Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect. David K. Sherman

Similar documents
Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect

Approach/avoidance motivation, message framing, and health behavior: understanding the congruency effect

Understanding How Tailored Communications Work: The Effects of Message Quality and Congruency on Perceptions of Health Messages. John A.

The relation of approach/avoidance motivation and message framing to the effectiveness of charitable appeals

The Cultural Congruency Effect: Culture, Regulatory Focus, and the Effectiveness of Gain- vs. Loss-Framed Health. Messages. Ayse K.

The cultural congruency effect: Culture, regulatory focus, and the effectiveness of gain- vs. loss-framed health messages

Using Message Framing to Promote Healthful Behaviors: An Update

Health Message Framing: Moderators, Mediators, and Mysteries. John A. Updegraff. Kent State University. Alexander J. Rothman. University of Minnesota

CAHIER DE RECHERCHE n E3

The Role of the Self in Responses to Health Communications: A Cultural Perspective. David K. Sherman. University of California, Santa Barbara

Healthy choices in context: How contextual cues can influence the persuasiveness of framed health messages

The influence of (in)congruence of communicator expertise and trustworthiness on acceptance of CCS technologies

The role of the self in responses to health communications: A cultural perspective

Healthy choices in context: How contextual cues can influence the persuasiveness of framed health messages

PERSUASION PROCESSES UNDERLYING DIFFERENT METHODS OF MESSAGE FRAMING. A thesis submitted. to Kent State University in partial

The Mediating Effect of Processing Fluency between Message Framing and Persuasion

role of biologically-based personality systems in the prediction of coping behaviors. As a

VERDIN MANUSCRIPT REVIEW HISTORY REVISION NOTES FROM AUTHORS (ROUND 2)

THE HEALTH BELIEF MODEL. Health Behavior CHAPTER 6

How Does Person-Organization Fit Affect Behavioral And Attitudinal Outcomes?

Running head: Message format and message frame in STI risk communication. Utrecht University. Master Psychology, Social Psychology THESIS

Title: The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) and Texting While Driving Behavior in College Students MS # Manuscript ID GCPI

Title: The Relationship between Locus of Control and Academic Level and Sex of Secondary School Students

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

Exposure to lad magazines and drive for muscularity in dating and non-dating young men

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Measure of Social Presence

The moderating effects of direct and indirect experience on the attitude-behavior relation in the reasoned and automatic processing modes.

Research Approaches Quantitative Approach. Research Methods vs Research Design

Behavior Change Theories

Evaluating Smokers Reactions to Advertising for New Lower Nicotine Quest Cigarettes

Choosing designs and subjects (Bordens & Abbott Chap. 4)

COGNITIVE INNOVATIVENESS AS A PREDICTOR OF STUDENT ATTITUDES AND INTENT: AN APPLICATION OF THE THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOR TO TECHNOLOGY DELIVERED

Maio & Esses. Maio & Esses 2001 NAQ Laresen & Diener, 1987 Taylor, et al., 1985 Cacioppo Petty, 2001 the Need for Affect

Do misery and happiness both love company? The emotional consequences of listening to experiences shared by others.

Negative Life Events, Self-Perceived Competence, and Depressive Symptoms in Young Adults

An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines

Title: Determinants of intention to get tested for STI/HIV among the Surinamese and Antilleans in the Netherlands: results of an online survey

Kent Academic Repository

CHAPTER 6 CONCEPTUAL DEVELOPMENT

The American Dental Association 1 recommends

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation

Is This a Meal or Snack? Environmental Cues or Timing that Drive Food Intake. Brian Wansink. Mitsuru Shimizu. Collin R. Payne

Computer tailored persuasion: Working mechanisms. Arie Dijkstra University of Groningen The Netherlands

CHAPTER VI SUMMARY, CONCLUSIONS, IMPLICATIONS AND RECOMMENDATIONS. Premenstrual syndrome is a set of physical psycho emotional and behavioral

An Empirical Study on Causal Relationships between Perceived Enjoyment and Perceived Ease of Use

Computer tailored persuasion: effectiveness and side-effects. Arie Dijkstra University of Groningen The Netherlands

Autobiographical memory as a dynamic process: Autobiographical memory mediates basic tendencies and characteristic adaptations

Prospect Theory and Gain-Framing: The Relationship Between Cost Perception and Satisfaction

Approach and Avoidance Social Motives and Goals

Interaction Effects: Centering, Variance Inflation Factor, and Interpretation Issues

Optimistic Versus Pessimistic Life-Orientation Beliefs among University Teachers

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

Determinants of Oral Health Behavior in different cultures

Internal Consistency and Reliability of the Networked Minds Social Presence Measure

A study of association between demographic factor income and emotional intelligence

THESIS. Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University

Why Is Theory Important?

