The DSM Classification of Personality Disorder: Clinical Wisdom or Empirical Truth? A Response to Alvin R. Mahrer s Problem 11

Similar documents
Personality Disorders. Mark Kimsey, M.D. March 8, 2014

Slide 1. Slide 2. Slide 3 Similar observations in all subsets of the disorder. Personality Disorders. General Symptoms. Chapter 9

Introduction to personality. disorders. University of Liverpool. James McGuire PRISON MENTAL HEALTH TRAINING WORKSHOP JUNE 2007

PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT INVENTORY

Redefining personality disorders: Proposed revisions for DSM-5

Neurotic Styles and the Five Factor Model of Personality

Twelve month test retest reliability of a Japanese version of the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Personality Disorders

Explainer: what are personality disorders and how are they treated?

A Structured Interview for the Assessment of the Five-Factor Model of Personality: Facet-Level Relations to the Axis II Personality Disorders

Personality Disorder in Primary Care. Dr Graham Ingram Consultant Psychiatrist

Personality disorders. Personality disorder defined: Characteristic areas of impairment: The contributions of Theodore Millon Ph.D.

Suggestions for a Framework for an Empirically Based Classification of Personality Disorder

HIERARCHICAL STRUCTURE OF MALADAPTIVE PERSONALITY TRAITS IN OLDER ADULTS: JOINT FACTOR ANALYSIS OF THE PID-5 AND THE DAPP-BQ

Can my personality be a disorder?!

The Big Seven Model of Personality and Its Relevance to Personality Pathology

Psychiatric Diagnoses In Developmentally Disabled Persons

Can my personality be a disorder?!

Journal of Research in Personality

Copyright American Psychological Association. Introduction

Coolidge Assessment Battery (CAB) Summary - Narrative Report

DIAGNOSIS OF PERSONALITY DISORDERS: SELECTED METHODS AND MODELS OF ASSESSMENT 1

Can my personality be a disorder?!

Can my personality be a disorder?!

A Clinical Translation of the Research Article Titled Antisocial Behavioral Syndromes and. Additional Psychiatric Comorbidity in Posttraumatic Stress

Personality disorders. Eccentric (Cluster A) Dramatic (Cluster B) Anxious(Cluster C)

What is schizoid personality disorder? Why is the salience or ability to focus and connect potential punishments important in training sociopathics?

Five Factor Model Prototype Matching Scores: Convergence Within Alternative Methods

5/6/2008. Psy 427 Cal State Northridge Andrew Ainsworth PhD

An Item Response Theory Integration of Normal and Abnormal Personality Scales

Measurement of pathological personality traits according to the DSM-5: A Polish adaptation of the PID-5 Part II empirical results

How to Win Friends and Influence People Lesson 6 Psychological Patterns and Disorders

CHAPTER 2. RESEARCH METHODS AND PERSONALITY ASSESSMENT (64 items)

Multidimensional Perfectionism Scale. Interpretive Report. Paul L. Hewitt, Ph.D. & Gordon L. Flett, Ph.D.

Cluster A personality disorders- are characterized by odd, eccentric thinking or behavior.

Traits: Prominent enduring aspects and qualities of a person.

Long term predictive validity of diagnostic models for personality disorder: Integrating trait and disorder concepts

9 - SCREENING MEASURES FOR PERSONALITY DISORDERS

Managing Personality Disorders in Primary Care

Cognitive-Behavioral Assessment of Depression: Clinical Validation of the Automatic Thoughts Questionnaire

The General Assessment of Personality Disorder (GAPD) as an Instrument for Assessing the Core Features of Personality Disorders

Comparing Methods for Scoring Personality Disorder Types Using Maladaptive Traits in DSM-5

Psychometric Qualities of the Dimensional Assessment of Personality Pathology Short Form for Adolescents

Contemporary Psychiatric-Mental Health Nursing Third Edition. Personality. Personality Traits 8/22/2016

Personality Disorders

Early Maladaptive Schemas And Personality. Disorder Symptoms An Examination In A Nonclinical

Personality traits predict current and future functioning comparably for individuals with major depressive and personality disorders

DEPRESSIVE PERSONALITY DISORDER: RATES OF COMORBIDITY WITH PERSONALITY DISORDERS AND RELATIONS TO THE FIVE FACTOR MODEL OF PERSONALITY

Personality disorders, from classification to pharmacotherapy. Peter Tyrer Imperial College

BroadcastMed Bipolar, Borderline, Both? Diagnostic/Formulation Issues in Mood and Personality Disorders

Personality and its disorders

Personality Disorders

A Paradigm Shift: From a Categorical to Dimensional Diagnostic Model of Personality Disorder

Practical Tips for Dealing with Difficult People (or What Do I Do In The Real World?)

