public speaking, but not silent reading, on PA, NA, and CV reactivity. Consistent with bivariate models of affect (cf. [8]), we predicted independent effects of stress on PA and NA and independent effects of NA and PA on CV reactivity and recovery. Finally, consistent with DMA we predicted that independent effects of NA and PA on CV reactivity and recovery would be greatest in the experimental stress condition. Method Design and Statistical Analyses A 2 2 (time: pretest/posttest) (stress: stress/no stress) design was used. Analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to examine the effect of stress on affect over time. Multiple regression was used to examine the effects of stress, affect, and stress affect interactions on CV reactivity and recovery. Participants Participants were 56 women, with a mean age of 18.9 (SD = 2.87; range 17 35) years, recruited from a sample enrolled in an undergraduate course in psychology at NUI Galway, who received course credits for taking part in the study. Participants had to be between 17 and 40 years of age and have no history of hypertension to be included in the study. Of those who were within the appropriate agerange, none had a history of hypertension. A female only sample was selected to avoid confounding by gender differences in CV reactivity to stressors [9]. Based on available prescreening data, those assigned to the stress and no stress conditions were matched on measures of trait anxiety (STAI, Form Y-2, [10]). Caffeine and nicotine use was not restricted as this may have produced unpredictable effects of withdrawal on affect and CV reactivity to stress [11]. We recorded smoking behavior (number smoked daily and time smoking), but found no correlation with measures of affect or cardiovascular responding. Affect Measurement The PANAS [12] was used to measure PA and NA. The scale consists of 20 adjectives describing different feelings and emotions; ten describe positive moods, and ten negative moods. Apparatus A MS 700 automatic digital blood pressure monitor, manufactured by Mars Corporation, was used to measure systolic and diastolic blood pressure and heart rate. Experimental Task Participants in the experimental condition completed an evaluative speaking task. Each participant was asked to prepare a speech defending themselves in a role-play scenario where they had been falsely accused of shoplifting. This task has been shown to be successful in eliciting cardiovascular responses [13]. Participants in the control condition were asked to read psychological journal articles for the duration of the experiment. Procedure Informed consent was obtained, and participants were tested individually. After the digital blood pressure monitor was attached, baseline systolic blood pressure (SBP), diastolic blood pressure (DBP), and heart rate (HR) readings were taken at the end of 1, 2.5, and 4 min. During this time, participants were asked to sit quietly and were offered magazines to read. Participants then completed the PANAS. Participants in the experimental condition were then given 3 min to prepare for the speech task, and, after a cue was provided to begin, they then had 5 min to deliver their speech. While participants spoke, SBP, DBP, and HR were measured at the end of 1, 2.5, and 4 min. Participants in the control condition read articles from psychological journals, during which time their blood pressure and heart rate measured at the same time intervals as those in the experimental group. At the end of the experimental phase, participants in both groups again completed the PANAS. Participants were then given 5 min to relax and SBP, DBP,

Int. J. Behav. Med. (2010) 17:189 194 191 and HR recovery readings were taken at the end of 1, 2.5, and 4 min. Results Affect Two 2 (time) 2 (stress) ANOVAs run separately on the two affect measures revealed the following pattern of results: For PA, there was a main effect of Time, F (1, 54) = 8.98, p=.004, η 2 =.143, with PA before the experimental condition (time 1) significantly higher than PA after (time 2), and a time stress interaction effect, F (1, 54) = 4.14, p=.047, η 2 =.071, with a significant decrease in PA for the stress group (p=.002) and a nonsignificant decrease for the no stress group (p>.05; see Table 1). For NA, there was a main effect of Time, F (1, 54) = 12.32, p <.001, η 2 =.186, and stress, F (1.54) = 29.82, p<.001, η 2 =.356. NA was higher in the stress group and higher at time 2 compared with time 1. Further, there as a significant time stress interaction