Cognitive stimulation therapy (CST): neuropsychological mechanisms of change

Similar documents
Cognitive Stimulation Therapy (CST) Intervention in the Community

Setting The setting was institutional and tertiary care in London, Essex and Hertfordshire in the UK.

Process of a neuropsychological assessment

M P---- Ph.D. Clinical Psychologist / Neuropsychologist

NEUROPSYCHOMETRIC TESTS

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER

INTRODUCTION Cognitive stimulation is defined as engagement in a range of activities and discussions

House, Riding House Street, London, UK. Available online: 04 Jul 2011

An Initial Validation of Virtual Human Administered Neuropsychological Assessments

Efficacy of a Cognitive Training Program for Individuals with Moderate Cognitive Impairment: Evaluating Cognition

Test Assessment Description Ref. Global Deterioration Rating Scale Dementia severity Rating scale of dementia stages (2) (4) delayed recognition

Overview. Case #1 4/20/2012. Neuropsychological assessment of older adults: what, when and why?

Trail making test A 2,3. Memory Logical memory Story A delayed recall 4,5. Rey auditory verbal learning test (RAVLT) 2,6

Review Article The Acceptability and Usefulness of Cognitive Stimulation Therapy for Older Adults with Dementia: A Narrative Review

DOES IMPAIRED EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONING DIFFERENTIALLY IMPACT VERBAL MEMORY MEASURES IN OLDER ADULTS WITH SUSPECTED DEMENTIA?

Carmen Inoa Vazquez, Ph.D., ABPP Clinical Professor NYU School of Medicine Lead Litigation Conference Philadelphia May 19, 2009 Presentation

SOCIABLE - NEXT GENERATION COGNITIVE TRAINING USING MULTI-TOUCH SURFACE COMPUTERS

The benefits of groups that provide cognitive stimulation for people with dementia

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP) FOCUSED QUESTION:

Improving the Methodology for Assessing Mild Cognitive Impairment Across the Lifespan

The Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of Neuropsychological Status Effort Scale

Jung Wan Kim* Department of Speech Pathology of Daegu University, Gyeongsan, South Korea

Psychosocial Intervention: Memory Clinics and Evidence Based Practice

Service Related Project. Kimberley Keegan

Using Neuropsychological Experts. Elizabeth L. Leonard, PhD

Outcome measures in dementia care research

DEMENTIA, THE BRAIN AND HOW IT WORKS AND WHY YOU MATTER

Assessment of Memory

NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER

Using contextual analysis to investigate the nature of spatial memory

Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) Overview for Best Practice in Stroke and Complex Neurological Conditions March 2013

Health and Retirement Study Harmonized Cognitive Assessment Protocol (HCAP) Study Protocol Summary

AD Prevention Trials: An Industry Perspective

NATIONAL INSTITUTE FOR CLINICAL EXCELLENCE SCOPE. Dementia: the management of dementia, including the use of antipsychotic medication in older people

Use a diagnostic neuropsychology HOW TO DO IT PRACTICAL NEUROLOGY

Recognizing Dementia can be Tricky

Base Rates of Impaired Neuropsychological Test Performance Among Healthy Older Adults

21/05/2018. Today s webinar will answer. Presented by: Valorie O Keefe Consultant Psychologist

Cognitive Stimulation Therapy (CST) for people with dementia in practice: A service

SOCIABLE DELIVERABLE D7.2a Interim Assessment of the SOCIABLE Platform and Services

Neuropsychological Evaluation of

AGED SPECIFIC ASSESSMENT TOOLS. Anna Ciotta Senior Clinical Neuropsychologist Peninsula Mental Health Services

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

LOTCA Assessment review. Georgina Wrack. University of the Sunshine Coast

Evidence profile: cognitive impairment

Alzheimer s & Dementia Intervention Program A Case Review

Dementia NICE Guidelines Update. Key points for primary care - NICE guideline (June 2018 update ) 26 September 2018

Treatment of AD with Stabilized Oral NADH: Preliminary Findings

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

Critically Appraised Paper for Efficacy of occupational therapy for patients with Parkinson's disease: A randomized controlled trial

The evaluation of individual Cognitive Stimulation Therapy (icst) for dementia. Lauren Yates Research assistant & PhD student

Cognitive Screening in Risk Assessment. Geoffrey Tremont, Ph.D. Rhode Island Hospital & Alpert Medical School of Brown University.

(Received 30 March 1990)

Interpreting change on the WAIS-III/WMS-III in clinical samples

Cognitive training in Alzheimer s disease: a meta-analysis of the literature

Critical Review: Does reminiscence therapy including life story work improve the quality of life of people with dementia?

WHI Memory Study (WHIMS) Investigator Data Release Data Preparation Guide April 2014

Cognitive Rehabilitation of Mildly Impaired Alzheimer Disease Patients on Cholinesterase Inhibitors

CommonKnowledge. Pacific University. Stefanie Fendrick Pacific University. Recommended Citation. Notice to Readers

Accessibility and Disability Service. A Guide to Services for Students with

Clinical Study Depressive Symptom Clusters and Neuropsychological Performance in Mild Alzheimer s and Cognitively Normal Elderly

PRELIMINARY NORMS FOR YEAR OLDS ON THE MEMORY TEST FOR OLDER ADULTS (MTOA:S) ABSTRACT

CLINICAL NEUROPSYCHOLOGY PSYC32

A comparison of the content of memory rehabilitation groups for patients with neurological disabilities

