A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

Similar documents
AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

Evaluation of Salient Stimulus Cues During. Brief Functional Analyses in a Classroom Setting. Kelly L. McConnell. B.S., University of Florida

Functional Communication Training with Demand Fading. To Reduce Self-Injurious Behavior. Introduction

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI

Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1. An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

ESCAPE BEHAVIOR DURING ACADEMIC TASKS: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS OF IDIOSYNCRATIC ESTABLISHING OPERATIONS JENNIFER MCCOMAS

REINFORCEMENT SCHEDULE THINNING FOLLOWING TREATMENT WITH FUNCTIONAL COMMUNICATION TRAINING GREGORY P. HANLEY, BRIAN A. IWATA, AND RACHEL H.

SIDE EFFECTS OF EXTINCTION: PREVALENCE OF BURSTING AND AGGRESSION DURING THE TREATMENT OF SELF-INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR DOROTHEA C.

Research. News and Notes About Scientific Research on ASD and Other Developmental and Behavioral Disorders.

Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative. Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency. A Thesis Presented

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

BRIEF (TEST-CONTROL) FUNCTIONAL ANALYSIS AND TREATMENT EVALUATION OF AGGRESSIVE BEHAVIOR EVOKED BY DIVIDED ATTENTION

NEW ENGLAND CENTER FOR CHILDREN NORTHEASTERN UNIVERSITY

USING PERCENTILE SCHEDULES TO INCREASE EYE CONTACT IN CHILDREN WITH FRAGILE X SYNDROME SCOTT S. HALL, NATALEE P. MAYNES, AND ALLAN L.

6/11/2017. Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism. stereotypy. Gregory P. Hanley Ph.D.

A comparison of response-contingent and noncontingent pairing in the conditioning of a reinforcer

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL

E-01 Use interventions based on manipulation of antecedents, such as motivating operations and discriminative stimuli.

The Persistence-Strengthening Effects of DRA: An Illustration of Bidirectional Translational Research

The Effects of Video Modeling on New Staff Training. of Discrete Trial Instruction. A thesis presented. Tamarra Forbes

A PROCEDURE TO TEACH SELF-CONTROL TO CHILDREN WITH ATTENTION DEFICIT HYPERACTIVITY DISORDER LISA M. BINDER AND MARK R. DIXON PATRICK M.

MAKING IT WORK AS AN ABA THERAPIST

What Triggers Disruptive Behaviors? 10/13/18. Practical Ways to Decrease Disruptive Behavior. Background. Lynn Kern Koegel, PhD, CCC-SLP

Reinforcement Systems for a Successful Classroom

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

Running Head: BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF CHOICE. A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Choice. A Thesis Presented. Jamie Leigh Lebowitz

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

Teaching Language to Young Children. Functional Analysis and Intervention

Teaching a Scanning Response to a Child with Autism

The Effects of a CABAS Conditioning Procedure To Increase Appropriate Toy Play and Decrease Stereotypy

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE AND KRIS M. KEENEY GWINNETT COUNTY PUBLIC SCHOOLS, GEORGIA

Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

BACB Fourth Edition Task List Assessment Form

Reinforcer Strength in Transfer of Stimulus Control From Tacts to Mands

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF EXCESSIVE STRAIGHTENING AND DESTRUCTIVE BEHAVIOR IN AN ADOLESCENT DIAGNOSED WITH AUTISM DAVID E. KUHN

MANDS FOR INFORMATION USING WHO? AND WHICH? IN THE PRESENCE OF ESTABLISHING AND ABOLISHING OPERATIONS M. ALICE SHILLINGSBURG

Functional Behavior Assessment: The beginning of a Function-Based Behavioral Approach to Eliminating Restraint and Seclusion

ABSTRACT. For Whom Have FCT Interventions Been Developed? 16 FCT REVIEW

EFFECTS OF AND PREFERENCE FOR CONDITIONS OF TOKEN EARN VERSUS TOKEN LOSS JEANNE M. DONALDSON ALYSSA B. FISHER

Functional Assessment and Analysis. Functional Analysis and Positive Behavior Support for Adults with Disabilities: Part Two 4/27/2016

PASS4TEST. IT Certification Guaranteed, The Easy Way! We offer free update service for one year

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman

The University of Iowa

Determining Preference for Social Interaction. A Thesis Presented. Hillary S. Balog. In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

EFFECTS OF ALTERNATIVE RESPONSES ON BEHAVIOR EXPOSED TO NONCONTINGENT REINFORCEMENT JAVIER VIRUES-ORTEGA BRIAN A. IWATA TARA A. FAHMIE JILL M.