effect, F (1.54)=25.64, p<.001, η 2 =.322; NA for the no stressgroup tended to reduce but not significantly (p>.05), whereas NA for the stress group increased from time 1 to time 2 (p<.001). Cardiovascular Responding Average heart rate, systolic blood pressure, and diastolic blood pressure were calculated for each of the three periods: baseline, task, and posttask. Table 1 lists means and SDs. Controlling for baseline cardiovascular responding, a series of six forward step-wise multiple regressions were conducted to examine the effect of group (stress, no stress), pretask PA and NA, PA NA, and group affect interactions on heart rate reactivity and recovery and systolic and diastolic blood pressure reactivity and recovery. There was an effect of stress group on systolic and diastolic blood pressure reactivity and recovery (see Table 2). A series of six one-sample t tests indicated no significant changes in BP or heart rate over time in the silent reading group. The public speaking group showed significant increases in systolic and diastolic BP reactivity during the task and significant decreases in systolic and diastolic BP recovery after the task (p<.05 for all four comparisons). Both pretask PA and NA accounted for significant variance in systolic reactivity and recovery. However, significant stress group affect interaction effects suggested that PA and NA were moderating systolic reactivity and recovery differently under conditions of stress versus no stress. Post hoc regression revealed that higher PA predicted with higher systolic BP reactivity for the stress group, β=.689, t=3.49, p<.005. Both higher PA, β=.558, t=3.00, p<.01, and higher NA, β=.468, t=2.86, p<.01, were significant predictors of greater systolic BP recovery. In the control group, higher pretask NA alone predicted lower systolic BP reactivity, β=.338, t= 2.20, p<.03. A stress group PA interaction effect was observed for both diastolic BP reactivity and recovery. Higher pretask PA predicted less diastolic BP reactivity (β=.653, t= 3.106, p=.005) and recovery (β=.552, t= 2.663, p=.014) in the control group. In the stress group, there were no effects of affect on diastolic reactivity or recovery. There were no effects of stress or affect on pulse reactivity and recovery. Discussion The current study examined the effects of stress on the experience of positive and negative affect (PA and NA), and the effects of affect and expectancy on cardiovascular reactivity and recovery. We found that, compared with the pretask and posttask affective experiences of a control group reading psychology journal articles, those participants who were asked to deliver a speech into a camera reported a significant increase in NA and a significant reduction in PA. However, the effect of stress on NA was far more pronounced than the effect of stress on PA, with the time stress interaction effect accounting for 32.2% of the variance in NA, but only 7.2% of the variance in PA. Table 1 Means and SDs of pulse, systolic BP, Diastolic BP, PA, and NA for stress (N=28) and no stress (N=28) groups No stress Stress Baseline Task Posttask Baseline Task Posttask Pulse (b/min) 73.71 (11.12) 71.33 (10.95) 70.94 (10.48) 80.49 (11.70) 81.90 (16.87) 76.27 (11.33) Systolic (mmhg) 99.74 (12.07) 98.75 (11.61) 98.52 (12.24) 101.49 (10.78) 128.21 (17.92) 102.00 (11.73) Diastolic (mmhg) 64.43 (7.32) 63.58 (8.01) 63.54 (7.75) 66.64 (8.14) 92.39 (21.83) 68.68 (9.91) PA 29.32 (6.28) 28.71 (7.28) 27.93 (5.12) 24.75 (7.30) NA 12.79 (3.08) 11.82 (2.91) 14.89 (3.44) 20.21 (6.60)

192 Int. J. Behav. Med. (2010) 17:189 194 Table 2 The effects of stress group, affect, and stress affect interactions on systolic and diastolic reactivity and recovery Dependent variable Predictor variables F p R 2 β p Systolic reactivity 19.68.000 Diastolic reactivity 6.74.000 Systolic recovery 27.51.000 Diastolic recovery 6.37.000 Baseline SBP 0.010.103.180 Stress 0.552.793.000 PA before 0.058.856.002 NA before 0.033.641.016 PA NA 0.000.015.864 Stress PA 0.079 1.026.000 Stress NA 0.042.717.008 Baseline DBP 0.015.129.246 Stress 0.365.654.000 PA before 0.037.699.067 NA before 0.000.023.949 PA NA 0.012.129.298 Stress PA 0.049.816.037 Stress NA 0.000.044.902 Baseline SBP 0.000.004.953 Stress 0.602.828.000 PA before 0.024.552.021 NA before 0.036.664.005 PA NA 0.004.076.332 Stress PA 0.041.741.003 Stress NA 0.053.814.001 Baseline DBP 0.001.030.788 Stress 0.292.585.000 PA Before 0.033.665.086 NA before 0.001.117.750 PA NA 0.012.131.295 Stress PA 0.056.877.027 Stress NA 0.007.299.417 