ORIGINAL ARTICLE Neuroscience INTRODUCTION MATERIALS AND METHODS

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

Stop Delirium! A complex intervention for delirium in care homes for older people

Understand the Process and Experience of Dementia

The treatment of postnatal depression: a comprehensive literature review Boath E, Henshaw C

Delirium. Quick reference guide. Issue date: July Diagnosis, prevention and management

Adaptive Design in CIAS

Petra Bevilacqua Psychologist

Recommendations on Screening for Cognitive Impairment in Older Adults 2015

1 Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, UK 2 Department of Psychology, Harit-Watt University, Edinburgh, UK

Funnelling Used to describe a process of narrowing down of focus within a literature review. So, the writer begins with a broad discussion providing b

NATIONAL INSTITUTE FOR HEALTH AND CLINICAL EXCELLENCE. Health Technology Appraisal

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

EARLY ONSET FRONTOTERMPORAL DEMENTIA AND ALZHEIMERS DISEASE: DIAGNOSIS, TREATMENT AND CARE

Present by Dr Phuong Leung Dr Vasiliki Orgeta Professor Martin Orrell. Division of Psychiatry University College London

Surveillance report Published: 13 April 2017 nice.org.uk. NICE All rights reserved.

Hubley Depression Scale for Older Adults (HDS-OA): Reliability, Validity, and a Comparison to the Geriatric Depression Scale

Charles P. Sabatino ABA Commission on Law and Aging May 20, 2009

Korean-VCI Harmonization Standardization- Neuropsychology Protocol (K-VCIHS-NP)

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

Cognitive remediation in Recent Onset Schizophrenia: Results of an effectiveness trial

Aims for todays session

Contextual bias and inferencing in adults with right hemisphere brain damage It is widely accepted that damage to the right cerebral hemisphere (RHD)

Demystifying the Neuropsychological Evaluation Report. Clinical Neuropsychologist 17 March 2017 Program Director, Neurobehavioral Program

Test review. Comprehensive Trail Making Test (CTMT) By Cecil R. Reynolds. Austin, Texas: PRO-ED, Inc., Test description

Drug Update. Treatments for Cognitive Impairment in the Older Adult. William Solan, M.D. Karen Sanders, Ph.D. Northwest Hospital Seattle

GREAT. Goal-oriented Cognitive Rehabilitation in Early-stage Alzheimer s and Related Dementias: Multi-centre Single-blind Randomised Controlled Trial

Plenary Session 2 Psychometric Assessment. Ralph H B Benedict, PhD, ABPP-CN Professor of Neurology and Psychiatry SUNY Buffalo

Naming Test of the Neuropsychological Assessment Battery: Convergent and Discriminant Validity

Cognitive Impairment - Parkinson's Disease Foundation (PDF)

Neuropsychological Performance in Cannabis Users and Non-Users Following Motivation Manipulation

CRITICALLY APPRAISED PAPER (CAP)

Home Based Memory Rehabilitation Programme

Cognitive Stimulation Therapy - New Zealand and an International Perspective

Transcription:

International Psychogeriatrics (2013), 25:3, 479 489 C International Psychogeriatric Association 2012 doi:10.1017/s1041610212001822 Cognitive stimulation therapy (CST): neuropsychological mechanisms of change... Louise Hall, 1 Martin Orrell, 2 Joshua Stott 1 and Aimee Spector 1 1 Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, UK 2 Research Department of Mental Health Sciences, University College London, 67 73 Riding House Street, London, W1W 7EJ, UK ABSTRACT Background: Cognitive stimulation therapy (CST) is an evidence-based psychosocial intervention for people with dementia consisting of 14 group sessions aiming to stimulate various areas of cognition. This study examined the effects of CST on specific cognitive domains and explored the neuropsychological processes underpinning any effects. Methods: A total of 34 participants with mild to moderate dementia were included. A one-group pretest posttest design was used. Participants completed a battery of neuropsychological tests in the week before and after the manualised seven-week CST programme. Results: There were significant improvement pre- to post-cst group on measures of delayed verbal recall (WMS III logical memory subtest delayed), visual memory (WMS III visual reproduction subtest delayed), orientation (WMS III information and orientation subscale), and auditory comprehension (Token Test). There were no significant changes on measures of naming (Boston Naming Test-2), attention (Trail Making Test A/Digit Span), executive function (DKEFS verbal fluency/trail Making Test B), praxis (WMS III visual reproduction immediate) or on a general cognitive screen (MMSE). Conclusions: Memory, comprehension of syntax, and orientation appear to be the cognitive domains most impacted by CST. One hypothesis is that the language-based nature of CST enhances neural pathways responsible for processing of syntax, possibly also aiding verbal recall. Another is that the reduction in negative self-stereotypes due to the de-stigmatising effect of CST may impact on language and memory, domains that are the primary focus of CST. Further research is required to substantiate these hypotheses. Key words: Alzheimer s disease, dementia, cognition, memory, language Introduction Cognitive stimulation for people with dementia In recent years, there has been growing interest in a number of cognition-focussed psychological interventions for the treatment of mild to moderate dementia (e.g. Clare and Woods, 2004). These interventions are based on the rationale that people in the earlier stages of dementia can learn and be rehabilitated (Clare et al., 2000). One such intervention, cognitive stimulation, has been defined by Clare and Woods (2004) as engagement in a range of group activities and discussions aimed Correspondence should be addressed to: Aimee Spector, PhD, DClinPsy, Research Department of Clinical, Educational and Health Psychology, University College London, Gower Street, London WC1E 6BT, United Kingdom. Phone: +44 (0)20 7679 1844. Email: a.spector@ucl.ac.uk. Received 29 May 2012; revision requested 12 Jul 2012; revised version received 3 Oct 2012; accepted 4 Oct 2012. First published online 12 November 2012. at general enhancement of cognitive and social functioning (p. 387). The central assumption underlying cognitive stimulation is that a lack of cognitive stimulation can hasten decline in both normal aging and dementia (Salthouse, 2006). Support for this assumption comes from increasing evidence that activation of neurons may enhance neuronal function and survival (Swaab et al., 2002). An evidence-based group cognitive stimulation therapy (CST) programme was developed and evaluated in a large multicentre, single-blind, randomised controlled trial (RCT; Spector et al., 2003). A significant benefit was found for the treatment group compared with the treatment as usual control group in two measures of cognitive function: the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975) and the cognitive subscale of the Alzheimer s Disease Assessment Scale (ADAS-Cog; Rosen et al., 1984). In addition,