Correspondence Among Observer Ratings of Rorschach, Big Five Model, and DSM IV Personality Disorder Constructs

Personality Disorders

SUMMARY AND DISCUSSION

Diagnosis of Mental Disorders. Historical Background. Rise of the Nomenclatures. History and Clinical Assessment

Personality Disorder

Other Disorders Myers for AP Module 69

A new five factor model of psychopathology: Preliminary psychometric characteristics of the five-dimensional personality test (5DPT) q

P sychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES) are episodes of

Personality Disorders Explained

Personality Disorders

A Simplified Technique for Scoring DSM-IV Personality Disorders With the Five-Factor Model

by Odd or Eccentric Behavior Paranoid Personality Disorder Pervasive suspiciousness Excessive mistrust of others No delusional thinking Overly sensiti

Examining Criterion A: DSM-5 Level of Personality Functioning as Assessed through Life Story Interviews

Self-Oriented and Socially Prescribed Perfectionism in the Eating Disorder Inventory Perfectionism Subscale

The DSM-5 Draft: Critique and Recommendations

CHAPTER 3 METHOD AND PROCEDURE

Traits & Trait Taxonomies

NATIONAL INSTITUTE FOR HEALTH AND CLINICAL EXCELLENCE SCOPE. Personality Disorder: the clinical management of borderline personality disorder

Grandiose and Vulnerable Narcissism and the DSM 5 Pathological Personality Trait Model

Ten-Year Rank-Order Stability of Personality Traits and Disorders in a Clinical Sample

Relationship Between Clinician Assessment and Self-Assessment of Personality Disorders Using the SWAP-200 and PAI

Taxometrics of Personality Disorders 1

S P O U S A L R ES E M B L A N C E I N PSYCHOPATHOLOGY: A C O M PA R I SO N O F PA R E N T S O F C H I LD R E N W I T H A N D WITHOUT PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

Responses to DSM-5. DSM-5 and Malingering. DSM-5: Development and Implementation. Oxford Medicine Online

Personality Disorders. Personality Disorders. Definition of personality. Trait theory of Personality. Trait theory of Personality

Submission to. MBS Review Taskforce Eating Disorders Working Group

SEMINAR ON SERVICE MARKETING

Dr Angela Busuttil Head of Psychology in Physical and Occupational Health Sussex Partnership NHS UK

Differential impairment as an indicator of sex bias in DSM-IV criteria for four personality disorders.

DEPARTMENT OF THE NAVY BOARD FOR CORRECTION OF NAVAL RECORDS 2 WASHINGTON DC

PSYCHOLOGY. Chapter 15 PSYCHOLOGICAL DISORDERS. Chaffey College Summer 2018 Professor Trujillo

Wesleyan University. From the SelectedWorks of Charles A. Sanislow, Ph.D.

May and Klonsky s (2016) meta-analysis of factors

Identifying Information

Pathways to Inflated Responsibility Beliefs in Adolescent Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder: A Preliminary Investigation

Chapter 14. Psychological Disorders 8 th Edition

VITA Shirley L. Campbell was born in Danville, Illinois on. She was reared in covington, Indiana and

PSYCHOMETRIC PROPERTIES OF CLINICAL PERFORMANCE RATINGS

Depression Remission at Six Months Specifications 2013 (02/01/2012 to 01/31/2013 Dates of Service) Revised 08/10/2012

Available online at

Diffusing the Stress in Financial Distress: The Intersection of Bankruptcy and Mental Health

Psychometric Properties and Concurrent Validity of the Schizotypal Ambivalence Scale