Running head: EVALUATING INHIBITORY STIMULUS CONTROL. Increasing the Reinforcing Efficacy of Low-Preference Teachers and Caretakers

Establishing Operations and the Mand

FA and RX Workbook (2017) by Gregory P. Hanley, Ph.D., BCBA-D

USING VIDEO MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT TO TEACH PERSPECTIVE-TAKING SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM LINDA A. LEBLANC AND ANDREA M.

ASSESSMENT AND TREATMENT OF SOCIAL AVOIDANCE JILL M. HARPER

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

CHAPTER 15 SKINNER'S OPERANT ANALYSIS 4/18/2008. Operant Conditioning

RESPONSE PATTERNS IN FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES: A PRELIMINARY ANALYSIS. Christine M. Gibson, B.S. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

MARCUS INSTITUTE AND EMORY UNIVERSITY SCHOOL OF MEDICINE

Chapter 7 Behavior and Social Cognitive Approaches

AN EVALUATION OF THE EFFECTS OF REINFORCER MAGNITUDE ON PREFERENCE AND ON-TASK BEHAVIOR. Brooke A. Jones

AN INVESTIGATION OF DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT OF ALTERNATIVE BEHAVIOR WITHOUT EXTINCTION ELIZABETH S. ATHENS TIMOTHY R. VOLLMER

Effects of delays to response blocking when used as treatment for problem behavior maintained by automatic reinforcement

It is important to recognize that DTT in itself is not a good or bad method, and like any teaching method, it may be implemented on a continuum from

FA and RX Workbook (2017) by Gregory P. Hanley, Ph.D., BCBA-D

Addressing Stereotypy The Importance of a Balanced Approach to this Core Symptom of Autism

Running Head: VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR STUDENTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

TEST-SPECIFIC CONTROL CONDITIONS FOR FUNCTIONAL ANALYSES TARA A. FAHMIE BRIAN A. IWATA ANGIE C. QUERIM JILL M. HARPER

12/19/2016. Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview. What is the Autism Spectrum? Autism Spectrum Disorder

Autism Spectrum Disorders & Positive Behavior Supports a brief overview

THE EFFECTS OF MAGNITUDE AND QUALITY OF REINFORCEMENT ON CHOICE RESPONDING DURING PLAY ACTIVITIES HANNAH HOCH NICKY FARANDA SHAYNA L.

APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS (ABA) THE LOVAAS METHODS LECTURE NOTE

The Effect of Choice-Making Opportunities during Activity Schedules on Task Engagement of Adults with Autism

GENERATING VARIABLE AND RANDOM SCHEDULES OF REINFORCEMENT USING MICROSOFT EXCEL MACROS STACIE L. BANCROFT AND JASON C. BOURRET

The Effects of Question-Present Versus Item Present Conditions on Acquisition of Mands

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

A TRANSLATIONAL EVALUATION OF THE INTERPOLATED REINFORCEMENT PROCEDURE WITH YOUNG CHILDREN

Jason Garner, M.A. ABA Clinical Director

Idiosyncratic Functions: Severe Problem Behavior Maintained by Access to Ritualistic Behaviors

POSITIVE REINFORCEMENT AS TREATMENT FOR PROBLEM BEHAVIOR MAINTAINED BY NEGATIVE REINFORCEMENT STEVEN W. PAYNE AND CLAUDIA L.

THE EFFECTS OF BEHAVIORAL SKILLS TRAINING ON CAREGIVER IMPLEMENTATION OF GUIDED COMPLIANCE NICHOLAS I. MILES AND DAVID A. WILDER

Instrumental Conditioning I

INTRODUCING NEW STIMULI IN FADING

Self-Management and Functional Analysis

USING A SELF-CONTROL TRAINING PROCEDURE TO INCREASE APPROPRIATE BEHAVIOR MARK R. DIXON AND LINDA J. HAYES

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli.

VB-MAPP Barriers Assessment

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

National University of Ireland, Maynooth

Managing Challenging Behavior. Explain - What is the problem? Reason - What is he/she getting out of it or avoiding? Access or Avoid = reinforce

Pairing & Manding. Vincent J. Carbone Ed.D., BCBA-D NYS Licensed Behavior Analyst

An Evaluation of the Value and Conditioning of Choice as a Reinforcer for Typically. Developing Children. Julie Ann Ackerlund Brandt

INCREASING INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICIENCY BY PRESENTING ADDITIONAL STIMULI IN LEARNING TRIALS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS JASON C.