Are Experimental Economists Prone to Framing Effects? A Natural Field Experiment

Optimal Flow Experience in Web Navigation

Does the Relationship between Motivation and Performance Differ with Ability?

REPEATED MEASURES DESIGNS

Childhood Healthcare Experience, Healthcare Attitudes, and Optimism as Predictors of Adolescents Healthcare Behavior

Self-Efficacy in the Prediction of Academic Performance and Perceived Career Options

Relationships between stage of change for stress management behavior and perceived stress and coping

Affect and Acceptance: Examining the Effects of Positive Mood on Technology Acceptance Model

Self Determination Theory, COACHE, and Faculty Outcomes in Higher Education. Lisa M. Larson Mack C. Shelley Sandra W. Gahn Matthew Seipel

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Negative events such as failing an exam or finding. Pursuit of Comfort and Pursuit of Harmony: Culture, Relationships, and Social Support Seeking

KIMIN EOM. Ph.D., Social Psychology Psychological & Brain Sciences, University of California, Santa Barbara, CA

Report on the Ontario Principals Council Leadership Study. Executive Summary

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

Perceived Organisational Support and Affective Commitment

Volitional Autonomy and Relatedness: Mediators explaining Non Tenure Track Faculty Job. Satisfaction

Critical Thinking Assessment at MCC. How are we doing?

Paper: Influence of need for cognition and need for cognitive closure on information behaviour

Matching Versus Mismatching Attitude Functions: Implications for Scrutiny of Persuasive Messages

Running Head: ROLE OF BOUNDARY GOALS 1. Setting Lower Limits High: The Role of Boundary Goals in Achievement Motivation

Testing the Persuasiveness of the Oklahoma Academy of Science Statement on Science, Religion, and Teaching Evolution

Self-Consciousness and its Effects on Dissonance-Evoking Behavior

The Influence of Framing Effects and Regret on Health Decision-Making

Examining Temperament in Exercise Dependence and Eating Disorders

Regret: The Roles of Consistency-Fit and Counterfactual Salience

Weekly Paper Topics psy230 / Bizer / Fall xxarticles are available at idol.union.edu/bizerg/readings230xx

Chapter 7: Cognitive Aspects of Personality. Copyright Allyn & Bacon (2009)

David M. Keating Assistant Professor

Visualizing Success: Investigating the Relationship between Ability and Self-Efficacy in the Domain of Visual Processing

Framing the frame: How task goals determine the likelihood and direction of framing effects

Career Decision-Making Difficulties Perceived by College Students in Taiwan *

Faculty Influence and Other Factors Associated with Student Membership in Professional Organizations 1

The Stability of Undergraduate Students Cognitive Test Anxiety Levels

Writing Reaction Papers Using the QuALMRI Framework

An Investigation of Factors Influencing Causal Attributions in Managerial Decision Making

Measurement of Constructs in Psychosocial Models of Health Behavior. March 26, 2012 Neil Steers, Ph.D.

All Types of Mortality Salience Are Not Equal: The Effect of Contemplating Natural versus Unnatural Death on. Materialism Behavior

Vitanya Vanno *, Wannee Kaemkate, Suwimon Wongwanich

Many everyday choices are driven by underlying, ongoing

Transcription:

1 Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect David K. Sherman University of California, Santa Barbara Traci Mann University of California, Los Angeles John A. Updegraff Kent State University Running head: THE CONGRUENCY EFFECT Word Count: 2217 (text) Citation: Sherman, D. K., Mann, T., & Updegraff, J. A. (in press). Approach/avoidance orientation, message framing, and health behavior: Understanding the congruency effect. Motivation and Emotion. Manuscript Correspondence: David Sherman, Ph. D. Department of Psychology University of California, Santa Barbara Santa Barbara, CA 93106-9660 david.sherman@psych.ucsb.edu June 21, 2004