Why we need a new classification of personality disorders

Relationship Between Attachment Patterns and Personality Pathology in Adolescents

The University of Calgary Department of Psychology. Adult Psychopathology. Psychology 651 (L01) Fall 2005

Self- and Peer Perspectives on Pathological Personality Traits and Interpersonal Problems

Transcription:

The DSM Classification of Personality Disorder: Clinical Wisdom or Empirical Truth? A Response to Alvin R. Mahrer s Problem 11 E. David Klonsky University of Virginia In a recent issue of the Journal of Clinical Psychology, Alvin R. Mahrer (1999, pp. 1147 1156) outlined 11 problems facing the field of psychotherapy. Problem 11 states that psychotherapy rests on a foundation of truths that have not been tested in ways that could find them to be false. This point is especially pertinent to the DSM classification of personality disorders. None of the DSM-IV categories of personality disorder were discovered through empirical research. Rather, they were hypothesized by psychiatrists and psychologists, and put into writing. A review of the relevant literature reveals that empirical research findings rarely agree with the DSM conceptions of personality disorder classification. It is concluded that only through continuing empirical research efforts can we discover how nature intended personality pathology to be classified. 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Inc. J Clin Psychol 56: 1615 1621, 2000. Keywords: personality disorder; classification; DSM; Axis II; factor structure In his article entitled Embarrassing Problems in the Field of Psychotherapy, Alvin R. Mahrer (1999) argues that the field of psychotherapy rests on a foundation of assumed truths that have not been subjected to adequate scientific inquiry. I propose that the DSM classification of personality disorder is one of these assumed truths. At present, the DSM is based primarily on clinical wisdom, despite 20 years of empirically based revisions. The first major effort to provide an empirical foundation for the DSM began around 1980. The creation of Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third I thank Irving I. Gottesman, Roy Cannon Thomas, and Heather L. Tencer for their comments on earlier versions of this article. Corresponding concerning this article should be addressed to: E. David Klonsky, Department of Psychology, University of Virginia, 102 Gilmer Hall, P.O. Box 400400, Charlottesville, VA 22904 4400, e-mail: klonsky@virginia.edu. JOURNAL OF CLINICAL PSYCHOLOGY, Vol. 56(12), 1615 1621 (2000) 2000 John Wiley & Sons, Inc.

1616 Journal of Clinical Psychology, December 2000 Edition (DSM-III; American Psychiatric Association, 1980) was guided by an increased emphasis on objective empirical investigation (Widiger, Frances, Pincus, Davis, & First, 1991). Specifically, diagnostic categories were defined by explicit criteria sets instead of the comparatively vague prose descriptions found in previous versions of the DSM. The explicit nature of the DSM-III diagnostic category definitions made researching these diagnoses more feasible than ever. Empirical investigations of reliability (internal consistency, interrater, and test-retest) and validity (face, content, convergent and discriminant, predictive, and construct) became frequent (Widiger et al., 1991). Consequently, revisions implemented in later DSM versions were based on findings from the evergrowing empirical research base. The problem is that, as a rule, the DSM diagnostic categories were not discovered through empirical research. Rather, they were hypothesized by psychiatrists and psychologists, put into writing, and then subjected to limited revision when psychometric investigations revealed extensive problems in reliability and validity. In other words, no data set reveals the natural existence of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV; American Psychiatric Association, 1994) disorders and corresponding criteria sets. Indeed, the very concept of a mental or personality disorder as an entity that can be defined according to the presence of a minimum number of specific criteria lacks an empirical foundation. Some may argue that the use of criteria sets allows for more precise operational definitions and measurements of reliability, and thereby facilitates empirical research. Nevertheless, the extent to which explicit DSM definitions of disorders improve interrater reliability and facilitate research has no bearing on the extent to which the definitions map onto valid, existing constructs. Though the Axis I disorders certainly are not immune to criticism, the Axis II disorders are particularly precarious. The DSM-IV defines a personality disorder as an enduring pattern of inner experience and behavior that deviates markedly from the expectations of the individual s culture, is pervasive and inflexible, has an onset in adolescence or early adulthood, is stable over time, and leads to distress or impairment (American Psychiatric Association, 1994, p. 629). There are ten personality disorders included in the DSM-IV, which are grouped into three clusters. Cluster A (schizoid, schizotypal, and paranoid) includes individuals who appear odd or eccentric; Cluster B (histrionic, borderline, narcissistic, and antisocial) individuals who appear dramatic, emotional, or erratic; and Cluster C (avoidant, dependent, and obsessive compulsive) individuals who appear anxious or fearful. Given the discrete DSM personality disorder categories, one might assume that they are as understood and easily classified as medical diseases. This is not the case, however. The DSM-IV classification of personality disorders is plagued by extensive diagnostic overlap, limited evidence of validity, and generally poor empirical support (Livesley, 1998). As a result, to accept the DSM personality disorder classification system at face value is to embrace several untested assumptions. Three assumptions implicitly endorsed by the DSM-IV are the subject of this discussion: (a) personality pathology is suited to be classified into discrete types, or disorders; (b) these disorders group themselves into three clusters; and (c) the diagnostic criteria naturally fall into the particular personality disorders to which they have been assigned (The DSM-IV defines each personality disorder by the presence of a minimum number of symptoms [i.e., criteria] ostensibly related to that disorder.) Covariation among the DSM Personality Disorders The first two assumptions that personality disorder can be divided into discrete types and that these types group themselves into three clusters can be tested by examining the

