SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION. Abstract

Similar documents
TESTING THE SCALAR PROPERTY WITH INTERVAL REPRODUCTIONS

A model of parallel time estimation

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

INFLUENCE ON TIME INTERVAL CATEGORIZATION OF DISTANCE BETWEEN MARKERS LOCATED ON A VERTICAL PLANE

Episodic temporal generalization: A developmental study

Are the Referents Remembered in Temporal Bisection?

Modality Differences in Timing: Testing the Pacemaker Speed Explanation

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

TIME-ORDER EFFECTS FOR AESTHETIC PREFERENCE

Production, reproduction, and verbal estimation of duration. John Wearden Keele University U.K.

Why ``Sound s Are Jud ged Longer T han Ligh ts : Application of a Model of th e Internal Clock in Hu m ans

Perception of the duration of auditory and visual stimuli in children and adults

Testing the representation of time in reference memory in the bisection and the generalization task: The utility of a developmental approach

Temporal Generalization in 3- to 8-Year-Old Children

Is Clock Slowing General or Specific?

When do auditory/visual differences in duration judgements occur?

ARTICLE IN PRESS. Acta Psychologica xxx (2008) xxx xxx. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Acta Psychologica

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION. Table 1 Patient characteristics Preoperative. language testing

Decision processes in models of timing

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Sensation is the conscious experience associated with an environmental stimulus. It is the acquisition of raw information by the body s sense organs

Categorical Perception

Behavioural Processes

Imperfect, Unlimited-Capacity, Parallel Search Yields Large Set-Size Effects. John Palmer and Jennifer McLean. University of Washington.

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Are Retrievals from Long-Term Memory Interruptible?

Business Statistics Probability

Conscious control of movements: increase of temporal precision in voluntarily delayed actions

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Unequal Weber fractions for the categorization of brief temporal intervals

The perception of empty and filled time intervals by pigeons

Meaning-based guidance of attention in scenes as revealed by meaning maps

Experimental and Computational Analyses of Strategy Usage in the Time-Left Task

Stochastic optimal control and the human oculomotor system

Pigeons timing of an arbitrary and a naturalistic auditory stimulus: tone versus cooing

DOES FILLED DURATION ILLUSION TAKE PLACE FOR VERY SHORT TIME INTERVALS?

Empirical Formula for Creating Error Bars for the Method of Paired Comparison

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

Still important ideas

Some methodological aspects for measuring asynchrony detection in audio-visual stimuli

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

CONTEXTUAL CHANGE EFFECTS IN RETROSPECTIVE AND PROSPECTIVE DURATION JUDGMENT PARADIGMS USING MUSICAL PIECES. Breanne Kay Smith

Supporting Information

Modeling Human Perception

..." , \ I \ ... / ~\ \ : \\ ~ ... I CD ~ \ CD> 00\ CD) CD 0. Relative frequencies of numerical responses in ratio estimation1

Lab 3: Perception of Loudness

Jeremie Jozefowiez. Timely Training School, April 6 th, 2011

Results & Statistics: Description and Correlation. I. Scales of Measurement A Review

On the empirical status of the matching law : Comment on McDowell (2013)

PERCEPTION OF UNATTENDED SPEECH. University of Sussex Falmer, Brighton, BN1 9QG, UK

Numerical bias in bounded and unbounded number line tasks

Little to be Expected from Auditory Training for Improving Visual Temporal Discrimination

Time Interval Estimation: Internal Clock or Attentional Mechanism?