Also, for participants delivering a speech to camera (N= 28), PA and NA both contributed independently to the prediction of CV reactivity and recovery. Higher pretask PA was associated with greater systolic blood pressure reactivity. Surprisingly, both higher pretask PA and higher pretask NA independently predicted swifter recovery. In the control group, higher pretask NA predicted less systolic BP reactivity, and higher baseline PA predicted less diastolic BP reactivity and recovery. These findings are interesting for a number of reasons. First, they add to the existing body of knowledge on the relationship between PA, NA, and stress when we found that, although PA decreases as NA increases during a stress episode, the magnitude of the changes accounted for by stress differ. This supports the view that, as neither the bipolar nor bivariate approach adequately explains the dynamics of affect during a stressful episode, a contextual model may be more appropriate. Consistent with this contextual view, we observed different effects of pretask affect on patterns of cardiovascular reactivity and recovery under different task conditions. How might we explain the counterintuitive finding that the more intense were positive emotions prior to delivering a speech, the greater were systolic blood pressure changes from pretask to task (i.e., SBP reactivity)? Importantly, researchers have identified different patterns of sympathetic nervous system response associated with distress and effort For example, Blascovich and Tomaka [14] have noted that an effort or challenge response is associated with increased cardiac responding

Int. J. Behav. Med. (2010) 17:189 194 193 and reduced vascular responding, whereas a distress or threat response tends to produce reduced cardiac responding and increased vascular responding. The effort/challenge pattern of physiological responding is much more likely when personal abilities are appraised as congruent with situational demands. In the current study, it may be that those participants with higher baseline PA were also those who perceived themselves as having higher self-efficacy when they entered the laboratory situation (i.e., personal abilities were appraised as congruent with situational demands). When it came to delivering their speech, they may have perceived it as more of a challenge than a threat, and this in turn would explain why they had higher systolic reactivity when compared to those with lower baseline PA. However, we did not have available a full hemodynamic profile [15], and further studies in this area need to tease apart the relationship between the hemodynamic profile and the performance profile of those who experience different amounts of both negative and positive emotions when engaging in potentially challenging or stressful tasks. Also consistent with the bivariate view, was the finding that both more positive and more negative emotion prior to the speech were associated with greater systolic blood pressure changes from task to posttask (i.e., SBP recovery). Rather than speculating about distinct or relative challenge versus stress patterns of responding, an alternative hypothesis is that when entering a situation where one is uncertain about the nature of the task to be performed, emotions associated with both perceived challenge and potential threat can be coactive. This greater state of activation could be characterized as greater emotional complexity and a readiness to engage the task and defend against any lasting impact of the stressor on physiological equilibrium. Alternatively, it might reflect a more conflicted state. After completing the task, it is conceivable that those participants who were in this conflicted state (i.e., experiencing both relatively high PA and NA simultaneously) would also be most likely to experience the greatest relief. However, if this were true, we might have expected to observe a significant PA NA interaction effect on recovery. We did not observe such an effect. As such, we found no evidence in support of the conflicted state hypothesis. We observed different effects of affect on patterns of cardiovascular responding in the control group. In particular, higher pretask NA predicted lower systolic BP reactivity, and higher baseline PA predicted lower diastolic BP reactivity and recovery. These results are not inconsistent with the pattern of results observed in the stress group, in the sense that they may represent an adaptive adjustment of cardiovascular responding for those who entered the