Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fmri

Similar documents
Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Supplementary Information

A region of mesial prefrontal cortex tracks monetarily rewarding outcomes: characterization with rapid event-related fmri

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Nat Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 March 18.

Distinct valuation subsystems in the human brain for effort and delay

smokers) aged 37.3 ± 7.4 yrs (mean ± sd) and a group of twelve, age matched, healthy

Nucleus accumbens activation mediates the influence of reward cues on financial risk-taking

Introduction. The Journal of Neuroscience, January 1, (1):

Dissociating Valence of Outcome from Behavioral Control in Human Orbital and Ventral Prefrontal Cortices

Valence and salience contribute to nucleus accumbens activation

Supporting online material. Materials and Methods. We scanned participants in two groups of 12 each. Group 1 was composed largely of

Reward Systems: Human

Neurally reconstructing expected utility

Supplemental Information. Triangulating the Neural, Psychological, and Economic Bases of Guilt Aversion

Distinguishing informational from value-related encoding of rewarding and punishing outcomes in the human brain

Twelve right-handed subjects between the ages of 22 and 30 were recruited from the

HHS Public Access Author manuscript Eur J Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 August 10.

Supplementary Information Methods Subjects The study was comprised of 84 chronic pain patients with either chronic back pain (CBP) or osteoarthritis

Motivation-dependent Responses in the Human Caudate Nucleus

An fmri study of reward-related probability learning

Supporting Online Material for

The effects of intranasal oxytocin on reward circuitry responses in children with autism spectrum disorder

The Inherent Reward of Choice. Lauren A. Leotti & Mauricio R. Delgado. Supplementary Methods

Methods to examine brain activity associated with emotional states and traits

Sensitivity of the nucleus accumbens to violations in expectation of reward

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

Functional magnetic resonance imaging of reward prediction Brian Knutson and Jeffrey C. Cooper

Functional topography of a distributed neural system for spatial and nonspatial information maintenance in working memory

Are face-responsive regions selective only for faces?

Cultural objects modulate reward circuitry

Testing the Reward Prediction Error Hypothesis with an Axiomatic Model

WHAT DOES THE BRAIN TELL US ABOUT TRUST AND DISTRUST? EVIDENCE FROM A FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING STUDY 1

SUPPLEMENTARY METHODS. Subjects and Confederates. We investigated a total of 32 healthy adult volunteers, 16

Distinct Value Signals in Anterior and Posterior Ventromedial Prefrontal Cortex

Hallucinations and conscious access to visual inputs in Parkinson s disease

Neural evidence for cultural differences in the valuation of positive facial expressions

The Neural Basis of Financial Risk Taking

The Neural Basis of Economic Decision- Making in The Ultimatum Game

Functional Dissociation in Frontal and Striatal Areas for Processing of Positive and Negative Reward Information

Prefrontal dysfunction in drug addiction: Cause or consequence? Christa Nijnens

Investigating directed influences between activated brain areas in a motor-response task using fmri

HST.583 Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging: Data Acquisition and Analysis Fall 2006

From affective value to decision-making in the prefrontal cortex

Role of the ventral striatum in developing anorexia nervosa

Contributions of the prefrontal cortex to the neural basis of human decision making

Activation in the VTA and nucleus accumbens increases in anticipation of both gains and losses

Psychological Science

Title: Falsifying the Reward Prediction Error Hypothesis with an Axiomatic Model. Abbreviated title: Testing the Reward Prediction Error Model Class

Dissociable but inter-related systems of cognitive control and reward during decision making: Evidence from pupillometry and event-related fmri

Predictive Neural Coding of Reward Preference Involves Dissociable Responses in Human Ventral Midbrain and Ventral Striatum

Comparing event-related and epoch analysis in blocked design fmri

Supplemental Data. Inclusion/exclusion criteria for major depressive disorder group and healthy control group

Contributions of the Amygdala to Reward Expectancy and Choice Signals in Human Prefrontal Cortex