Abstract The Congruency Effect 2 Health messages framed to be congruent with individuals approach/avoidance motivations have been found to be more effective in promoting health behaviors than health messages incongruent with dispositional motivations. The present study examines the processes underlying this congruency effect. Participants (undergraduate students, N = 67) completed a measure of approach/avoidance orientation (the BIS/BAS scales) and read either a gain- or loss-framed message promoting dental flossing. Results demonstrated a congruency effect: Participants who read a congruently framed message had greater flossing efficacy, intended to floss more, and used more dental flosses than participants who read an incongruent message. Moreover, intention to perform the behavior predicted the congruency effect and self-efficacy mediated participants intentions to perform the health behavior. Discussion centers on the role of personality factors and situational factors in models of behavior change. Keywords: approach and avoidance motivation, message framing, health behavior, selfefficacy, behavioral intentions

3 Approach/Avoidance Motivation, Message Framing, and Health Behavior: Understanding the Congruency Effect Health communications can be framed in terms of the benefits of engaging in a particular behavior (a gain frame), or in terms of the costs of failing to engage in the behavior (a loss frame). Differential effects of gain and loss frames on behaviors are predicted by prospect theory (Tversky & Kahneman, 1981), which suggests that individuals are risk-seeking in the domain of losses and risk-averse in the domain of gains. This framework has been useful in determining how to most effectively frame health communications (Rothman & Salovey, 1997). The type of message frame that will be most effective in a particular situation depends, in part, on aspects of the individual being targeted by the health communication (Mann, Sherman, & Updegraff, 2004). According to several theories of individual differences in motivation (Carver, Sutton, & Scheier, 2000), behavior is regulated by two distinct systems, an approach system (the behavioral activation system, BAS, Gray, 1990) that regulates appetitive behavior towards potential rewards, and an avoidance system (the behavioral inhibition system, BIS, Gray, 1990) that regulates behavior away from potential threats or punishments. People with a strong approach orientation are more responsive to cues of reward, whereas people with a predominant avoidance orientation are more responsive to cues of threat and punishment (Carver et al., 2000). In a study demonstrating what we have termed the congruency effect, participants were classified as either approach- or avoidance-oriented and then read either a gain- or loss-framed article about the health behavior of dental flossing. When given the lossframed message, avoidance-oriented people flossed more than approach-oriented people

4 and when given the gain-framed message, approach-oriented people flossed more than avoidance-oriented people (Mann et al., 2004). In the current study, we examine the pathways through which the interaction of dispositional motivations and message framing leads to health behavior change. Models of health behavior change suggest three potential mediating psychological processes, which all may be affected by motivational orientations. First, people may be predisposed to notice cues congruent with their motivational orientation (Derryberry & Reed, 1994). Second, they may pursue goals congruent with their motivational orientation (Elliot & McGregor, 2001), so information framed to be congruent with motivational orientations may lead people to form intentions to perform the health behavior. A third related mediator is self-efficacy, which is important in the performance of a wide range of health behaviors (Bandura, 1998) and which may influence health behaviors directly, but also through the formation of goals or intentions (Ajzen, 1991). The present study explores these three potential mediators of the congruency effect for the health-promoting behavior of dental flossing. Participants motivational orientation was measured and then they read either a gain-framed or a loss-framed message on flossing. We assessed perception of the message, self-efficacy, and intentions to perform the behavior, and one week later we assessed flossing behavior. Method Participants 71 undergraduate students received extra credit in a psychology course for participation. Four participants were omitted for not completing all central measures, leaving a total of 67 participants (sex: 28 males, 39 females; age: M = 19.8 years;