DSM Personality Disorder 1617 covariation among DSM personality disorder diagnoses. 1 Typically, such studies factor analyze dimensional scores of the DSM personality disorders to examine whether they overlap and whether they group themselves into clusters. The DSM assumptions suggest that (a) we would find little covariation among the different personality disorders, and that (b) to the extent the personality disorders do covary, they would in a manner reflecting the three clusters. Hyler and Lyons (1988) factor analyzed scaled ratings of the DSM-III personality disorders. The emerging factors were similar to those specified by the DSM-III. The study used a sample of 358 patients being seen by 287 different psychiatrists from clinics and private practices nationwide to avoid the potential confound of idiosyncratic diagnostician bias. Four factors were found compared to the three in the DSM-III; however, the fourth factor contained only compulsive personality disorder whereas the first three factors were consistent with the three DSM-III clusters. Factor 1 comprised schizotypal, schizoid, and paranoid; Factor 2 comprised narcissistic, histrionic, borderline, and antisocial; and Factor 3 comprised dependent, passive aggressive, and avoidant. The four factor solution accounted for 62% of the variance, and the internal consistencies (Cronbach s alpha) for the three DSM-like clusters were.54,.70, and.43, respectively. Hyler and Lyons (1988) conclude that their results lend support for the three DSM-III clusters. More recent empirical studies, however, yielded divergent results. Moldin, Rice, Erlenmeyer-Kimling, and Squires-Wheeler (1994) examined the covariation of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Third Edition, Revised (DSM-III-R; American Psychiatric Association, 1987) personality disorders (diagnosed through Personality Disorder Examination dimensional ratings) in a nonclinical sample of 302 participants. Their analyses yielded a three-factor structure different from that assumed in the DSM-III-R, and different in males than in females. The following factors emerged: for males, social inhibition/restricted affectivity, irritable aloofness/hypersensitivity, emotional instability/dependence avoidance; and for females, social inhibition/aloof hypersensitivity, inflexible hypersensitivity/emotional hyperreactivity, and emotional instability/ dependence avoidance. Also of note, Moldin et al. (1994) found extensive intercorrelations among the DSM-III-R personality disorders irrespective of cluster membership. There was little tendency for personality disorders placed in a particular cluster by the DSM to correlate more with each other than with personality disorders from other clusters. For example, although schizotypal and paranoid are placed in Cluster A by the DSM, Moldin et al. (1994) found correlations in the.5 to.6 range between these two personality disorders and non-cluster A personality disorders such as borderline, narcissistic, avoidant, and obsessive compulsive. Blais, McCann, Benedict, and Norman (1997) examined the covariation of the DSM- III-R personality disorders in a sample of 320 personality disordered patients. They found a three-factor solution that did not correspond to either the Moldin et al. (1994) or to the DSM-III-R clusters. The three emerging factors were termed by Blais et al. (1997): detached/socially withdrawn, independent/self-oriented, and dependent personality types, respectively. Factor 1 included primary loadings for the avoidant, schizoid, and schizotypal personality disorders and secondary loadings for the compulsive and passive aggressive personality disorders. Factor 2 included primary loadings for the narcissistic and antisocial personality disorders and a moderate loading for paranoid. Factor 3 included 1 Although some personality disorder diagnoses were added, dropped, or altered from one revision to another, the clusters and their member personality disorders have remained generally the same across revisions since the DSM-III. Consequently, studies focusing on the DSM-III, the DSM-III-R, and the DSM-IV personality disorders will be included in this discussion.