An fmri Study of Risky Decision Making: The Role of Mental Preparation and Conflict

Functional MRI Mapping Cognition

Neuroimaging vs. other methods

The application of modern cognitive neuroscience methods

Supporting Information. Demonstration of effort-discounting in dlpfc

NeuroImage 54 (2011) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. NeuroImage. journal homepage:

Supplementary Online Content

The Neuroscience of Addiction: A mini-review

Neural effects of positive and negative incentives during marijuana withdrawal

Different neural systems adjust motor behavior in response to reward and punishment

SUPPLEMENT: DYNAMIC FUNCTIONAL CONNECTIVITY IN DEPRESSION. Supplemental Information. Dynamic Resting-State Functional Connectivity in Major Depression

For better or for worse: neural systems supporting the cognitive down- and up-regulation of negative emotion

Investigations in Resting State Connectivity. Overview

RETRACTED. Motivated Cognitive Control: Reward Incentives Modulate Preparatory Neural Activity during Task-Switching

There are, in total, four free parameters. The learning rate a controls how sharply the model

Reward representations and reward-related learning in the human brain: insights from neuroimaging John P O Doherty 1,2

Prefrontal cortex and recognition memory Functional-MRI evidence for context-dependent retrieval processes

The Anterior Cingulate and Error Avoidance

THE BASAL GANGLIA AND TRAINING OF ARITHMETIC FLUENCY. Andrea Ponting. B.S., University of Pittsburgh, Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of

Selective attention to emotional stimuli in a verbal go/no-go task: an fmri study

FINAL PROGRESS REPORT

Supplementary Online Content

QUANTIFYING CEREBRAL CONTRIBUTIONS TO PAIN 1

9/13/2018. Neurobiological Aspects of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) DSM-5 Diagnostic Criteria

Reward, Context, and Human Behaviour

ABSTRACT. Title of Document: NEURAL CORRELATES OF APPROACH AND AVOIDANCE LEARNING IN BEHAVIORAL INHIBITION. Sarah M. Helfinstein, Ph.D.

Supporting Information

Effects Of Attention And Perceptual Uncertainty On Cerebellar Activity During Visual Motion Perception

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIALS: Appetitive and aversive goal values are encoded in the medial orbitofrontal cortex at the time of decision-making

Hippocampal brain-network coordination during volitionally controlled exploratory behavior enhances learning

Supplementary Online Content

Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders 2012, 2:11

Supplementary information Detailed Materials and Methods

Supplementary Online Content

SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL. Table. Neuroimaging studies on the premonitory urge and sensory function in patients with Tourette syndrome.

Supplementary Methods and Results

Performance Feedback Drives Caudate Activation in a Phonological Learning Task

The effects of single-trial averaging upon the spatial extent of fmri activation

Presupplementary Motor Area Activation during Sequence Learning Reflects Visuo-Motor Association

5th Mini-Symposium on Cognition, Decision-making and Social Function: In Memory of Kang Cheng

Overt Verbal Responding during fmri Scanning: Empirical Investigations of Problems and Potential Solutions

Reviews and Overviews. Mechanisms of Psychiatric Illness

Do women with fragile X syndrome have problems in switching attention: Preliminary findings from ERP and fmri

Psych3BN3 Topic 4 Emotion. Bilateral amygdala pathology: Case of S.M. (fig 9.1) S.M. s ratings of emotional intensity of faces (fig 9.