480 L. Hall et al. the authors found benefits to quality of life and showed that these were mediated by the improvements in cognition (Woods et al., 2006). On the basis of this trial, the National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence Social Care Institute for Excellence (NICE-SCIE) in the United Kingdom recommended structured group CST as the non-pharmacological treatment of choice for the cognitive symptoms of mild to moderate dementia of all types (NICE-SCIE, 2007). A recent Cochrane review of cognitive stimulation (Woods et al., 2012) concluded that there is strong evidence that cognitive stimulation has a positive impact on cognitive function, depression, activities of daily living, and behaviour for people with dementia, over and above any medication effects. Despite strong evidence that cognitive stimulation leads to generalised cognitive benefits, very few studies to date have looked at its impact on specific areas of cognition. Most have looked only at generic cognitive change on brief measures such as the MMSE and ADAS-Cog (Woods et al., 2012). Little is currently known about whether some aspects of cognition change more than others and why. Only two trials of cognitive stimulation to date have examined neuropsychological outcomes in depth. Participants in Breuil et al. (1994) completed the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer s Disease (CERAD) neuropsychological test battery (Morris et al., 1989) together with an additional battery of three tests: a picture pair associate test, an immediate associate test, and a further test of verbal fluency. Alongside a statistically significant improvement in MMSE scores, they found a non-significant trend towards improvement on the CERAD word list memory task and on one of the association tests. They did not find any change in verbal fluency and they discarded the remaining five items of the CERAD battery due to ceiling and floor effects. The authors concluded that cognitive stimulation appears to induce increased memory performance; however, they did not speculate on the possible mechanisms for this change. Spector et al. (2010) investigated the effects of their CST programme on specific areas of cognition, by analysing the three subscales of the ADAS-Cog (memory and new learning, praxis, and language) using the data from their RCT (Spector et al., 2003). They found a statistically significant improvement in the language subscale in the treatment group compared with the control group, with statistically significant improvements found on the commands and spoken language items. There were no significant changes in praxis and, in contrast to Breuil et al. s (1994) findings, there were no changes in memory and new learning. The authors concluded that CST appears to have particular effects in promoting language. In terms of mechanisms of change, Spector et al. (2010) suggested that this could be through generating opinions and creating new semantic links through categorisation, and that through encouragement of expression of views and opinions, CST may improve conversation and communication. They suggested that this may have generalised benefits, helping to explain why improvements in quality of life were mediated by changes in cognition. Limitations of current findings The frequent use of brief screening tools in research trials, such as the MMSE and ADAS-Cog, has limited the evaluation of cognitive outcomes to date. The ADAS-Cog is a research tool as opposed to a clinical tool and does not assess cognitive domains in any depth. It has been widely recognised as suboptimal with respect to the measurement of some areas of cognition that are often compromised in dementia including attention, planning, working memory, and executive function (Mohs et al., 1997). Three language items are rated through the assessor s subjective observations and it lacks sensitivity for measuring cognitive change in patients with mild dementia. Aims and objectives This study aimed to investigate the effects of the CST programme of Spector et al. (2003) on specific areas of cognition in depth, using more objective, sensitive, and detailed neuropsychological testing. It aimed to address the following questions: Which cognitive domains show improvement in people with dementia following participation in group CST? Which neuropsychological processes might underpin these changes? Method Participants and settings S AMPLING STRATEGY Services running CST groups for people with dementia were identified by contacting professionals who had attended a CST training workshop. Of these 100 services, eight agreed to participate. These services consisted of two National Health Service (NHS) memory clinics, three NHS day hospitals, and three local authority day centres. Group facilitators identified potential participants through the usual CST group recruitment