Classic experiments in sensation and perception

JUDGMENTAL MODEL OF THE EBBINGHAUS ILLUSION NORMAN H. ANDERSON

Time Perception: Beyond Simple Interval Estimation

Orientation Specific Effects of Automatic Access to Categorical Information in Biological Motion Perception

Detection Theory: Sensory and Decision Processes

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

SAMENESS AND REDUNDANCY IN TARGET DETECTION AND TARGET COMPARISON. Boaz M. Ben-David and Daniel Algom Tel-Aviv University

Absolute Identification is Surprisingly Faster with More Closely Spaced Stimuli

Lab 4: Perception of Loudness

The Clock Ticking Changes Our Performance

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

Keywords: time perception; illusion; empty interval; filled intervals; cluster analysis

The effect of sample duration and cue on a double temporal discrimination q

STUDY OF THE BISECTION OPERATION

EFFECTS OF RHYTHM ON THE PERCEPTION OF URGENCY AND IRRITATION IN 1 AUDITORY SIGNALS. Utrecht University

Age and IQ Effects on Stimulus and Response Timing

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 13 (emphasis on Chapter 12) Online readings: Appendix D, E & F

Readings: Textbook readings: OpenStax - Chapters 1 11 Online readings: Appendix D, E & F Plous Chapters 10, 11, 12 and 14

Describe what is meant by a placebo Contrast the double-blind procedure with the single-blind procedure Review the structure for organizing a memo

Underestimating Correlation from Scatterplots

CONTEXTUAL EFFECTS IN INFORMATION INTEGRATION

Appendix B Statistical Methods

Hierarchical organization of temporal patterns

Jan Kaiser, Andrzej Beauvale and Jarostaw Bener. Institute of Psychology, Jagiellonian University, 13 Golcbia St., ?

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Still important ideas

A FRÖHLICH EFFECT IN MEMORY FOR AUDITORY PITCH: EFFECTS OF CUEING AND OF REPRESENTATIONAL GRAVITY. Timothy L. Hubbard 1 & Susan E.

Sensory Thresholds and Signal Detection. Lecture 13

Numerical Subtraction in the Pigeon: Evidence for a Linear Subjective Number Scale

Detection Theory: Sensitivity and Response Bias

Common Representations of Abstract Quantities Sara Cordes, Christina L. Williams, and Warren H. Meck

Time flies when we read taboo words

Toward Extending Auditory Steady-State Response (ASSR) Testing to Longer-Latency Equivalent Potentials

Psychology of Perception PSYC Spring 2017 Laboratory 2: Perception of Loudness

PSYC 441 Cognitive Psychology II

Statistics is the science of collecting, organizing, presenting, analyzing, and interpreting data to assist in making effective decisions

INTRODUCTION. Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA Electronic mail:

A linear relation between loudness and decibels

Perception Lecture 1

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings

Classical Psychophysical Methods (cont.)

Effects of Flashing Lights and Beeping Tones on Subjective Time Estimation

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013


= add definition here. Definition Slide

Transcription:

SCALAR TIMING (EXPECTANCY) THEORY: A COMPARISON BETWEEN PROSPECTIVE AND RETROSPECTIVE DURATION Hannes Eisler and Anna D. Eisler Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, Stockholm, Sweden he@psychology.su.se Abstract One hundred and twenty human subjects participated in both a prospective and a retrospective duration reproduction experiment. A scaling approach to the collected data can be found in A. D. Eisler, Eisler, and Montgomery (2004). In the present part of the comprehensive project the data were treated according to the requirements of Scalar Timing (Expectancy) Theory (SET). For both the prospective and the retrospective data sets the distribution of the ratio reproduction/standard durations proved skewed to the right, and the coefficient of variation decreased with the durations rather than being constant. Both findings are at variance with SET, which assumes the validity of Weber's law and agreement between subjective and chronometric time. However, the outcome could be predicted from the generalized Weber law (common for many continua, see H. Eisler, 1965) and a nonlinear psychophysical function for duration. The symmetric distributions obtained from animals are explained by stimulus error. From the point of view of SET, as opposed to the scaling approach, except for greater scatter, the retrospective durations did not differ from the prospective in any essential way. Key words: Chronometric time; duration; neural loops; psychophysics; scaling; subjective time; Weber law Scalar Timing (Expectancy) Theory (SET) is a quantitative model that describes the behavior of organisms in temporal tasks (e.g., Church & Gibbon, 1982; Gallistel & Gibbon, 2000; Machado & Arantes, 2006). The oldest reference to scalar timing seems to be Gibbon (1971), which deals with operant conditioning. The model was originally developed for animal subjects. More recently, its applicability has been investigated for human subjects, too. An excellent and comprehensive survey of both the theoretical development of the model and pertinent experiments is given by Allan (1998). In principle, the model consists of three components: (1) an "internal clock," that is, a timer or pacemaker-accumulator, (2) a memory, or memories, and (3) decision processes, e.g., a comparator. The output from the pacemaker is assumed to be Poisson-distributed pulses ("ticks"). They are collected in the accumulator resulting in a count proportional to chronometric time. Furthermore, SET builds on Weber's law for duration. Accordingly, central values of responses should be proportional to presented standards (target durations) in, e.g., identification tasks, as the peak procedure, and the distributions of temporal responses should be (almost) symmetrical and congruent when normalized by the different standard durations. The coefficient of variation, s/m, should be constant, that is, the same for all standard durations. Retrospective and Prospective Subjective Durations Time perception studies distinguish between two paradigms. In a prospective paradigm, the researcher informs the subject beforehand that s/he will be required to make duration judgments. In a retrospective paradigm, on the other hand, the subject is unaware of the fact that s/he will be required to make duration judgments. Only after the presentation of a 221