Temporal Prediction Errors in a Passive Learning Task Activate Human Striatum

Functional MRI study of gender effects in brain activations during verbal working

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

Transcription:

BRAIN IMAGING Dissociation of reward anticipation and outcome with event-related fmri Brian Knutson, 1,CA Grace W. Fong, Charles M. Adams, Jerald L. Varner and Daniel Hommer National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and alcoholism, National Institutes of Health, 1 Center Drive, Bldg. 1, Rm. 6S24, Bethesda, MD, 2892-161; 1 Department of Psychology, Bldg. 42, Jordan Hall, Stanford, CA 9435-213, USA CA Corresponding Author Received 2 August 21; accepted 14 September 21 Reward processing involves both appetitive and consummatory phases. We sought to examine whether reward anticipation vs outcomes would recruit different regions of ventral forebrain circuitry using event-related fmri. Nine healthy volunteers participated in a monetary incentive delays task in which they either responded to a cued target for monetary reward, responded to a cued target for no reward, or did not respond to a cued target during scanning. Multiple regression analyses indicated that while anticipation of reward vs non-reward activated foci in the ventral striatum, reward vs non-reward outcomes activated foci in the ventromedial frontal cortex. These ndings suggest that reward anticipation and outcomes may differentially recruit distinct regions that lie along the trajectory of ascending dopamine projections. NeuroReport 12:3683±3687 & 21 Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Key words: Reward; Anticipation; Outcome; Nucleus accumbens; Ventromedial frontal cortex; fmri; Human INTRODUCTION Ethologists have traditionally distinguished between appetitive and consummatory stages of reward processing [1,2]. In addition to occurring at different timepoints (i.e., reward anticipation vs outcome) and potentially involving qualitatively different affective phenomenology (i.e., high vs low arousal) [3], recent animal studies suggest that appetitive and consummatory stages of reward may recruit distinct neuroanatomical and neurochemical mechanisms [4]. While brain imaging studies have begun to demonstrate that tasks involving monetary reward can recruit both ventral striatal and frontal activity [5±1], it is unclear whether different parts of this circuitry are involved in distinct stages of reward processing in humans. Primate electrophysiological studies suggest that ventral striatal regions such as the nucleus accumbens (NAcc) show neural activity as monkeys anticipate making a response for a reward, but the ventromedial (VMFC) and orbital frontal cortex (OFC) subsequently become active after the monkey has responded and receives the reward [11]. At present, only two fmri studies have experimentally separated anticipatory from outcome phases of reward processing. In a study focusing exclusively on anticipation of monetary reward, we found that the NAcc showed reward-proportional activity during anticipation of increasing monetary rewards but not punishments. However, detectable activity was not observed in the VMFC or OFC during reward anticipation [3]. Concurrently, another group observed a similar pattern of ventral striatal activation in addition to VMFC and OFC activation during anticipation of monetary reward using a different task and analytic technique. They also reported that this pattern of activation did not change appreciably during receipt of reward outcomes [12]. Using event-related fmri, we examined whether reward outcomes would recruit different brain regions than reward anticipation. Based on our prior ndings, we predicted that the NAcc would be activated more by anticipation of responding for a reward than by anticipation of responding for no reward. Based on primate research, we also predicted that the VMFC or OFC would be more activated by presentation of reward outcomes than non-reward outcomes [11]. MATERIALS AND METHODS Nine physically and psychiatrically healthy volunteers (seven women, right-handed, mean ( s.d.) age 26.45 5.85) participated in the study. Before entering the scanner, participants completed a practice version of the task and were shown the money that they could earn by performing the task successfully. Once in the scanner, anatomical and functional scans were collected. Participants engaged in a 5 min 24 s session of the monetary incentive delay (MID) task during functional scan acquisition [7]. All participants gave written informed consent, and the study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism. MID task: The MID reward task session consisted of 54 6 s trials. During each trial, participants saw one of three colored squares (cue, 25 ms), then xated on a cross-hair as they waited a variable interval (delay, 2±25 ms), 959-4965 & Lippincott Williams & Wilkins Vol 12 No 17 4 December 21 3683