Neuropsychological mechanisms of change in CST 481 processes within each service. Potential participants and their family caregiver(s), when possible, were then contacted to discuss the study further and to arrange an appointment to provide written informed consent, be screened for suitability, and complete the pre-group assessment. I NCLUSION CRITERIA These followed the criteria outlined by Spector et al. (2003). People were considered suitable for participation in the study if they: (a) Had been given a diagnosis of dementia; (b) Had mild to moderate dementia (MMSE score of 10 or more); (c) Had some ability to communicate and understand communication; (d) Had English as their first language or had sufficient English to complete the neuropsychological tests validly; (e) Were able to see and hear well enough to participate in the group and complete the neuropsychological tests; (f) Did not have a learning disability; (g) Did not have a major physical illness or disability which would significantly limit their participation in the group; (h) Were not too impaired to understand the nature of the study. S AMPLE SIZE A power analysis was conducted using the G Power 3 computer program (Faul et al., 2007). Using the results from Spector et al. (2003), it was estimated that a sample size of 42 was required to achieve 80% power to detect an effect size of 0.31 using a within group t-test with a 0.05 (two-tailed) level of significance. In the eight participating services, group facilitators identified a total of 74 people who were interested in taking part in a CST group. Of these, 46 were screened, 41 entered the study and, of these, 34 completed both a pre-group and postgroup assessment (Figure 1). D EMOGRAPHICS OF FINAL SAMPLE The final sample of 34 participants consisted of 14 males and 20 females who were either white British (n = 32) or white European (n = 2) and were aged between 57.7 and 92.7 years (M = 80.3, SD = 8.8). The majority of the sample were given a diagnosis of dementia unspecified (n = 12). The remainder had been diagnosed with Alzheimer s (n = 8), vascular dementia (n = 7), mixed dementia (n = 5), fronto-temporal dementia (n = 1), and dementia with Lewy bodies (n = 1). Regarding severity of cognitive impairment, baseline MMSE scores ranged from 10 to 29 (M = 20.3, SD = People who agreed to take part in a CST group (n = 74) People who agreed to be screened by researcher (n = 46) People Included (n = 41) People excluded (n = 5) Too hearing impaired: 1 Drop outs (n = 7) Became distressed during initial assessment: 2 Died before follow-up: 1 Refused follow-up: 1 Too physically ill at followup: 3 Completed study (n = 34) Figure 1. Profile of attrition. Too visually impaired: 1 Unable to consent: 2 Too physically ill: 1 5.1). The majority of participants reported leaving education after high school (n = 25), some reported attending technical college (n = 6), and a small number reported attending university (n = 3). Approximately half of the participants (n = 16) were taking anti-dementia medication (cholinesterase inhibitors) at the commencement of the study, with most taking donepezil (n = 14), one participant taking galantamine, and one participant taking rivastigmine. Information regarding the length of time participants had been taking cholinesterase inhibitors was available for 11 participants and ranged from 1.6 to 35.9 months (M = 11.2, SD = 11.5). Only one participant had been taking antidementia medication for less than two months. Ethics The appropriate multi-centre and local research ethics committees granted ethical approval. Only people able to provide informed consent were included. A caregiver (either a member of staff or a family member) was asked to witness the informed consent process whenever possible. Research design and procedure The study used a one-group pretest posttest design. Participants completed a neuropsychological test battery (Table 1), which took approximately 1 1.5 hours in the week prior to and the week following CST. All assessments were conducted strictly according to test instructions and tests were administered in the same order for each participant. I NTERVENTION The 14-session (two 45-minute sessions per week over seven weeks) manualised CST programme

Table 1. Neuropsychological test battery TEST AUTHOR(S) BRIEF DESCRIPTION OF TEST SCORE(S) PRIMARY COGNITIVE DOMAIN(S) ASSESSED... WMS-III Information and Orientation WMS-III Logical Memory WMS-III Visual Reproduction Wechsler (1997) Wechsler (1997) Participants answer a series of questions regarding orientation to time and place and personal and general knowledge. Participants recall two short stories read aloud by the examiner both immediately and after a 25 35 minute delay. A yes/no recognition task follows delayed recall. Wechsler (1997) Participants look at geometric figures for 10 seconds and then reproduce them from memory. Participants are asked to reproduce the figures from memory after 25 35 minutes and then recognise which figures they have seen before. In addition, participants are asked to simply copy the line drawings. WMS-III Digit Span Wechsler (1997) Pairs of random number sequences of increasing length are read aloud at the pace of one per second. In the digit span forwards subtest participants are required to repeat each sequence exactly as it is given. In the backwards subtest, participants are asked to repeat the sequence in reverse order. Literature does not support a conceptual distinction between forwards and backwards digit span (Strauss et al., 2006). Token Test Boston Naming Test-2 (BNT-2) Spreen and Benton (1969, 1977) as reproduced in Strauss et al. (2006) Kaplan et al. (2001) Participants follow a series of verbal commands of increasing length and complexity. Participants name a series of drawings of objects that are graded in terms of their familiarity. Total score LMI Recall (immediate recall) LMI Thematic (immediate thematic recall) LMII Recall (delayed recall) LMII Thematic (delayed thematic recall) LMII Recognition (delayed recognition) VRI Recall (immediate recall) VRII Recall (delayed recall) VRII Recognition (delayed recognition) VRII Copy Total score Total accuracy score Total correct Orientation Memory: Verbal episodic learning, recall, and recognition Memory: Visual episodic learning, recall, and recognition Constructional praxis Auditory Working Memory Language Comprehension Language Expression: Naming 482 L. Hall et al.