duration will the experimenter ask the subject to judge it. For example, the subject can be asked unexpectedly to reproduce a presented duration or to estimate it verbally. It follows that a retrospective duration judgment can be obtained only once from a given subject because s/he is then alerted to the task being actually concerned with time perception. A Time Perception Study on Retrospective Duration Reproduction A comprehensive comparative study of retrospective and prospective subjective duration, using psychophysical scaling approaches (duration reproduction and verbal estimation, among others), has been carried out. A detailed description of experimental methods, data analyses, and results can be found in A. D. Eisler, Eisler, and Montgomery (2004). In that study data were treated according to the Parallel-Clock model, developed by H. Eisler (1975). It was found that subjective duration in the second range, not only prospective, but also retrospective, can be described by the psychophysical power function where denotes subjective and physical (clock) duration. The difference between prospective and retrospective subjective duration could be attributed to different values of the subjective zero, with unchanged values of the exponent and the proportionality constant. The detailed description of theory and results can be found in A. D. Eisler et al. (2004). Our intention in the present paper is to explore retrospective duration reproductions in the vein of the Scalar Timing Model. The two models are not entirely incompatible; two of the components, namely the internal clock and a decision-making device, are more or less the same in terms of their function. However, the Parallel-Clock Model disposes of any memory for the duration reproduction task. The aim of this paper is to investigate the extent to which our duration reproductions agree with SET, and whether prospective and retrospective durations differ in that respect. Of particular importance in this study is that, to our knowledge, no studies have previously examined retrospective duration data in accordance with SET. Before proceeding to the method section, a number of differences should be noted between the present experiment and common SET experiments. First, both human and animal experiments pertinent for SET require some kind of a learning phase with repetitions, whereas both retrospective reproductions and the prospective reproduction used in this study are single responses. Second, SET is based to a large extent on the investigation of scatter. However, such investigations are most informative if the scatter is obtained from the same organism, that is, intraindividual. Interindividual scatter, which is all that is obtainable from our data, may be "diluted" in the sense that human subjects' parameter values always show a large range; even in animals there are noticeable individual differences (A. D. Eisler, 2003; A. D. Eisler & Eisler, 1994; A. D. Eisler, et al., 2004; H. Eisler, 1984a,b, 1989, 2003; H. Eisler & Eisler, 1991; Zeiler & Hoyert, 1989). Thus, our scatter necessarily includes individual differences. Method For experimental details (of the comprehensive project) see A. D. Eisler et al. (2004). For the present part of the study see the following description. Apparatus. The experiment was conducted on a Swedish microcomputer (ABC80) equipped with a loudspeaker, which presented the standard durations and registered the reproduced durations, both indicated by noise. Subjects. One hundred and twenty subjects (96 female and 24 male; mean age 28.5 years) participated individually in the experiment; most of them were students. All the subjects reported normal hearing. None of the subjects had previously participated in perception experiments. Their task was to reproduce the standard duration. Stimuli. The sound-pressure level of the noise that indicated the durations, both standards and reproductions, was 50 db. There were ten different standard durations, ranging from 1.3 to 20 s in logarithmic steps (1.3, 1.8, 2.5, 3.3, 4.5, 6.0, 8.1, 11.0, 14.8, 20.0 s). 222