5 ethnicity: 20 European Americans, 18 Asian/Asian-Americans, 8 Latinos/Latinas, 21 missing/other). Measure of Dispositional Motivation As part of a pre-test, participants completed the BIS/BAS scale (Carver & White, 1994), a 20-item scale that was designed to assess the relative strengths of people s approach (BAS) and avoidance (BIS) motivations. The seven BIS items (a =.80) measure concern over the possibility of bad occurrences and sensitivity to such events. The thirteen BAS items (a =.84) measure desire to approach positive occurrences. Procedures An undergraduate experimenter, unaware of the hypotheses of the study, ran participants individually. Participants completed the study about the processing of healthrelated messages 1. Participants were randomly assigned to read either the gain-framed or the loss-framed flossing article and had ten minutes to read it. The articles were adapted from the American Dental Association s web page. In the gain-framed message, entitled Great Breath, Healthy Gums Only a Floss Away, the potential benefits of regular flossing were emphasized. In the loss-framed message, titled Floss Now and Avoid Bad Breath and Gum Disease, the potential dangers of not flossing were emphasized. After reading the article, participants indicated their perceptions of the article on several dimensions: accurate, memorable, important, helpful, and useful, on appropriately labeled 7-point scales. These items were averaged to form a reliable index of perceptions of the article, (a =.87). Nine items assessed participants self-efficacy about their ability to floss over the coming week on 10-point scales (e.g., I can floss even if my gums bleed. a =.93). Participants also indicated their flossing goals by responding to an item

6 on flossing intentions, Over the next week, I intend to floss times. Response options ranged from 0 to 8+. At the end of the study, participants were given seven individually wrapped floss sachets, and were told to use them the next seven times they flossed. After one week, we emailed participants and asked how many times they flossed. Results Categorizing Avoidance versus Approach Orientations Our sample was divided into two groups based on responses to a pretest questionnaire: those who had higher BIS than BAS scores (avoidance people; N = 23) and those who had higher BAS than BIS scores (approach people; N = 44). Flossing Behavior We submitted the behavioral data to a 2 (Motivational Orientation: Approach vs. Avoidance) X 2 (Message Frame: Gain vs. Loss) ANOVA. There were no main effects, but there was a Motivational Orientation X Message Frame interaction, F (1, 63) = 5.51, p =.02. 2 As can be seen in Figure 1, when given the loss-framed article, avoidance people flossed more (M = 4.50, SE =.82) than approach people (M = 2.96, SE =.53). When given the gain-framed article, approach people flossed more (M = 4.00, SE =.58) than avoidance people (M = 2.39, SE =.72). Messages congruent with dispositional motivations were most effective at promoting health behaviors (as in Mann et al., 2004). Perceptions of the Health Message No main effects or interactions emerged when examining perceptions of the article as a dependent variable, F (1, 63) =.38, ns. Self-efficacy

7 There were no main effects but there was a Motivational Orientation X Message Frame interaction when examining self-efficacy as a dependent variable, F (1, 63) = 4.61, p =.04. When given a loss-framed article, avoidance people had stronger beliefs in their ability to floss regularly (M = 6.53, SE =.77) than approach people (M = 5.67, SE =.50). When given the gain-framed article, approach people had stronger beliefs in their ability to floss regularly (M = 7.16, SE =.54) than avoidance people (M = 5.31, SE =.67). Goals to Perform Health Behavior There were no main effects but there was a Motivational Orientation X Message Frame interaction when examining flossing intentions as a dependent variable, F (1, 63) = 9.33, p =.003. When given a loss-framed article, avoidance people had greater flossing intentions (M = 5.10, SE =.69) than approach people (M = 3.46, SE =.44). When given the gain-framed article, approach people had greater flossing intentions (M = 5.25, SE =.49) than avoidance people (M = 3.46, SE =.44). Mediational Analysis Perceptions of the article, self-efficacy, and flossing intentions were all examined as potential mediators of the congruency effect, following Baron and Kenny s (1986) three-step procedure for testing mediation. Specifically, for each of the three possible mediators, we used multiple regression to test (a) whether the planned interaction predicted the outcome of flossing behavior. For each mediator, the results of this step are identical; the interaction significantly predicts flossing, ß =.30, t (63) = 2.35, p =.02; (b) whether the planned interaction between motivational orientation and message frame predicted the proposed mediator; and (c) whether the proposed mediator remains a significant predictor of flossing behavior after controlling for the effect of the planned