D-KEFS Verbal Fluency Delis et al. (2001) Participants begin by saying as many words as they can that begin with a specified letter (Letter Fluency) within one minute. Participants are then asked to say as many words as they can that belong to a designated semantic category (Category Fluency). Participants are then asked to switch between saying items from two semantic categories (Category Switching). Trail Making Reitan and Wolfson (1992) This test consists of two parts labelled A and B. Trail Making A consists of 25 consecutive numbered circles that participants connect by drawing a line through each element in the series. Trail Making B is a more complex task in which a series of numbers (1 13) and letters (A L) are presented on the page within circles. Participants are required to work through the entire set alternately connecting numbers and letters as rapidly as possible. Note. WMS-III: Wechsler Memory Scale 3rd edn; D-KEFS: Delis-Kaplan executive function system. Letter fluency total correct Category fluency total correct Category switching accuracy Time taken to complete Trails A Time taken to complete Trails B Language Expression: Phonological and Semantic Fluency Executive Function: Switching/Cognitive Flexibility Attention Executive Function: Cognitive Flexibility Neuropsychological mechanisms of change in CST 483

484 L. Hall et al. was followed (Spector et al., 2006). Themed sessions involve topics including word association, object categorisation, current affairs, and famous faces. The programme follows key principles including implicit rather than explicit learning (with a focus on information processing rather than factual knowledge), multisensory stimulation, reminiscence to aid orientation and creating continuity between sessions. In each service, the CST programme was facilitated by one or more people (e.g. care worker, nurse, occupational therapist or assistant psychologist) who had attended a CST training workshop. Facilitators were asked to complete an adherence checklist of the key principles of CST after each session. Measures S CREENING TOOL Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE; Folstein et al., 1975). This brief, widely used test of cognitive function was used to determine the level of severity of dementia. It was also re-administered at followup in order to make comparisons with the findings of the original Spector et al. (2003) RCT. The MMSE has good reliability and validity. R ATIONALE FOR SELECTION OF COGNITIVE DOMAINS AND NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL TESTS The following cognitive domains were assessed, being areas that tend to become impaired in dementia according to Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) criteria (APA, 2000): Orientation Attention/working memory Memory (verbal and non-verbal) Language (expression and comprehension) Executive function Praxis All neuropsychological tests selected for the battery (see Table 1) were considered suitable for assessing older people, had age appropriate norms (Clare, 2008), had good reliability, validity, and sensitivity and had a relative lack of ceiling and floor effects in mild to moderate dementia. A primary measure was identified for each cognitive ability (referencing Strauss et al., 2006). As some measures generate several scores assessing different aspects of cognition, only the most relevant scores were calculated and analysed (Table 1). Further considerations were that the total battery should take no longer than 1.5 hours to administer, minimizing burden on participants; and that the key cognitive functions assessed should overlap minimally in order to reduce redundancy. Additionally, the Wechsler Test of Adult Reading (WTAR; Wechsler, 2001) was administered to provide an estimate of premorbid intellectual functioning. Data analysis Data were analysed using SPSS version 17.0. Several distributions had statistically significant levels of skewness and kurtosis at the 0.01 level. For these variables, a non-parametric two-tailed Wilcoxon test was used instead of paired t-tests to examine within-participant changes post- CST at the 0.05 level. Severity of impairment (baseline MMSE score) between completers and non-completers was examined. Results Of the 39 participants at baseline, 34 completed a follow-up assessment (Figure 1). One or more of the tests were not completed at baseline, follow-up, or both by 12 participants due to refusal, fatigue, or difficulty retaining task instructions. The number of participants who completed each test at each stage is reported in Table 2. The mean baseline MMSE score of the 22 participants who completed every test at both pre- and post-group assessment (M = 22.32,SD = 4.05) was significantly higher than that of the 12 participants who did not complete one or more items (M = 16.58, SD = 4.80) (t(32) = 3.67, p = 0.001). The mean number of sessions attended by participants was 13.1 (SD = 1.7, range 6 14) and 85% of participants (n = 29) attended 13 or 14 sessions. Across the eight participating services, 14 CST groups were run in total with between four and seven participants in each CST group. Adherence to key principles Adherence checklists were completed and returned by four of the participating services for four out of the 14 CST groups. Within these, the percentage adherence to the key CST principles across all 14 of the CST sessions ranged from 85% to 100% (M = 94.15, SD = 6.37). Floor and ceiling effects No ceiling effects were found on any of the measures used; however, there were large floor effects on the Trail Making B test, delayed recall variable (LMII Recall) of the WMS-III Logical Memory test, and the two WMS-III delayed recognition variables (LMII Recognition and VRII Recognition).