B. KNUTSON ET AL. and nally responded to a white target square which appeared for a variable length of time (target, 16±26 ms) with a button press. Feedback (outcome, 165 ms) which followed the target's disappearance noti ed participants whether they had won money during that trial and indicated their cumulative total at that point. fmri volume acquisitions were time-locked to the offset of each cue and thus were acquired during anticipatory as well as during outcome periods [7]. Cues signaled potentially rewarded response (n ˆ 18, yellow squares), an unrewarded response requirement (n ˆ 18, blue squares), or no response requirement (n ˆ 18, red squares). In the reward trials, participants won $1. if they pressed a button during display of the subsequent target. Target durations were adjusted such that each participant would succeed on approximately 66% of his or her responses, based on reaction times obtained during the prescan practice session. In the non-rewarded response trials, participants did not win money for responding, but were still asked to rapidly press the button during display of the subsequent target. In the non-response trials, participants were asked to refrain from pressing the button during display of the subsequent target. fmri acquisition: Imaging was performed using a 1.5 T General Electric MRI scanner with a standard quadrature head coil. Sixteen contiguous sagittal 7 mm slices (in-plane resolution 3.75 3 3.75 mm) centered about the intra-hemispheric ssure were acquired, providing whole brain coverage, including subcortical regions of interest such as the NAcc, as well as the VMFC and rostral OFC. Only the posterior OFC (Brodmann's area (BA) 25) showed. 6% signal dropout due to proximity to tissue boundaries. Functional scans were acquired using a T2 -sensitive gradient echo sequence that measured changes in blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) contrast with parameters of repetition time (TR) ˆ 2 ms, echo time (TE) ˆ 4 ms, ip ˆ 98, number of volumes ˆ 162. Whole-brain structural scans were acquired using a T1-weighted spoiled grass pulse sequence (TR ˆ 1 ms; TE ˆ 7 ms, ip ˆ 98,.9375 3.9375 3 2 mm voxels), which facilitated localization and co-registration of functional data. fmri analysis: Analyses focused on changes in BOLD contrast that occurred during anticipatory and outcome periods and were conducted using Analysis of Functional Neural Images (AFNI) software [13]. For preprocessing, voxel time series were interpolated to correct for nonsimultaneous slice acquisition within each volume, corrected for three-dimensional motion, and slightly spatially smoothed (kernel FWHM ˆ 4 mm). Visual inspection of motion correction estimates con rmed that no participant's head moved more than 1.5 mm in any dimension from one volume acquisition to the next and that no participant overtly vocalized in response to incentive outcomes. Preprocessed timeseries data for each individual were analyzed with an orthogonal multiple regression model consisting of 12 regressors [14]. Two orthogonal regressors of interest contrasted: (1) anticipation of rewarded vs unrewarded response and (2) reward hit vs miss outcomes (reward trials only). Two orthogonal control regressors contrasted: (3) anticipation of response (both rewarded and unrewarded) vs anticipation of non-response and (4) outcomes on reward trials (both hit and miss) vs outcomes on other trials. Regressors of interest and control regressors were convolved with a gamma variate function modeling a prototypical hemodynamic response prior to inclusion in the regression model [15]. The remaining regressors modeled residual motion (six parameters) as well as a baseline and linear trend for the experimental session (two pararmeters). Maps of t-statistics for the regressors of interest and control regressors were transformed into Z-scores, coregistered with structural maps, spatially normalized by warping to Talairach space, spatially smoothed (FWHM ˆ 4 mm), and combined into a group map using a metaanalytic formula (average Z 3 p (n)) [7]. Group maps were thresholded at an omnibus value of p,.1. This threshold was based on a prior convention for multiple test correction in subcortical (i.e., NAcc, putamen, caudate, thalamus) and mesial cortical gray matter (i.e., OFC, VMFC, anterior cingulate, supplementary motor area, and primary motor cortex) regions in a representative brain (5 voxels; p,.5, corrected) [7]. Activation foci (peak values) appearing in ventral striatal, ventral forebrain, and striatal motor control volumes of interest [16] that passed this threshold were used to construct three spherical volumes of interest (VOIs) of 4 mm diameter. Averaged BOLD contrast timeseries (calculated as percent change from overall intensity mean with linear trends removed) were extracted from these spherical VOIs for each trial type and for each individual. The averaged timeseries for NAcc, VMFC, and putamen VOIs were then analyzed with 4 (trial type, within) 3 9 (epoch, within) repeated-measures ANOVA ( p,.5) Average activation levels during anticipation and outcome periods were compared across trial types in each VOI at 6 s lags using Tukey's honestly signi cant difference paired comparisons ( p,.5) [3,12]. RESULTS Behavior: Participants achieved criterion or hit (i.e. received $1.) on an average of 61. 14% of the reward trials, approximating the targeted 66% hit rate, and on an average of 5. 11%) of the non-rewarded response trials. Participants' average reaction time on rewarded hits (197.9 23.13 ms) was slightly faster than their average reaction time on non-rewarded hits (213.11 26.72 ms; t(11) ˆ 5.1, p,.1). Voxel-by-voxel analysis: The contrast of anticipation of responding for a reward vs no reward revealed a signi cant activation focus in the bilateral NAcc, as hypothesized. This contrast also revealed activation foci in the right anterior insula, bilateral caudate, left putamen, anterior thalamus right medial amygdala, mesial prefrontal cortex, supplementary motor area, left motor cortex, and right cerebellar vermis ( p,.1, uncorrected). This pattern replicated our prior ndings (Table 1) [3]. Contrast of reward vs non-reward outcomes yielded a different pattern of activation foci including VMFC (Brodmann's Area 32/1), as hypothesized. This contrast also revealed foci in the right dorsal caudate, left frontal pole 3684 Vol 12 No 17 4 December 21