1618 Journal of Clinical Psychology, December 2000 primary loadings on the dependent and borderline personality disorders, and a secondary loading for histrionic. Studies examining the covariation of the DSM personality disorders yielded mixed results. An important finding is the absence of a three-factor solution precisely mapping onto the three DSM clusters. Perhaps the most consistent finding is the extensive intercorrelation among the personality disorders in all of the studies, conflicting with the idea that personality disorder is composed of discrete categories or types. In each of the aforementioned studies, every personality disorder had multiple significant correlations with other personality disorders. The intercorrelations generally centered around the.3 to.4 range, although several were higher than.5. Some personality disorders were inclined to correlate more with each other than with others. For example, schizoid and schizotypal, paranoid and schizotypal, narcissistic and histrionic, and dependent and avoidant are four pairs of personality disorders that tended to intercorrelate across studies a pattern consistent with, although not equivalent to, the DSM clusters. The overall pattern of findings provides moderate support for the theoretical notion of clusters and limited support for the clusters specified by the DSM. The findings do not provide evidence that personality disorders should be classified into discrete types, as they are in the DSM-IV. On the contrary, there appears to be substantial overlap among all the DSM personality disorders. Covariation among the DSM Personality Disorder Criteria The third assumption that the DSM personality disorder criteria naturally cluster into the particular categories to which they have been assigned can be tested by factor analyzing the entire set of criteria. Using a national sample of 291 patients diagnosed as having personality disorders by their treating clinician, Morey (1988) applied a clusteranalytic technique to the DSM-III-R personality disorder criteria that he considered most likely to recover the DSM-III-R categorical structure. Generally, the cluster analysis yielded a classification fairly similar to the DSM-III-R structure. Criteria from particular personality disorders tended to group together. There were also several areas in which the clusteranalytic model diverged from the DSM-III-R model. For example, although criteria for paranoid personality disorder tended to group together, mixed into this grouping were at least one criterion from each of the schizotypal, narcissistic, borderline, avoidant, and dependent personality disorders. Additionally, criteria for the narcissistic and antisocial personality disorders grouped together, as did criteria for the schizoid and schizotypal, and the borderline and dependent personality disorders, suggesting that these personality disorders may not represent unitary entities as they were intended to. Torgensen, Skre, Onstad, Edvardsen, and Kringlen (1993) examined the covariation of the DSM-III-R criteria in a sample of probands and relatives from an ongoing twinfamily study of psychiatric patients. The sample consisted of 72 index twins, 106 co-twins, 190 siblings, and 77 parents. A factor analysis with oblique rotation yielded 12 factors that resembled the DSM-III-R personality disorders. However, ten of the emergent factors contained criteria from more than one personality disorder. Only three of the 11 DSM-III-R personality disorders had their constituent criteria fall within a single factor (paranoid, antisocial, and passive aggressive). Criteria belonging to the schizotypal, dependent, obsessive compulsive, and narcissistic personality disorders scattered into five, four, three, and three factors, respectively. A third study demonstrated the largest discrepancy between empirically derived and DSM hypothesized groupings of personality disorder criteria. Ekselius, Lindstrom, Von Knorring, Bodlund, and Kullgren (1994) examined the covariation of diagnostic criteria