Neuropsychological mechanisms of change in CST 485 Premorbid intellectual functioning The WTAR was completed by 33 of the 34 participants, with one participant having lost the ability to read. Although the WTAR is relatively resistant to brain damage (Strauss et al., 2006), the performance of several participants (e.g. those with more significant language difficulties) may have been significantly affected by their cognitive impairments. Hence, the mean standard score of 93.73 (SD = 21.58) may have been somewhat of an underestimate. MMSE and orientation There was no statistically significant change over time on the MMSE (Table 2). There was a significant improvement in scores on the WMS- III Information and Orientation test at follow-up compared to baseline (Table 2). Memory In terms of verbal memory, there was a significant improvement on the immediate and delayed thematic recall variables of the WMS-III Logical Memory test at the 0.05 level (Table 2). Trends towards statistical significance were also found on the delayed recall and recognition variables but not on the immediate recall variable (Table 2). A significant improvement over time was also found on the delayed recall variable of the WMS-III Visual Reproduction test (Table 2) which measures visual memory. No significant changes were found on the immediate recall or recognition variables (Table 2). Attention/working memory A trend towards a statistically significant decline was found on the total score of the WAIS-III Digit Span subtest, which is a measure of attention/working memory (Table 2). There was no significant change over time on the Trail Making A test which is also a measure of attention (Table 2). Language In terms of language comprehension, a significant improvement over time was found on the Token Test (Table 2). This test is a measure of auditory comprehension of commands. With regard to language expression, there were no statistically significant changes on measures of naming (BNT- 2) or verbal fluency (D-KEFS) over time (Table 2). Executive function There were no significant changes over time on the D-KEFS verbal fluency switching task, which assesses cognitive flexibility. The Trail Making B test, which also measures this domain, suffered from large floor effects with only five participants able to complete this task at both pre-group and postgroup assessments. The results for this measure were therefore not analysed. Praxis Praxis was assessed using the copy component of the WMS-III Visual Reproduction test. There were no significant changes over time on this measure (Table 2). Discussion Main findings The findings of this study suggest that memory, language comprehension, and orientation may be the cognitive domains most amenable to improvement in people with mild to moderate dementia who participate in group CST. The results also indicate that CST may not have an impact on the cognitive domains of attention/working memory, executive function, or praxis. Comparison with previous findings Overall, these findings are consistent with the conclusion that cognitive stimulation can have a positive impact on cognitive function (Woods et al., 2012). Furthermore, the findings are partially consistent with those of Spector et al. (2010) that language is the domain most influenced by cognitive stimulation. The improvement in language comprehension found in the current study is commensurate with Spector et al. s finding of an improvement in the ability to follow commands. On the contrary, no statistically significant improvements were found on two measures of language expression (naming and fluency) whereas Spector et al. found an improvement in spoken language. In contrast to the current findings, Spector et al. (2010) found no changes in memory and concluded that this was consistent with the approach of cognitive stimulation which does not emphasise explicit rehearsal of information. On the other hand, an improvement in memory is consistent with Breuil et al. s (1994) finding of a trend towards improvement on two measures of memory. A further discrepancy between the current findings and the findings of previous studies is that both Spector et al. (2003) and Breuil et al. (1994) found a statistically significant improvement on the MMSE, whereas no improvements were found on this measure of general cognitive function in the current study.

Table 2. Changes from baseline on neuropsychological test battery at follow-up 486 L. Hall et al. MEAN SCORE MEAN SCORE MEAN CHANGE t(df) OR AT BASELINE AT FOLLOW-UP FROM BASELINE W ILCOXON SIG. MEASURE n M ( SD) M ( SD) M ( SD) Z (2- TAILED)... MMSE 34 20.29(5.08) 20.41(5.09) +0.12(2.43) 0.28(33) 0.780 WMS-III Information and Orientation 34 8.38(2.87) 9.06(2.52) +0.68(2.72) 1.45(33) 0.003 WMS-III Logical Memory LMI Recall 34 8.79(8.29) 9.59(9.54) +0.79(3.97) 1.17(33) 0.252 LMI Thematic 34 4.59(4.55) 5.88(5.55) +1.29(3.33) 2.27(33) 0.030 LMII Recall 34 1.71(2.55) 2.59(3.88) +0.88(2.82) 1.80 0.071 LMII Thematic 34 1.15(2.00) 2.00(2.91) +0.85(2.22) 2.14 0.032 LMII Recognition 29 18.17(3.04) 19.62(2.94) +1.45(3.84) 2.03(28) 0.052 WMS-III Visual Reproduction VRI Recall 33 26.27(17.11) 27.03(19.24) +0.76(13.69) 0.32(32) 0.753 VRII Recall 33 1.94(5.02) 4.70(9.14) +2.76(6.94) 2.20 0.028 VRII Recognition 29 31.17(4.55) 31.76(4.76) +0.59(4.53) 0.70(28) 0.492 VRII Copy 32 79.72(23.06) 80.28(21.49) +0.56(13.42) 0.19 0.852 WMS-III Digit Span Total 34 11.62(3.95) 10.88(3.81) 0.74(2.79) 1.72 0.085 Token Test 32 143.75(15.89) 147.19(14.49) +5.53(13.99) 2.68 0.007 BNT-2 34 33.59(13.36) 34.06(13.75) +0.47(3.28) 0.84(33) 0.408 D-KEFS Verbal Fluency Letter Fluency 28 21.75(13.34) 21.57(11.58) 0.18(7.04) 0.23 0.819 Category Fluency 30 15.97(8.94) 16.33(9.91) +0.37(4.88) 0.41(29) 0.684 Category Switching Accuracy 28 3.14(2.73) 3.36(3.16) +0.46(2.89) 0.44(27) 0.665 Trail Making A 30 104.80(67.45) 93.53(47.53) 11.27(47.27) 1.31 0.202 Note. Please refer to Table 1 for full titles of measures. For all measures except Trail Making A, a higher score represents a better performance. p < 0.10; p < 0.05; p < 0.01.