The Relationship Between Motivating Operations & Behavioral Variability Penn State Autism Conference 8/3/2016

Evaluation of DRO Schedules to Reduce Disruptive Behavior in a Preschool Classroom

REINFORCEMENT OF PROBE RESPONSES AND ACQUISITION OF STIMULUS CONTROL IN FADING PROCEDURES

Transcription:

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2008, 41, 125 130 NUMBER 1(SPRING 2008) A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER LOUISIANA STATE UNIVERSITY GREGORY P. HANLEY WESTERN NEW ENGLAND COLLEGE AND KYLIE M. LARSEN UNIVERSITY OF KANSAS Multiple schedules using continuous discriminative stimuli have been used to minimize children s disruptive requesting for teacher attention (e.g., colored floral leis; Tiger & Hanley, 2004; Tiger, Hanley, & Heal, 2006). The present study evaluated the effectiveness of, and children s preferences for, two multiple-schedule arrangements in which brief experimenter vocalizations served as discriminative stimuli. Results showed that brief signals were highly effective discriminative stimuli for 3 of the 4 children, and that all children preferred one or both variations of the multiple schedule to a control arrangement. For 1 child, highly discriminated responding was achieved only when continuous signals were introduced. DESCRIPTORS: signals, choice, concurrent-chains arrangement, multiple-schedule arrangements, preference assessment, preschool Portions of this paper were submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the PhD degree by the first author. We thank Rachel Thompson, Gregory Madden, and Joseph Spradlin for their helpful comments on earlier versions of this manuscript. Address correspondence to Jeffrey Tiger, Department of Psychology, 218 Audubon Hall, Louisiana State University, Baton Rouge, Louisiana 70803 (e-mail: jtiger@ lsu.edu). doi: 10.1901/jaba.2008.41-125 In preschool classrooms, children who recruit their teacher s attention at inappropriate times may disrupt teaching sequences and limit their peers learning opportunities. Recently, we have described a procedure to promote discrimination of periods in which teacher attention was or was not available (Cammilleri,Tiger,&Hanley,inpress;Tiger & Hanley, 2004, 2005; Tiger, Hanley, & Heal, 2006) by including salient visual stimuli correlated with these periods. This procedure, termed a multiple schedule (Ferster & Skinner, 1957), involved teachers wearing one colored lei when their attention was available, and a second colored lei or no lei when their attention was not available. This procedure has been shown to be effective for reducing the illtimed requesting of both preschool-aged and elementary-aged children during group instruction (Cammilleri et al.). Our current research efforts are focused on evaluating variations of this multiple-schedule procedure to address practical limitations. For instance, one study found that the presentation of an extinction-correlated stimulus might negatively affect the educational context in which the procedure is implemented (Tiger et al., 2006). In this study, children s preferences for two multiple-schedule arrangements were assessed relative to a control arrangement. In one multiple schedule, an explicit signal was present during both reinforcement (S+) and extinction (S2) periods. In the other multiple 125

126 JEFFREY H. TIGER et al. schedule, an explicit signal was present during reinforcement periods but no explicit signal was present during extinction periods (i.e., the absence of the S+ served as the S2). In the control arrangement, one signal was provided during both reinforcement and extinction periods (i.e., an uncorrelated signal). Four children in this study preferred the S+ only arrangement, suggesting that the S2 stimulus had acquired some aversive properties. The inclusion of S2 during instructional situations then may evoke avoidance or escape behavior (e.g., children may not attend available instructional opportunities due to the S2). The use of floral leis as discriminative stimuli also may limit the practicality of the multipleschedule procedure. Floral leis were selected initially because they were inexpensive, portable, and visually salient from all perspectives. These putative advantages may be overshadowed by the fact that the procedure also requires teachers to have continuous access to one or two discriminative stimuli per student, replace lost or damaged stimuli, and introduce artificial stimuli into the classroom. The use of more naturally occurring discriminative stimuli, such as teacher vocalizations, could obviate these limitations. The purpose of the present study was to evaluate the effectiveness of multiple schedules in which brief vocal signals were programmed as discriminative stimuli. Based on the results of Tiger et al. (2006), we evaluated two variants of this procedure. The first involved the presentation of vocal discriminative stimuli during both reinforcement and extinction periods. The second involved the presentation of a vocal discriminative stimulus during reinforcement periods only. We also assessed children s preferences for these variations as an indicator of the social validity of the procedures. Finally, we compared the effectiveness of the briefly signaled multiple schedule with a continuously signaled multiple schedule for one participant. METHOD Participants and Setting Participants were selected from a full-day university-based inclusive preschool that served children from 2 years 6 months to 5 years 6 months old of typical and atypical development. At the beginning of the study, Alice was 3 years old, Quincy was 4 years old, Lana was 4 years old, and Trent was 5 years old. Sessions were conducted in a small room designed to simulate classroom periods in which a teacher provided instruction (i.e., children were provided with developmentally appropriate materials while seated across from the experimenter). Procedure Each session was conducted using a concurrent-chains design similar to that described by Tiger et al. (2006). That is, each session consisted of one initial-link response opportunity, in which a participant stood outside the session room door and selected one of three different colored cards (10 cm by 10 cm), each of which led to a distinct 3-min terminal link, in which a participant sat across from an experimenter in the simulated classroom. In each terminal link, 15-, 30-, and 45-s periods of reinforcement and extinction alternated on time-based schedules. During reinforcement periods, the experimenter diverted his attention from the child (e.g., looked down or away) except when providing a brief statement of attention contingent on each social approach by the child. During extinction periods, the experimenter also diverted his attention but did not respond to any social approach. The three terminal links differed only in regards to discriminative-stimulus presentation. During S+/S2 links, the experimenter said, It is your time at the onset of each reinforcement period and It is my time at the onset of each extinction period. During S+ links, the experimenter said, It is your time at the onset of each reinforcement period but did not say anything at the onset of each extinction period.