DISSOCIATION OF REWARD ANTICIPATION AND OUTCOME Table 1. Group maximum Z-scores and Talairach Coordinates of activation foci ( p,.1, uncorrected; n ˆ 9). Area Ant: Reward vs non-reward response Ant: Response vs nonresponse Out: $1. reward vs $5 reward Out: all reward trials vs nonreward trials Max Z TC (R,A,S) Max Z TC (R,A,S) Max Z TC (R,A,S) Max Z TC (R,A,S) R anterior insula (BA13) 4.41 33,17, 2 L anterior insula (BA13) R NAcc 5.69 11,11, L NAcc 4.95 8,12, R caudate 5.92 11,9,3 3.89 16,17,6 L caudate 5.34 9,5,4 R putamen 5.15 24,6,2 5.5 18,1, 3 L putamen 3.97 2,1, 4 5.46 21, 1,6 Thalamus 6.29 3, 2,7 4.97 8, 17,9 4.1 1, 7,13 R amygdala 4.86 16, 7, 7 L amygdala Orbitofrontal cortex (BA1/32) 3.88 8,45, 13 4.74 4,43, 14 4.4 6,38, 9 L frontal pole (BA 1/32) 4.15 3,51,8 Anterior cingulate (BA24) Posterior cingulate (BA26/3) 4.25 3, 51,17 Parietal cortex (BA7) 4.75 4, 58,49 4.9 1, 3,52 Mesial prefrontal cortex (BA32) 3.95 5,41,12 6. 3,5,44 4,73 3,11,43 Supplementary motor area (BA6) 3.99 1, 1,48 7.27 3, 4,5 L motor cortex (BA4) 4.74 4, 15,51 7,61 36, 18,48 Cerebellar vermis 4.67 3, 59, 5 6.24 4, 6, 9 (BA 32/1), posterior cingulate, and parietal cortex ( p,.1, uncorrected). Control regressor maps yielded distinct patterns of activation. The contrast of anticipation of all motor responses vs anticipation of no response revealed foci primarily in motor preparatory regions including bilateral putamen, ventrolateral thalamus, mesial prefrontal cortex, supplementary motor area, left motor cortex, and right cerebellar vermis [7]. The contrast of all reward trial outcomes (hit miss) vs outcomes on other trials revealed an activation focus in the thalamus and deactivation foci in the VMFC (BA 32/1), right putamen, and parietal cortex ( p,.1, uncorrected). Volume of interest analysis: Analysis of individual timeseries extracted from the right NAcc VOI yielded a signi cant interaction of trial type (4) 3 epoch (9) (F(24,192) ˆ 5.28, p,.1), indicating that some types of trials differed signi cantly over time. Planned comparisons for anticipation (lag ˆ 6 s) revealed signi cantly greater NAcc activity during anticipation of reward response (on both hit and miss trials) vs anticipation of non-rewarded motor response ( p,.5) or non-response ( p,.1). Interestingly, planned comparisons of outcomes revealed signi cantly decreased NAcc activity during noti cation of non-reward (miss) but not reward (hit) outcomes relative to both types of control outcomes ( p,.5; Fig. 1). Analysis of VMFC VOI timeseries also indicated a signi cant interaction of trial type (4) 3 epoch (9) (F(24,192) ˆ 4.41, p,.5). Planned comparisons for anticipation revealed no signi cant differences between any of the trial types. However, planned comparisons of outcomes again revealed decreased VMFC activity during noti cation of non-reward (miss) but not rewarded (hit) outcomes relative to both types of control outcomes ( p,.1). Analysis of the putamen VOI timeseries also yielded a signi cant interaction of trial type (4) 3 epoch (9) (F(24,192) ˆ 4.41, p,.1). Planned comparisons for anticipation revealed increased putamen activity during anticipation of reward (on both hit and miss trials) relative to anticipation of non-response ( p,.5), but not relative to anticipation of non-rewarded response. As in other VOIs, planned comparisons of outcomes revealed decreased putamen activity during presentation of nonreward (miss) outcomes but not reward (hit) outcomes relative to both types of control outcomes ( p,.5). DISCUSSION By separately examining reward anticipation and outcomes with event-related fmri, we found that the NAcc was primarily recruited by anticipation of monetary reward, that this activation subsided during delivery of rewarding outcomes, and further, that NAcc activity was suppressed when anticipated rewards were not obtained. Reward anticipation did not activate the VMFC, but omission of anticipated rewards did suppress VMFC activity. Deactivation of these regions during reward non-delivery is consistent with primate electrophysiological recordings showing decreased midbrain dopamine neural ring during reward omission [17]. Although participants had slightly faster reaction times when responding to reward vs control targets, several lines of evidence suggest that NAcc activity was more tightly linked to anticipation of reward than motor expectancy. First, a control regressor contrasting anticipation of all motor responses vs anticipation of non-response highlighted more lateral putamen foci but not the NAcc. Vol 12 No 17 4 December 21 3685