Neuropsychological mechanisms of change in CST 487 There are a number of possible reasons for these discrepancies. This study might have picked up previously undetected changes, due to the complexity and sensitivity of the test used (as opposed to blunter tools, such as the MMSE and ADAS-Cog, used in previous research). Language has multiple components (Hodges, 2007) and it is therefore possible that the qualitative improvements in spoken language found by Spector et al. (2010) were due to a component other than naming or fluency, which were the only two aspects of language measured in the current study. Another possibility is that the smaller sample size in the current study lacked power to detect an effect in the domain of language expression. The failure to replicate an improvement on the MMSE could also be due to a lack of power to detect a relatively small effect. In terms of the discrepancy in the memory findings, one possibility is that the word-list recall task, on which the Spector et al. (2010) conclusions were based, may be less sensitive to improvements than the story recall task used in the current study which, perhaps, has more ecological validity. Neuropsychological mechanisms of change One can cautiously speculate on the possible meaning of the findings in terms of understanding the neuropsychological mechanisms of change underpinning cognitive stimulation. One possibility is that the emphasis on verbal communication, central to the CST programme, could help to preserve syntax (i.e. the correct use of nonsubstantive components of language such as articles and prepositions according to strict grammatical rules; Hodges, 2007). This hypothesis would fit with the finding of an improvement on the Token Test which involves comprehension of increasingly syntactically complex commands. There is an increasing amount of literature supporting the use it or lose it principle which refers to the idea that activation of neurons may have a beneficial effect on neuronal function and survival during both normal aging and dementia (Swaab et al., 2002). The positive reinforcement of thinking, questioning, and expressing opinions throughout the CST programme could enhance the use of the neural pathways responsible for syntax and thus preserve this cognitive function. Given the focus of CST on implicit learning and the absence of explicit rehearsal of material or training in encoding and retrieval strategies, the improvements found on both the verbal and non-verbal memory tests are somewhat unexpected. The verbal memory task involved listening to and recalling two short stories both immediately and after a delay. A parsimonious explanation for the improvement found on this task is that this could be mediated by an improvement in the ability to use and comprehend syntax since this would be likely to facilitate the comprehension and re-telling of stories. This does not, however, account for the enhancement in delayed visual recall. An alternative explanation is that the intervention directly enhances learning and memory. The sessions create a positive but challenging learning environment which is designed to be optimal for people with dementia. It is possible that this environment could directly stimulate neuronal systems supporting memory in the brain, thus enhancing encoding and retrieval capacities (Swaab et al., 2002). The functioning of existing neuronal networks could thus be enhanced through stimulation but it is also possible that the challenges posed by CST promote the functioning of alternative neuronal pathways (Stern, 2002). Time and place information is directly questioned at the beginning of each CST session. The findings of an improvement in orientation may reflect learning based on the explicit rehearsal of this information twice weekly. Other mechanisms of change There is evidence that negative age stereotypes can have a detrimental impact on the performance of older people on neuropsychological measures of memory (Hess et al., 2004). CST is a de-stigmatising intervention which values and encourages participants opinions and views. This could lead to a reduction in negative selfstereotyping, which could have a positive impact on test performance. The cognitive domains which appear to be most influenced by CST are language and memory. These domains are the main focus of CST and thus are likely to be the aspects of cognitive functioning for which stigma is most reduced. Limitations The findings need to be considered as preliminary and interpreted conservatively. The pretest posttest design of the study meant that only complete data sets could be included in the analysis. The baseline differences in MMSE scores between those who completed the full battery of measures and those who failed to complete one or more items suggest that the findings are somewhat biased towards those with a higher level of overall cognitive functioning. The lack of a control group means that it is not possible to entirely rule out the possibility that the improvements found were simply due to placebo or practice effects. However, it is unlikely that participants with dementia would have much, if any, explicit recall of the content of the tests after a

488 L. Hall et al. nine-week period and there is evidence demonstrating that practice effects do not occur in people with dementia, even in the mildest stages (Cooper et al., 2001). Furthermore, control participants in the original Spector et al. (2003) RCT declined over time on both the MMSE and ADAS-Cog, thus demonstrating an absence of practice effects on these measures. A further drawback of not having a control group was that it was only possible to assess for improvements in cognitive function. A positive outcome could be maintenance of cognitive function (i.e. no change) given the expected trajectory of decline. The limitations created by the lack of a control group were, however, outweighed by the ethical difficulties in creating a waiting list control group for a treatment with proven efficacy. There was no control for the use of antidementia medication. Of those taking medication, the majority were on a stable dose of cholinesterase inhibitor (i.e. they had been taking their current dose for at least two months) at pre-group assessment and therefore any improvements in cognitive functioning due to medication should have already plateaued. However, information regarding length of medication use was not available for all participants and it is possible that changes to anti-dementia medication could have occurred during the study. Finally, the study was slightly under-powered, which means that the probability of making Type II errors was somewhat increased. Implications This study points towards benefits in memory, comprehension, and orientation for people with mild to moderate dementia who participate in CST. The clinical significance of these findings in terms of functional benefits is unclear. Possibly, the lack of demonstrated effects on attention, executive function, and praxis may reduce the impact of cognition-focussed interventions like CST on clinical outcomes. Additionally, small statistically significant gains on neuropsychological tests do not necessarily equate with improved performance of daily tasks, although a previous study did find that cognitive improvements mediated improvements in self-rated quality of life for people with dementia (Woods et al., 2006). If improvements in memory, comprehension, and orientation are perceived by people with dementia who take part in CST, this could have a positive impact on self-esteem which may, in turn, enhance mood and quality of life. Future research Future research is needed to further investigate the neuropsychological outcomes and mechanisms of change of CST. The current study could be repeated and refined by using a larger sample to increase power and revising the test battery in order to minimise floor effects. The battery could also be designed to further explore specific hypotheses regarding the mechanisms of change. For example, the language processes that may mediate a perceived improvement in spoken language could be further explored by using a detailed language assessment battery such as the Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination (BDAE-3; Goodglass et al., 2001). Memory processes could be examined by using a measure such as the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT-II; Delis et al.,2000), and executive functioning using the Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (D-KEFS; Delis et al., 2001). Another potentially useful avenue for future research would be a dismantling study to identify the active ingredients of CST. These findings could be used to inform the care of people with dementia more generally in terms of the key aspects of CST being incorporated in day-to-day care and interactions. Finally, it would be interesting to use functional brain imaging to see if any of the neuropsychological changes promoted by CST correlate with changes in the activity of the brain at a neurobiological level. Conflict of interest There was no source of financial support for this work. Aimee Spector and Joshua Stott offer CST training courses on a commercial basis. Description of authors roles Louise Hall conducted this research for her DClinPsy thesis. She collected and analysed the data, and wrote the first draft of the paper. Aimee Spector was the primary supervisor for this research. She has commented and edited several drafts of the paper. Martin Orrell was the secondary supervisor for this research. He has commented on several drafts of the paper. Joshua Stott consulted on the neuropsychological aspects of the research. He has commented on drafts of the paper. Acknowledgments We would like to thank all of the people with dementia and the staff of the day centres and memory clinics who agreed to take part in this study.