Procedure. At the start of the experiment each subject was told that s/he would be participating in a sound perception experiment in which s/he would hear a sound and then would be asked to rate its degree of pleasantness/unpleasantness. Then the standard duration (one of the ten), indicated by noise, was presented. After its offset the subject was told that the same sound would resume and that s/he should terminate it by pressing a button when s/he experiences that this second noise had lasted as long as the first (retrospective paradigm). Before the start of the prospective task the subject was told that s/he is participating in a time perception experiment and would hear a sound again (it was the same standard duration as for the retrospective reproduction) and should again reproduce its duration (prospective paradigm). Thus, each of the 120 subjects reproduced one of the ten standard durations, once retrospectively and once prospectively, so that twelve prospective and twelve retrospective reproductions of each of the ten standard durations were obtained. (As mentioned earlier, in a retrospective design a subject can make only one judgment, since s/he then knows that the experiment concerns time.) It should be mentioned that the comprehensive experiment included judgments of the pleasantness of the sound and of the speed of time as well as a verbal estimation of the duration. For two-thirds of the subjects, one or two of these judgments intervened between their retrospective reproduction and the prospective reproduction task. No significant difference between these three groups was found (A. D. Eisler et al., 2004), indicating that the prospective reproductions were not affected by the experimental tasks preceding them. Furthermore, no systematic deviation could be observed between the present prospective reproductions and corresponding ones from previous experiments which had used a prospective paradigm only (e.g., A. D. Eisler, 1992; A. D. Eisler & Eisler, 1994; H. Eisler & Eisler, 1992). Results Figure 1, upper panels, shows histograms of the ratio reproduction/standard duration for the prospective and retrospective data. Both distributions are clearly skewed to the right, in accordance with Wearden's (1999) statement regarding human subjects. Table 1 shows the skewness values (also with the two outliers in the prospective data omitted); their significance is beyond all our tables of the normal distribution. The mode does not agree with the standard in either plot; it is at a lower value than that of the standard for both the prospective and the retrospective data. This is opposed to SET, though to be expected, because reproductions typically differ systematically from the standards (e.g., H. Eisler, 1975). As an alternative denominator of the ratio Wearden (1999) suggested the contents of long-term memory. Since duration reproduction, according to the Parallel-Clock Model (A. D. Eisler & Eisler, 1994, 2001; A. D. Eisler et al., 2004; H. Eisler, 1975, 2003), disposes of any memory, we used instead the mean of each of the ten sets of the 12 reproductions as the denominator; the corresponding histograms are shown in the bottom panels. The skewness is somewhat less, but remains. Table 1 Skewness of Normalized Reproductions Repr. /standard duration Reproduction/mean Prospective 4.01 2.60 Prospective without outliers 1.83 1.18 Retrospective 1.16 0.91 223

Figure 1. Histograms for prospective and retrospective duration reproductions. Top panel: Frequency against the ratio of reproduction over standard duration; Bottom panel: Frequency against the ratio of reproduction over the reproduction means. Table 2 Sum of Squared Residuals for Fitted Coefficient of Variation (N=240) Fitted Function y = a+b/x y =c Standard durations.50.68 Independent Variable Mean of reproductions.54.67 Figure 2 shows the coefficients of variation (based on the interindividual standard deviations) plotted against the standard durations (upper panel) and against the mean reproductions (lower panel). SET requires constancy, which may be found for the longer standards but not for the whole range. To illustrate a well-known trend (see below), we fitted a hyperbola of the form y = a + b/x for both prospective and retrospective data together. Such a hyperbola is dealt with theoretically by Killeen and Weiss (1987), who also found empirical support from a number of time perception studies. However, the hyperbolical trend of coefficients of 224