8 interaction. 3 Lastly, Baron and Kenny s (1986) modification of the Sobel (1982) test for indirect effects was used to test the significance of the indirect path. With regard to perceptions of the article, the planned interaction between motivational orientation and message frame was not a significant predictor, ß =.31, t (63) =.62, p =.54. Thus, message perceptions did not mediate the congruency effect. With regard to flossing efficacy, the planned interaction between motivational orientation and message frame significantly predicted participants beliefs that they could floss regularly, ß =.27, t (63) = 2.15, p =.04. When self-efficacy and the planned interaction were included in a regression predicting flossing behavior, efficacy was a significant predictor of flossing, ß =.58, t (62) = 5.52, p <.001, but the planned interaction was no longer significant, ß =.14, t (62) = 1.30, p =.20. The modified Sobel test was also significant, Z = 1.98, p <.05, suggesting that flossing efficacy is a pathway by which motivational orientation and message framing influence flossing behavior. With regard to intentions to floss, the planned interaction between motivational orientation and message frame significantly predicted how much participants intended to floss over the upcoming week, ß =.38, t (63) = 3.05, p =.003. When intention and the planned interaction were included in a regression predicting flossing behavior, intention was a significant predictor of flossing behavior, ß =.67, t (62) = 6.70, p <.001, but the planned interaction was not, ß =.05, t (62) =.45, ns. The modified Sobel test was significant, Z = 2.81, p <.01, showing that intention is an additional pathway by which motivational orientation and message framing influence flossing behavior. Next, flossing efficacy and intention were both included in a simultaneous regression to determine the more proximal predictor of behavior. Intention emerged as

9 the more proximal predictor of flossing, ß =.51, t (64) = 3.81, p <.001, whereas the unique influence of efficacy on behavior was no longer significant, ß =.23, t (64) = 1.69, p =.10. However, in separate analyses, efficacy was a clear and significant predictor of intention, both in analyses where it was the predictor of intention, ß =.74, t (65) = 8.78, p <.001, as well as when simultaneously controlling for the direct effects of message frame and motivational orientation on intention, ß =.69, t (62) = 7.96, p <.001. Figure 2 shows the path model in which efficacy and intention predict the effect of motivational orientation and message frame on flossing behavior. This model fit the data well, c 2 (7) = 9.70, p =.21; NFI =.92; CFI =.97; RMSEA =.08) and other models including paths from the interaction to either intention or behavior were not significant. Discussion In the present study, dispositional motivation moderated the effectiveness of differentially framed health messages. Participants who had an approach orientation flossed more after reading a gain-framed article and participants who had an avoidance orientation flossed more after reading a loss-framed article. More importantly, the present study suggests that self-efficacy and intentions form a pathway by which dispositional motivations interact with message framing to produce positive health behaviors. In addition, the present study demonstrates that such factors as message framing and dispositional motivations can be integrated into larger theories of health behavior change. The theory of planned behavior (Ajzen, 1991) and social cognitive theory (Bandura, 1998) specify processes that lead to behavior change, such as perceived efficacy and intentions. The current study found both an individual difference factor (approach/avoidance motivation) and a situational factor (message framing) that interact

10 within the context of a particular health message to enhance self-efficacy, intentions, and behavior change. In so doing, the present research provides an attempt to integrate both person and situation variables within broad theories of behavior change. The present study joins a number of studies that have found that matching health messages to dispositional tendencies can increase the effectiveness of the message, such as need for cognition (Steward, Schneider, Pizarro, & Salovey, 2004) and monitorblunter coping style (Williams-Piehota, Pizarro, Schneider, Mowad, & Salovey, 2005). A common mechanism was suggested by Petty and Wegener (1998), who found that matched messages lead to greater scrutiny of the message, and consequently, greater persuasion. In support of this possibility, recent research in our lab (Updegraff, Sherman, & Mann, 2005) has found that the congruency effect obtains only for health messages with strong (and not weak) arguments in favor of the health behavior. The present study demonstrates that approach/avoidance motivation can moderate the differential effectiveness of gain- vs. loss-framed health messages. Moreover, the present study demonstrates the psychological processes underlying the congruency effect. Receiving a message that is congruent with a long-standing disposition leads to greater self-efficacy, stronger intentions to perform behavior, and subsequently, behavior change. Not only does the present study suggest important personality and situational factors that moderate the effectiveness of health messages, but it suggests that these same factors could be implemented among health-care practitioners in delivering health communications. Practitioners who determine the approach/avoidance motivation of a patient and deliver health messages framed accordingly may find their message more persuasive and effective at promoting positive health behavior.