DSM Personality Disorder 1619 in a sample including 176 healthy controls and 388 psychiatric patients. They conducted a principle component analysis of the 76 DSM-III-R criteria, which yielded 23 factors with Eigen values greater than 1. The resulting factors did not resemble the DSM-III-R classification structure, although there was some tendency for criteria belonging to the same personality disorder to cluster together. In sum, analyzing the covariation of the DSM personality disorder criteria yields empirically derived factors that differ in number and nature from the DSM personality disorder categories. Different studies reported different covariation patterns even though they examined equivalent criteria sets. As a consequence, no consistent empirical grouping emerged, and only minimal support could be garnered for the DSM classification of personality disorder. Empirical Alternatives for Classifying Personality Pathology If the DSM classification system for personality pathology lacks an empirical basis, what are the alternatives? I would argue that empirical research leading to alternative classification systems must begin somewhere other than with the DSM criteria, clusters, or categories. Although it is highly convenient to focus on the DSM criteria because they are part of a standardized nomenclature, the choice to use only the DSM criteria imposes an artificial limitation on the number and nature of the factors that can be discovered. In general, personality disorder features, traits, and criteria are phenotypic languageterms imperfectly and ambiguously mapped onto underlying constructs, which are themselves imperfect and ambiguous. Therefore, the features included in a study on the factor structure of personality pathology should be numerous and diverse enough to map onto the entire range of personality pathology. Only in this manner will all of the factors underlying personality disorder be reflected in the emergent clusters of covarying phenotypic language-terms. Granted, this requires compiling features from numerous and diverse sources a laborious task. A few researchers, however, did just that, and produced some of the most empirically sound findings regarding the classification of personality pathology. The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI Hathaway & McKinley, 1940), developed several decades ago, represents the earliest of such undertakings. The MMPI consists of several hundred items shown empirically to differentiate between various forms of psychopathology. An MMPI profile includes several kinds of information (e.g., scale elevations, code-types) useful for classification purposes. Today, MMPI profiles remain useful clinically for capturing the personality and psychopathology of a particular patient, as well as in research for capturing the personality and psychopathology of a particular patient population. One could argue that the MMPI represents a classification of personality pathology far more practical and empirically valid than that proposed by the DSM-IV. More recently, W. John Livesley sought to compile an item pool sufficient in breadth and diversity to cover the entire range of personality pathology. He solicited items from psychiatrists, mental health workers, and research associates, identifying suitable items from content analysis of interviews and from behaviors rated as highly typical of various dimensions of personality pathology (Livesley, Jackson, & Schroeder, 1991). The resulting questionnaire comprised 1,943 items measuring 100 traits as well as social desirability (Livesley et al., 1991). Various portions of the questionnaire were administered to a total of 3,256 subjects from the general population drawn from a street directory. After extensive further revisions, the result was a 290-item questionnaire [Dimensional Assess-