Coefficient of variation 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0.0 0 5 10 15 20 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 Prospective Retrospective Standard duration in seconds Prospective Retrospective 0.0 0 5 10 15 20 25 Mean of reproductions in seconds Figure 2. Coefficients of variation for prospective and retrospective reproductions plotted against the standard duration (top panel) and against the mean reproductions (bottom panel). The curved broken lines are hyperbolas fitted simultaneously to both sets of data points, the horizontal broken lines are at the mean of all points. variation in data collected by psychophysical methods is by no means limited to time perception. Coefficients of variation from 16 experiments (H. Eisler, 1965) for different continua (none of them duration) show exactly the same trend. These experiments also illustrate the differences between intra- and interindividual and "mixed" SDs, and thus between coefficients of variation. We recognize there the "time perception" hyperbola, as well as the point of maximum sensitivity discussed by Grondin (2001). This finding is a reminder that time perception shares quite a few psychophysical features with other perceptual continua. In order to compare the constant coefficient of variation required by SET with the hyperbola we computed the sum of the squared residuals after fitting the two models, see Table 2.The fit is clearly better for the hyperbola, which, on the other hand, requires two parameter values, versus only one for a constant coefficient of variation. In Figure 2 both models are indicated by broken lines. The great scatter makes any clear decision difficult. Discussion The hyperbolic function for the coefficient of variation plots is easily described by the well- k by adding a constant : k, that is, k (cf. Killeen and Weiss, 1987). Another, similar, point is SET's postulation of proportionality or even veridicality of subjective and chronometric time. The neurophysiological model presented below will explain the psychophysical function for time, and also question the common claim that pulses emitted by a pacemaker are Poisson distributed. Consider pacemakers consisting of neural loops (Thatcher & John, 1977). The pacemaker emits one tick or pulse after each passage through the loop, and each subsequent passage of the loop requires more (clock) time than the previous one, perhaps because of a lengthened refractory period. From the retarding rate of "ticks" an exponent for subjective duration < 1 can be derived (A. D. Eisler & Eisler, 1994; H. Eisler, 1996; H. Eisler & Eisler, 1991). Empirically, the exponent is about.9 for adults, much smaller for children, and about.5 for rats (H. Eisler, 1976, 1984a, b). A consequence from the 225