laboratory ready for a challenging or a stressful task. In the absence of significant challenge or stress during the silent reading task, it may be that those who entered the laboratory anticipating task demands responded with a greater relative relaxation response. Again, further research using hemoodynamic profiling is needed to better understand these subtle relationships between emotions, task demands, and different patterns of cardiovascular responding associated with distress and effort. In their dynamic model of affect, Zautra and colleagues (cf. [8]) posit that the relationship between PA and NA is contextual, that is, changes as a function of ongoing events, and personality features. In our study, not only did PA and NA have independent effects on cardiovascular reactivity and cardiovascular recovery, the stress context appears to have made a difference in these relationships. Contextual models such as the DMA are needed to fully understand how affective processes may contribute to adaptive functioning. More generally, dynamic system modeling is needed to assess changing patterns of relationship between emotional responding, physiological responding, and behavioral performance [16]. Fredrickson and Lasoda [17] have suggested that a specific ratio of positive to negative affect may provide a balance of emotions under which humans flourish. However, any such search for the optimal interplay between positive and negative affective states needs to account for contextual factors that shape affective and task demands. Also, future research that examines the nature of stress reactivity itself will need to focus on the independent effects of both positive and negative affects and expectancies. A plan of research designed to map the subtle contextual dynamics in the relationship between emotions, stress, and health presents us with a formidable agenda, but the new insights gleaned will help us to better understand the conditions that maximize resilience in the face of stress. References 1. Russell JA, Carroll JM. On the bipolarity of positive and negative affect. Psychol Bull. 1999;125:611 7. 2. Watson D, Clark LA. Towards a consensual structure of mood. Psychol Bull. 1985;98:219 35. 3. Vaillant GE. Aging well: surprising guideposts to a happier life from the landmark Harvard study of adult development. Boston: Little, Brown, and Company, 2002. 4. Fredrickson BL, Levenson RW. Positive emotions speed recovery from the cardiovascular sequelae of negative emotions. Cogn Emot. 1998;12:191 220. 5. Zautra AJ. Emotions, stress and health. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2003. 6. Feldman PJ, Cohen S, Lepore SJ, Matthews KA, Kamarack TW, Marsland AL. Negative emotions and acute physiological responses to stress. Annals Behav Med. 1999;21:216 22. 7. Maier KJ, Waldstein SR, Synowski SJ. Relation of cognitive appraisal to cardiovascular reactivity, affect, and task management. Annals Behav Med. 2003;26:32 41.

194 Int. J. Behav. Med. (2010) 17:189 194 8. Reich JW, Zautra AJ, Davis M. Dimensions of the affect relationship: models and their implications. Rev Gen Psychol. 2003;7:66 83. 9. Stroud LR, Niagura RS, Stoney CM. Sex differences in cardiovascular reactivity to physical appearance and performance challenges. Int J Behav Med. 2001;8:240 50. 10. Speilberger CD, Gorsuch DL, Lushene RE. Manual for the state-trait anxiety inventory. Palo Alto: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1970. 11. James JE, Gregg ME. Hemodynamic effects of dietary caffeine, sleep restriction, and laboratory stress. Psychophysiology. 2004;41 (6):914 23. 12. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988;54:1063 70. 13. Hughes JW, Stoney CM. Depressed mood is related to highfrequency heart-rate variability during stressors. Psychosom Med. 2000;62:796 803. 14. Blascovich J, Tomaka J. The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. 1996;28:1 51. 15. Gregg ME, Matyas TA, James JE. A new model of individual differences in hemodynamic profile and blood pressure reactivity. Psychophysiology. 2002;39(1):64 72. 16. Chow SM, Ram N, Boker SM, Fujita F, Clore G. Emotion as a thermostat: representing emotion regulation using a damped oscillator model. Emotion. 2005;5(2):208 25. 17. Fredrickson BL, Losada MF. Positive affect and the complex dynamics of human flourishing. Am Psychol. 2005;60:678 86.

Copyright of International Journal of Behavioral Medicine is the property of Springer Science & Business Media B.V. and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.