1620 Journal of Clinical Psychology, December 2000 ment of Personality Pathology Basic Questionnaire (DAPP-BQ)] assessing 18 dimensions of personality pathology. The 18 dimensions (also referred to as trait scales) are anxiousness, suspiciousness, affective lability, self-harming behaviors, identity problems, cognitive distortions, callousness, rejection, oppositionality, narcissism, stimulus seeking, restricted expression, social avoidance, intimacy problems, submissiveness, insecure attachment, conduct problems, and compulsivity. Lee Anna Clark embarked on a similar venture. She compiled a broad set of personality disorder features from the DSM, various non-dsm conceptualizations of personality disorder, and selected Axis-I disorders with chronic features (Clark, Livesley, Schroeder, & Irish, 1996). The symptoms were then grouped into 22 clusters based on their conceptualized affiliations. After an iterative series of data collection and psychometric analyses, the result was a 375-item questionnaire (Schedule for Nonadaptive and Adaptive Personality [SNAP]) assessing 15 dimensions of personality disorder (Clark et al., 1996). The 15 dimensions (also referred to as trait scales) are negative temperament, mistrust, self-harm, eccentric perceptions, aggression, manipulativeness, entitlement, exhibitionism, positive temperament, detachment, dependency, disinhibition, impulsivity, workaholism, and propriety. What is promising is that the systems independently constructed by Livesley and Clark appear to show striking similarity. To examine the convergence of the two systems, Clark et al. (1996) administered both instruments to two nonclinical samples (163 college students and 74 adults). The results were encouraging. First, the two systems were demonstrated to share equivalent higher order structures; that is, for each inventory, the various dimensions combined to form four robust factors: neuroticism, introversion, (dis)agreeableness, and (low) conscientiousness. A fifth, less robust higher order factor representing dependency and related interpersonal difficulties also emerged on both inventories. Perhaps more remarkably, the SNAP and the DAPP-BQ displayed convergence between their two sets of trait scales (Clark et al., 1996). For example, the SNAP negative temperament scale correlated.78 with the DAPP-BQ anxiousness scale, SNAP entitlement correlated.41 with the DAPP-BQ narcissism, and SNAP dependency correlated.58 with DAPP-BQ submissiveness and.33 with DAPP-BQ insecure attachment. Correlations between the two schemes lower order scales that were predicted to match centered around.53. To the knowledge of the authors, this degree of convergence at the level of specific trait scales has not previously been demonstrated empirically (Clark et al., 1996, p. 301). It also is noteworthy that the average correlation between scales not predicted to match was only.22, indicating a strong discriminant pattern. Although the convergence between the SNAP and the DAPP-BQ is far from perfect and the problem of personality disorder classification far from resolved, both systems could claim more empirical validity than can the DSM-IV. Clearly, the answers provided by empirical research are not yet conclusive. What can be determined conclusively is that there exists a disparity between the dimensions of personality disorder proposed by the DSM and those derived by empirical techniques. Such discrepancies are obviously troubling given the widespread use of the DSM-IV in psychological and psychiatric communities. The DSM practice of putting expert opinions into writing and only then conducting tests of reliability and validity cannot lead to an acceptable classification system for personality disorders. Rather, it directs scientists to conduct research on, and practitioners to put their trust in, diagnostic labels that may or may not map onto valid constructs that exist in nature. Instead, researchers must turn to objective, empirical methodologies to discover the dimensions of personality pathology, letting the data fall where they may, and letting the data determine how personality disorder is best classified.

DSM Personality Disorder 1621 References American Psychiatric Association. (1980). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3rd ed., revised). Washington, DC: Author. American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author. Blais, M.A., McCann, J.T., Benedict, K.B., & Norman, D.K. (1997). Toward an empirical/ theoretical grouping of the DSM-III-R personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 11, 191 198. Clark, L.A., Livesley, W.J., Schroeder, M.L., & Irish, S.L. (1996). Convergence of two systems for assessing specific traits of personality disorder. Psychological Assessment, 8, 294 303. Ekselius, L., Lindstrom, E., Von Knorring, L., Bodlund, O., & Kullgren, G. (1994). A principal component analysis of the DSM-III-R Axis-II personality disorders. Journal of Personality Disorders, 8, 140 148. Hathaway, S.R., & McKinley, J.C. (1940). A multiphasic personality schedule (Minnesota): I. Construction of the schedule. Journal of Psychology, 10, 249 254. Hyler, S.E., & Lyons, M. (1988). Factor analysis of the DSM-III personality disorder clusters: A replication. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 29, 304 308. Livesley, W.J. (1998). Suggestions for a framework for an empirically based classification of personality disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 137 147. Livesley, W.J., Jackson, D.N., & Schroeder, M.L. (1991). Dimensions of personality pathology. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 36, 557 562. Mahrer, A.R. (1999). Embarrassing problems for the field of psychotherapy. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55, 1147 1156. Moldin, S.O., Rice, J.P., Erlenmeyer-Kimling, L., & Squires-Wheeler, E. (1994). Latent structure of DSM-III-R Axis-II psychopathology in a normal sample. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103, 259 266. Morey, L.C. (1988). The categorical representation of personality disorder: A cluster analysis of DSM-III-R personality features. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 97, 314 321. Torgensen, S., Skre, I., Onstad, S., Edvardsen, J., & Kringlen, E. (1993). The psychometric genetic structure of DSM-III-R personality disorder criteria. Journal of Personality Disorders, 7, 196 213. Widiger, T.A., Frances, A.J., Pincus, H.A., Davis, W.W., & First, M.B. (1991). Toward an empirical classification for the DSM-IV. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 100, 280 288.