References The Congruency Effect 11 Ajzen, I. (1991). The theory of planned behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50, 179-211. Bandura (1998). Health promotion from the perspective of social cognitive theory. Psychology and Health, 13, 623-649. Baron, R. M. & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182. Carver, C. S., Sutton, S. K., & Scheier, M. F. (2000). Action, emotion, and personality: Emerging conceptual integration. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 741-751. Carver, C. S., & White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319-333. Derryberry, D., & Reed, M. A. (1994). Temperament and attention: Orienting toward and away from positive and negative signals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 1128-1139. Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. A. (2001). A 2 x 2 achievement goal framework. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 501-519. Gray, J. A., (1990). Brain systems that mediate both emotion and cognition. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 269-288.

12 Mann, T., Sherman, D., & Updegraff, J. (2004). Dispositional motivations and message framing: A test of the congruency hypothesis in college students. Health Psychology, 23, 330-334. Petty, R. E., & Wegener, D. T. (1998). Matching versus mismatching attitude functions: Implications for scrutiny of persuasive messages. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24, 227-240. Rothman, A. J., & Salovey, P. (1997). Shaping perceptions to motivate healthy behavior: The role of message framing. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 3-19. Sobel, M. E. (1982). Asymptotic confidence intervals for indirect effects in structural equation models. In S. Leinhart (Ed.), Sociological Methodology 1982 (pp. 290-312). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Steward, W. T., Schneider, T. R., Pizarro, J., & Salovey, P. (2004). Need for cognition moderates responses to framed smoking cessation messages. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 19, 407-423. Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211, 453-458. Updegraff, J. A., Sherman, D. K., & Mann, T. (2005). Message quality and the congruency effect. Manuscript in preparation. Williams-Piehota, P., Pizarro, J., Schneider, T. R., Mowad, L., & Salovey, P. (2005). Matching health messages to monitor-blunter coping styles to motivate screening mammography. Health Psychology, 24, 58-67.

Author Notes The Congruency Effect 13 David Sherman, Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Barbara, Traci Mann, Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, and John Updegraff, Department of Psychology, Kent State University. This research was supported in part by a Council on Research Academic Senate Grant to the second author. We thank Shelly Gable, Curtis Hardin, Heejung Kim, David Pizarro, Matt Rohlfes, and Shelley Taylor for their helpful comments on an earlier version of this paper, and the following individuals for their assistance with these studies: Melissa Dunagan, Annie Kwan, Jen Mato, Dee Michelbock, Dani Pallafacchina, and Anna Park. Finally, we acknowledge Benjamin and Jonah Engelmann and Jacob and Isabelle Kim- Sherman for their continuing cheerful presence. This study was reported at the American Psychological Association Annual Convention, 2002. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to David Sherman, University of California, Santa Barbara, Department of Psychology, Santa Barbara, CA 93106. Electronic mail may be sent to david.sherman@psych.ucsb.edu.

Footnotes The Congruency Effect 14 1 Prior to reading the health article, participants first completed a computer-based anagram-solving task. There was a manipulation embedded in the anagram task. Half of the participants were given one ticket for every anagram they got correct (up to 10), and half of the participants were given 10 tickets, and one was taken away for every anagram they got incorrect. The manipulation did not affect flossing behavior or intentions and will not be mentioned further. 2 We tested our interaction hypothesis using two-way ANOVA and a categorical, rather than continuous, operationalization of motivational orientation. As our main dependent measure (flossing) showed substantial deviation from normality, ANOVA allows for a more robust test of the hypotheses as compared to moderated multiple regression. However, to ensure that our results were not simply an artifact of analytical procedure, we replicated the interaction by recoding the flossing measure into 3 roughly equal categories, and used ordered logit regression with a continuous (BIS minus BAS) rather than categorical measure of motivational orientation (interaction coefficient = - 1.92, standard error =.88, z = -2.17, p =.03). 3 In all steps of the mediational analyses, the main effects of message frame and motivational orientation were included, but are not reported for the sake of brevity.

15 Figure 1. The effect of message framing and approach/avoidance orientation on flossing behavior. Avoidance People Approach People Flosses Used 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 Loss-Framed Article Message Frame Gain-Framed Article

16 Figure 2. Path model showing efficacy and intention as predictors of the effect of approach/avoidance orientation and message frame on flossing behavior. * p <.05. ** p <.01.