B. KNUTSON ET AL. Reward vs Nonreward Anticipation Response vs Nonresponse Anticipation Reward vs Nonreward Outcome (n 9) R 11 R 24 R 6 A 11 A 6 A 38 1 7 1 6 1 5 NAcc Put VMFC % signal change ( sem).3.1.1.3.6 rew ant/rew out.4 rew ant/nrew out rsp ant/nrew out.1 non ant/nrew out.1.4.3.3.6 2 4 6 8 1 12 14 16 2 4 6 8 1 12 14 16 2 4 6 8 1 12 14 16 Epoch (s) Fig. 1. Group activation maps for reward anticipation vs non-reward anticipation, response vs non-response anticipation, and reward vs non-reward outcome. Timecourse plots highlight these contrasts in NAcc, Put, and VMFC volumes of interest. R ˆ right, A ˆ anterior, NAcc ˆ nucleus accumbens, Put ˆ putamen, VMFC ˆ ventromedial frontal cortex. Signi cantly different from rsp ant/non-reward out and non-ant/non-reward out; { ˆ signi cantly different from non-ant/non-reward out only ( p,.5). Second, timecourse analyses indicated that at the NAcc focus, anticipation of rewarded responses elicited signi cantly more activation than anticipation of non-rewarded responses, but at the putamen focus, anticipation of rewarded and non-rewarded responses elicited similar levels of activation. Third, in a separate study, we have demonstrated that anticipation of increasing monetary punishments, which might also reduce reaction time, does not signi cantly increase activity in the rostral NAcc [3]. Together, these results suggest that the rostral NAcc is more robustly recruited by reward anticipation than by anticipation of motor responding per se. The presently observed neuroanatomical dissociation between reward anticipation and outcome stands in contrast to ndings reported in the only other published study of this type [12]. Whereas other investigators have reported that reward expectancy and outcome both recruited NAcc, we observed NAcc activity primarily during reward anticipation. Further, while those investigators also reported that reward expectancy and outcome both recruited regions in 3686 Vol 12 No 17 4 December 21