BRIEFLY SIGNALED MULTIPLE SCHEDULE 127 During mixed links, the experimenter did not say anything at the onset of reinforcement or extinction components. The mixed link allowed evaluation of responding in the absence of schedule-correlated discriminative stimuli (i.e., a control condition for the S+/S2 and S+) and identification of children s preferences for the presence (S+/S2 and S+) or absence (mixed) of programmed discriminative stimuli. Each participant experienced two distinct phases during his or her evaluation. The forcedchoice phase was designed to assess the relative effectiveness of the S+/S2, S+, and mixed arrangements for promoting discriminated social approaches. During this phase s initial links, the experimenter vocally prompted the child to select specific cards in a random and counterbalanced order, with one or two exposures to each terminal link daily (i.e., a total of three or six sessions were conducted daily). Thus, the three conditions were alternated in a multielement design during this phase. This phase continued until participants engaged in discriminated responding (i.e., elevated responding during reinforcement components relative to extinction components) for six consecutive sessions in one condition. The free-choice phase of the evaluation was designed to assess children s preferences for the three arrangements in the terminal links. During the initial links in this phase, participants were told, Choose the card you like. Children then experienced the terminal link that was correlated with their selected card. Terminal-link contingencies were identical to those described during the initial phase. An arrangement was considered preferred if (a) it had been selected on six consecutive opportunities or (b) it had been selected on six opportunities more than either of the other two options. After either of these preference criteria were met, the preferred arrangement was removed from the array (e.g., the card associated with the preferred link was removed), such that a preference could then be determined for the remaining two arrangements. The same preference criteria again were applied; the evaluation was terminated following a determination of a preference hierarchy. A reversal design was used for Alice to compare the relative efficacy of briefly signaled and continuously signaled multiple schedules (similar to those described by Tiger et al., 2006). Sessions were identical to those described above except that during the continuous S+/S2 condition, the experimenter wore a red floral lei throughout each reinforcement period and a blue floral lei throughout each extinction period. Response Measurement and Interobserver Agreement During the initial links, card selection was defined as handing a card to the experimenter. During the terminal links, children s social approaches were scored as any vocal (e.g., Look at what I built ) or nonvocal (e.g., waving a hand while making eye contact) behavior directed towards the experimenter. Interobserver agreement was assessed by having a second observer simultaneously but independently score children s responding during at least 23% of session across participants (range, 23% to 42%). Card selection was never scored in disagreement. Social-approach agreement was determined by using the block-byblock method (i.e., total agreement within 10-s intervals). Mean agreement was 88% for Alice, 91% for Quincy, 98% for Lana, and 99% for Trent. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION Figure 1 depicts participants responding. All children were observed to engage in discriminated responding during both the S+/S2 and S+ conditions when brief vocal stimuli were programmed as discriminative stimuli, with 3 of the 4 children exhibiting near-zero levels of extinction responding under both arrangements, suggesting that brief vocal stimuli during multiple schedules may be a practical and

128 JEFFREY H. TIGER et al. Figure 1. Participants data are depicted in columns, and each terminal-link arrangement is depicted in the top three rows. Graphs in the top row depict responding under the S+/S2 arrangement, graphs in the second row depict responding under the S+ arrangement, and graphs in the third row depict responding under the mixed arrangement. Each graph includes a discrimination index (DI) from the forced-choice phase of the analysis and two data paths, responding during reinforcement (white circles) and extinction (black circles) components. The bottom row depicts the cumulative initial-link selections during the preference assessment. The point at which a data path stops represents the restriction of that link (i.e., a preference had been identified and that option was no longer presented during the initial links). Terminal-link data were lost for two sessions (Lana, Session 14, S+; Trent, Session 33, S+) due to computer failures.