Neuropsychological mechanisms of change in CST 489 References American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn, text revision. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association. Breuil, V., De Rotrou, J., Forette, F., Tortrar, D., Ganancia-Ganem, A. and Frambourt, A. (1994). Cognitive stimulation of patients with dementia: preliminary results. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 9, 211 17. Clare, L. (2008). Neuropsychological assessment of the older person. In R. Woods and L. Clare (eds.), Handbook of the Clinical Psychology of Ageing. UK: John Wiley and Sons Ltd. Clare, L. and Woods, R. T. (2004). Cognitive training and cognitive rehabilitation for people with early-stage Alzheimer s disease: a review. Neuropsychological Rehabilitation, 14, 385 401. Clare, L., Wilson, B. A., Carter, G., Breen, K., Gosses, A. and Hodges, J. R. (2000). Intervening with everyday memory problems in dementia of Alzheimer type: an errorless learning approach. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 22, 132 146. Cooper, D. B. et al. (2001) Effects of practice on category fluency in Alzheimer s disease. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15, 125 128. Delis, D. C., Kramer, J. H., Kaplan, E. and Ober, B. A. (2000). California Verbal Learning Test, 2nd edn. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Delis, D. C., Kaplan, E. and Kramer, J. H. (2001). Delis Kaplan Executive Function System. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G. and Buchner, A. (2007). G Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 175 191. Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E. and McHugh, P. R. (1975). Mini-Mental State: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189 198. Goodglass, H., Kaplan, E. and Baresi, B. (2001). Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination, 3rd edn. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins. Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T. and Statham, J. A. (2004). Explicit and implicit stereotype activation effects on memory: do age and awareness moderate the impact of priming? Psychology and Aging, 19, 495 505. Hodges, J. R. (2007). Cognitive Assessment for Clinicians. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Kaplan, E. F., Goodglass, H. and Weintraub, S. (2001). The Boston Naming Test, 2nd edn. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott, Williams and Wilkins. Mohs, R. C. et al. (1997). Development of cognitive instruments for use in clinical trials of antidementia drugs: additions to the Alzheimer s disease assessment scale that broaden its scope. Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders, 11 (Suppl 2), 13 21. Morris, J. C. et al. (1989). The Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer s Disease (CERAD). Part I. Clinical and neuropsychological assessment of Alzheimer s disease. Neurology, 39, 1159 1165. NICE-SCIE (2007). Dementia: Supporting People with Dementia and their Carers in Health and Social Care. NICE Clinical Guideline 42. London: National Institute for Health and Clinical Excellence (www.nice.org.uk). Reitan, R. M. and Wolfson, D. (1992). The Halstead Reitan Neuropsychological Test Battery: Theory and Clinical Interpretation, 2nd edn. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press. Rosen, W. G., Mohs, R. C. and Davis, K. L. (1984). A new rating scale for Alzheimer s disease. American Journal of Psychiatry, 141, 1356 1364. Salthouse, T. A. (2006). Mental exercise and mental aging: evaluating the validity of the use it or lose it hypothesis. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 68 87. Spector, A. et al. (2003). Efficacy of an evidence-based cognitive stimulation therapy programme for people with dementia: Randomised Controlled Trial. British Journal of Psychiatry, 183, 248 254. Spector, A., Thorgrimsen, L., Woods, B. and Orrell, M. (2006). Making a Difference: An Evidence-Based Group Programme to Offer Cognitive Stimulation Therapy (CST) to People with Dementia: Manual for Group Leaders. UK: Hawker Publications. Spector, A., Orrell, M. and Woods, B. (2010). Cognitive Stimulation Therapy (CST): effects on different areas of cognitive function for people with dementia. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 25, 1253 1258. Stern, Y. (2002). What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 448 460. Strauss, E., Sherman, E. M. S. and Spreen, O. (2006). A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests, 3rd edn. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Swaab, D. F., Dubelaar, E. J. G., Hofman, M. A., Scherder, E. J. A., van Someren, E. J. W. and Verwer, R. W. H. (2002). Brain aging and Alzheimer s disease; use it or lose it. Progress in Brain Research, 138, 345 375. Wechsler, D. (1997). Wechsler Memory Scale, 3rd edn. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Wechsler, D. (2001). Wechsler Test of Adult Reading. San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation. Woods, B., Thorgrimsen, L., Spector, A., Royan, L. and Orrell, M. (2006). Improved quality of life and cognitive stimulation therapy in dementia. Aging and Mental Health, 10, 219 226. Woods, B., Aguirre, E., Orrell, M. and Spector, A. (2012). Cognitive stimulation for people with dementia (review). The Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews. Chichester: Wiley.