DISSOCIATION OF REWARD ANTICIPATION AND OUTCOME the VMFC, we observed the clearest evidence of VMFC recruitment during presentation of reward outcomes only. Differences in task design may account for some of these disparities. For instance, the MID task was developed to maximize affective and motivational aspects of reward processing by employing rapid presentation of stimuli (e.g. 2 s for anticipation and outcome periods) and invoking contingency (i.e. participants perceive that their performance determines their outcome) [7]. On the other hand, the task employed in the earlier report may invoke more cognitive and deliberative aspects of reward processing, since it involves slower presentation of stimuli (e.g. 6 s for anticipation and outcome periods) and no contingency (i.e. participants perceive that their outcomes are due to chance). Increased cognitive processing may be more likely to recruit cortical as well as subcortical components of reward circuitry. Interestingly, both studies produced evidence for deactivation in reward circuitry when expected rewards were not obtained. While these deactivations may seem unexpected from the perspective of the brain imaging literature, they can be predicted on the basis of primate electrophysiology research [11]. CONCLUSION These ndings suggest that ventral striatal and forebrain sites may be differentially recruited by reward anticipation vs outcome in humans [11], and that fmri may provide investigators with a tool for neuroanatomically dissecting different stages of reward processing. REFERENCES 1. Sherrington CS. The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. New York: Charles Scribner; 196, p. 433. 2. Craig W. Biol Bull 34, 91±17 (1918). 3. Knutson B, Adams CM, Fong GW et al. J Neurosci 21, 1±5 (21). 4. Berridge KC and Robinson TE. Brain Res Rev 28, 39±369 (1998). 5. Thut G, Schultz W, Roelcke U et al. Neuroreport 8, 1225±1228 (1997). 6. Koepp MJ, Gunn RN, Lawrence AD et al. Nature 393, 266±268 (1998). 7. Knutson B, Westdorp A, Kaiser E et al. Neurolmage 12, 2±27 (2). 8. Delgado MR, Nystrom LE, Fissell C et al. J Neurophysiol 84, 372±377 (2). 9. Elliott R, Friston KJ and Dolan RJ. J Neurosci 2, 6159±6165 (2). 1. O'Doherty J, Kringelbach ML, Rolls et al. Nature Neurosci 4, 95±12 (21). 11. Schultz W, Tremblay L and Hollerman JR. Cerebr Cortex 1, 272±283 (2). 12. Breiter HC, Aharon I, Kahneman D et al. Neuron 3, 619±639 (21). 13. Cox RW. Comp Biomed Res 29, 162±173 (1996). 14. Neter J, Kutner MH, Nachtsheim CJ et al. Applied Linear Statistical Models. Chicago: Irwin; 1996, p. 148. 15. Cohen MS. NeuroImage 6, 93±13 (1997). 16. Breiter HC, Gollub RM, Weisskoff RM et al. Neuron 19, 591±611 (1997). 17. Mirenowicz J and Schultz W. Nature 379, 449±451 (1996). Acknowledgements: This work was supported by the NIAAA Intramural Research Program. We thank Erica Kaiser, Reza Momenan, Michael Kerich, Thomas Lionetti, and Jonathan Walker for support; as well as Steven Grant and an anonymous reviewer for comments on earlier drafts of the manuscript. Vol 12 No 17 4 December 21 3687