BRIEFLY SIGNALED MULTIPLE SCHEDULE 129 Figure 2. Responding during reinforcement and extinction components of the S+/S2 arrangement when component changes were signaled continuously or signaled briefly at the onset of each component change for Alice. effective alternative to continuous visual stimuli (e.g., floral leis) when arranging multiple schedules. Comparisons of discrimination indexes for the two briefly signaled multipleschedule variations showed that the S+/S2 arrangement was more effective than the S+ only arrangement for 3 of the 4 children. The bottom panel of Figure 1 shows that all 4 children preferred the S+/S2 arrangement relative to the S+ only arrangement (i.e., they met preference and restriction criteria for the S+/S2 arrangement during the free-choice phase). Because extinction responding persisted for Alice, a continuously signaled multiple schedule was compared to the briefly signaled arrangement (Figure 2) and was found to result in a greater decrease (to zero levels) in responding during extinction. Several of these findings differ from previous research, albeit in understandable ways. First, Tiger et al. (2006) reported that the continuously signaled S+/S2 and S+ resulted in nearly equivalent patterns of discriminated social approaches (i.e., equal discrimination indexes). The differences in the two studies likely can be attributed to the discriminative properties of the offset of the S+. That is, under continuous S+ conditions, the removal of the S+ signaled the onset of an extinction component whereas under the briefly signaled S+ condition, no stimulus change occurred at the onset of an extinction component; hence, children were more likely to continue responding in the extinction component. Second, Tiger et al. (2006) reported that 4 of 7 children preferred the S+ multiple schedule relative to the S+/S2 multiple schedule. The results of that study suggested that preference for the S+ condition was likely to develop when children had a history of high levels of responding during extinction components relative to reinforcement components (i.e., the history of contacting extinction in the presence of the S2 decreased children s preference for the S+/S2 condition; see also Mulvaney, Dinsmoor, Jwaideh, & Hughes, 1974; Terrace, 1971). Given that S2 stimuli were presented briefly in the present study, it is likely that responding rarely occurred in the presence of the S2; thus, an aversion to the S2 probably never developed. It is also possible that the availability of reinforcement was more predictable during the brief S+/S2 condition relative to the brief S+ condition, due to the consistent signal at the onset of extinction periods. This increased predictability may have offset any devaluing of the S+/S2 condition that resulted from a history of responding during extinction. Although the brief multiple-schedule procedure resulted in discriminated responding for 3 of 4 children, a continuously signaled multiple schedule was necessary to minimize responding during extinction for Alice. The range of conditions under which continuously presented discriminative stimuli are necessary were not identified in the current study; however, persistent rates of responding during extinction may be one set of conditions in which continuous signals should be considered in lieu of brief signals. In addition, the relative efficacy of and children s preferences for continuous versus briefly presented schedule-correlated stimuli were not evaluated. This represents an interesting area of further research. The results of the present study suggest that brief multiple schedules may be a suitable and

130 JEFFREY H. TIGER et al. practical procedure for teaching children when their teacher s attention is or is not available. However, given that the current study was conducted in a simulated classroom by an experimenter, claims regarding the extension of these findings to preschool classrooms are tentative; replications by teachers in typical classroom settings are now needed. REFERENCES Cammilleri, A. P., Tiger, J. H., & Hanley, G. P. (in press). Developing stimulus control of young children s requests to teachers: Classwide applications of multiple schedules. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis. Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of reinforcement. Acton, MA: Copley. Mulvaney, D. E., Dinsmoor, J. A., Jwaideh, A. R., & Hughes, L. H. (1974). Punishment of observing by the negative discriminative stimulus. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 21, 37 44. Terrace, H. S. (1971). Escape from S2. Learning and Motivation, 2, 148 163. Tiger, J. H., & Hanley, G. P. (2004). Developing stimulus control of preschooler mands: An analysis of schedulecorrelated and contingency-specifying stimuli. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 37, 517 521. Tiger, J. H., & Hanley, G. P. (2005). An example of discovery research involving the transfer of stimulus control. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 38, 499 509. Tiger, J. H., Hanley, G. P., & Heal, N. A. (2006). The effectiveness of and preschoolers preferences for variations of multiple-schedule arrangements. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 39, 475 488. Received August 15, 2006 Final acceptance November 17, 2006 Action Editor, James Carr