neural-loops model is that, because of the increasing refractory periods, the pulses are not independent, and thus incompatible with a Poisson distribution. We may conclude that the SET model's requirement of a constant average rate for the pacemaker's ticks has to be abandoned. The psychophysical function for duration is not linear. Therefore, the distributions are skewed and the means deviate from the standards (Figure 1). Wearden (1999) attributes the difference in skewness in the animal and human distributions to different decision rules. We are rather inclined to interpret the skewness difference to a criterion change. Due to the verbal instructions and no feedback, the human data express sensed duration as is, and thus they are based on the nonlinear psychophysical function for duration. In contrast, animals are taught to commit the stimulus error (cp. H. Eisler, 1996, p. 69), that is, react to veridical (chronometric) duration values, by being reinforced at standard durations. The feedback teaches animals "to correct" their immediate memory (or corresponding mechanisms) to achieve veridicality change of decision criterion. Thus the procedure of feedback necessarily results in a linear duration function, entailing symmetric and congruent normalized distributions. One could say that feedback generates a bias towards veridicality. In sum, not all that surprisingly, the retrospective data show more scatter than the prospective, though the latter do contain two clear outliers. Both data sets are skewed to the right, at variance with SET. A second observation is that the coefficients of variation seem to display a hyperbolic trend like other continua, rather than the constancy required by SET. The generalized form of Weber's law and a non-proportional subjective duration function can explain these findings. Furthermore, in regard to SET, as opposed to the scaling approach, and on the basis of interindividual scatter, there is no fundamental difference between prospective and retrospective duration perception. References Allan, L. G. (1998). The influence of the scalar timing model on human timing research. Behavioural Processes, 44, 101-117. Church, R. M., & Gibbon, J. (1982). Temporal generalization. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 8, 165-186. Eisler, A. D. (1992). Time perception: Reproduction of duration by two cultural groups. In S. Iwawaki, Y. Kashima & K. Leung (Eds.), Innovations in cross-cultural psychology (pp. 304-310). Amsterdam: Swets & Zeitlinger. Eisler, A. D. (2003). The human sense of time: Biological, cognitive and cultural considerations. In R. Buccheri, M. Saniga, & W. M. Stuckey (Eds.), The nature of time: Geometry, physics and perception (pp. 5-18). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. Eisler, A. D., & Eisler, H. (1994). Subjective time scaling: Influence of age, gender, and type A and type B behavior. Chronobiologia, 21, 185-200. Eisler, A. D., & Eisler, H. (2001). Subjective time in a patient with neurological impairment. Psychologica, 28, 193-206. Eisler, A. D., Eisler, H., & Montgomery, H. (2004). A quantitative model for retrospective subjective duration. NeuroQuantology, 2, 263-291. Eisler, H. (1965). The connection between magnitude and discrimination scales and direct and indirect scaling methods. Psychometrika, 30, 271-289. Eisler, H. (1975). Subjective duration and psychophysics. Psychological Review, 82, 429-450. Eisler, H. (1976). Experiments on subjective duration 1868-1975: A collection of power function exponents. Psychological Bulletin, 83, 1154-1171. Eisler, H. (1984a). Knowing before doing: Discrimination by rats of a brief interruption of a tone. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 329-340. Eisler, H. (1984b). Subjective duration in rats: The psychophysical function. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 423, 43-51. Eisler, H. (1989). Serendipity in animal experimentation: Examples from duration scaling in rats. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 3, 137-149. Eisler, H. (1996). Time perception from a psychophysicist's perspective. In H. Helfrich (Ed.), Time and mind (pp. 65-86). Kirkland, WA: Hogrefe & Huber. Eisler, H. (2003). The Parallel-Clock model: A tool for quantification of experienced duration. In R. Buccheri, M. Saniga, & W. M. Stuckey (Eds.), The nature of time: Geometry, physics and perception (pp. 19-26). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer. Eisler, H., & Eisler, A. D. (1991). A mathematical model for time perception with experimentally obtained subjective time scales for humans and rats. Chronobiologia, 18, 79-88. Eisler, H., & Eisler, A. D. (1992). Time perception: Effects of sex and sound intensity on scales of subjective duration. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 33, 339-358. Gallistel, C. R., & Gibbon, J. (2000). Time, rate, and conditioning. Psychological Review, 107, 289-344. Gibbon, J. (1971). Scalar timing and semi-markov chains in free-operant avoidance. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 8, 109-138. Grondin, S. (2001). From physical time to the first and second moments of psychological time. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 22-44. Killeen, P. R., & Weiss, N. A. (1987). Optimal timing and the Weber function. Psychological Review, 94, 455-468. Machado, A., & Arantes, J. (2006). Further tests of the Scalar Expectancy Theory (SET) and the Learning-to-Time model (LeT) in a temporal bisection task. Behavioural Processes, 72, 195-206. Thatcher, R. W., & John, E. R. (1977). The neural representation of time. In R. W. Thatcher and E. R. John (Eds.), Foundations of cognitive processes: Vol. 1. Functional neuroscience (pp. 165 179). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wearden, J. H. (1999). "Beyond the fields we know...": Exploring and developing scalar timing theory. Behavioural Processes, 45, 3-21. Zeiler, M. D., & Hoyert, M. S. (1989). Temporal reproduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 52, 81-95. 226