Age-related quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability

Similar documents
Putative Mechanisms Underlying Risky Decision-Making in High Risk Drivers

Studying social cognition in patients with schizophrenia and patients with frontotemporal dementia: Theory of mind and the perception of sarcasm

Running head: Cognitive decision models in cocaine abusers. Cognitive modeling analysis of the decision-making processes used by cocaine abusers

Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions

Baseline Characteristics of Patients Attending the Memory Clinic Serving the South Shore of Boston

Components of working memory and somatic markers in decision making

Decision-Making in Frontotemporal Dementia: Clinical, Theoretical and Legal Implications

AMERICAN JOURNAL OF PSYCHOLOGICAL RESEARCH

Decision making cognition in primary progressive aphasia

A Contribution of Cognitive Decision Models to Clinical Assessment: Decomposing Performance on the Bechara Gambling Task

Neuropsychologia 51 (2013) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Anorexia, bulimia, and obesity: Shared decision making deficits on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT)

The Role of Orbitofrontal Cortex in Decision Making

Running head: OLFACTORY AND HIGHER CORTICAL FUNCTIONS. Association between Olfactory and Higher Cortical Functions in Alzheimer s disease

Decision-making deficit in chronic migraine patients with medication overuse

Piano playing skills in a patient with frontotemporal dementia: A longitudinal case study

Perceived time pressure and the Iowa Gambling Task

Pharmacologyonline 3: (2010)

Individual differences in decision-making

William W. Hale III, 1 Quinten A. W. Raaijmakers, 1 Anne van Hoof, 2 and Wim H. J. Meeus 1,3. 1. Introduction

Somatic Marker Hypothesis. Tasha Poppa Affective Computing Guest Lecture Fall 2017

Decision-making and addiction (part II): myopia for the future or hypersensitivity to reward?

Right lateral prefrontal cortex Specificity for inhibition or strategy use?

Clinical Study A New Approach for the Quantitative Evaluation of the Clock Drawing Test: Preliminary Results on Subjects with Parkinson s Disease

Risky Decision Making Assessed With the Gambling Task in Adults with HIV

Academic year Lecture 16 Emotions LECTURE 16 EMOTIONS

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Safe models for risky decisions Steingröver, H.M. Link to publication

Evaluating internal consistency in cognitive models for repeated choice tasks

A Comparison of Reinforcement Learning Models for the Iowa Gambling Task Using Parameter Space Partitioning. Abstract

Clinical Study Patient Aesthetic Satisfaction with Timing of Nasal Fracture Manipulation

Neural Basis of Decision Making. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

The Somatic Marker Hypothesis: Human Emotions in Decision-Making

Research Article Prevalence and Trends of Adult Obesity in the US,

Brain Imaging studies in substance abuse. Jody Tanabe, MD University of Colorado Denver

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) Safe models for risky decisions Steingröver, H.M. Link to publication

EARLY ONSET FRONTOTERMPORAL DEMENTIA AND ALZHEIMERS DISEASE: DIAGNOSIS, TREATMENT AND CARE

Research Article Continuous Positive Airway Pressure Device Time to Procurement in a Disadvantaged Population

Screening for Cognitive Dysfunction in Corticobasal Syndrome: Utility of Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination

Right and Left Medial Orbitofrontal Volumes Show an Opposite Relationship to Agreeableness in FTD

Recent publications using the NACC Database. Lilah Besser

Histological Value of Duodenal Biopsies

Neural Basis of Decision Making. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Research Article Self-Monitoring of Blood Pressure in Hypertension: AUKPrimaryCareSurvey

Exploration of social rule violation in patients with focal prefrontal neurosurgical lesions

Effects of decision-phase time constraints on emotion-based learning in the Iowa Gambling Task q

Social and Emotional Decision-making Following Frontal Lobe Injury

Research Article Apathy and Emotion-Based Decision-Making in Amnesic Mild Cognitive Impairment and Alzheimer s Disease

Connect with amygdala (emotional center) Compares expected with actual Compare expected reward/punishment with actual reward/punishment Intuitive

Buy full version here - for $ 7.00

WORKING MEMORY AND INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES IN DECISION MAKING TINA L. JAMESON

Alireza Bakhshaeekia and Sina Ghiasi-hafezi. 1. Introduction. 2. Patients and Methods

Correlation Between Intelligence Test Scores and Executive Function Measures

SOCIABLE DELIVERABLE D7.2a Interim Assessment of the SOCIABLE Platform and Services

Research Article Assessment of Early Cognitive Impairment in Patients with Clinically Isolated Syndromes and Multiple Sclerosis

Resistance to forgetting associated with hippocampus-mediated. reactivation during new learning

Daofang Zhu, Xianming Dou, Liang Tang, Dongdong Tang, Guiyi Liao, Weihua Fang, and Xiansheng Zhang

Under Review: Please do not reproduce or distribute

Sebastião Rodrigues Ferreira-Filho, Camila Caetano Cardoso, Luiz Augusto Vieira de Castro, Ricardo Mendes Oliveira, and Renata Rodrigues Sá

Differentiation of semantic dementia and Alzheimer s disease using the Addenbrooke s Cognitive Examination (ACE)

APNA 25th Annual Conference October 19, Session 1022

Scams: Influencing the Aging Brain

Progressive decline of decision-making performances during multiple sclerosis

Setting The setting was institutional and tertiary care in London, Essex and Hertfordshire in the UK.

Renal Pelvis Squamous Cell Carcinoma and Renal Cell Carcinoma in a Tuberculous Kidney

Storage and processing working memory functions in Alzheimer-type dementia

Research Article Predictions of the Length of Lumbar Puncture Needles

Phineas gauged: decision-making and the human prefrontal cortex

5 Verbal Fluency in Adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome

Research Article Opioid Use Is Not Associated with Incomplete Wireless Capsule Endoscopy for Inpatient or Outpatient Procedures

Fatigue in patients with Parkinson s disease

Fact Sheet Alzheimer s disease

Cognitive and neuroimaging findings in pathological gambling

University of Groningen. Visual hallucinations in Parkinson's disease Meppelink, Anne Marthe

DATA CORE MEETING. Observational studies in dementia and the MELODEM initiative. V. Shane Pankratz, Ph.D.

CHAPTER 5 NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL PROFILE OF ALZHEIMER S DISEASE

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE. Full terms and conditions of use:

An Application of Propensity Modeling: Comparing Unweighted and Weighted Logistic Regression Models for Nonresponse Adjustments

Prevalence of psychosomatic and other medical illnesses in anorexic and bulimic patients 1

Correspondence should be addressed to Taha Numan Yıkılmaz;

On-line Table 1: Dementia diagnoses and related ICD codes for the diagnostic groups a

Mild cognitive impairment A view on grey areas of a grey area diagnosis

Title. CitationAustralasian Journal on Ageing, 31(3): Issue Date Doc URL. Rights. Type. File Information

Frontotemporal Degeneration

NeuRA Decision making April 2016

Neurodegenerative diseases that degrade regions of the brain will eventually become

Sylwia Mizia, 1 Dorota Dera-Joachimiak, 1 Malgorzata Polak, 1 Katarzyna Koscinska, 1 Mariola Sedzimirska, 1 and Andrzej Lange 1, 2. 1.

F rontotemporal dementia (FTD) is the term currently used

Research Article Estimating Measurement Error of the Patient Activation Measure for Respondents with Partially Missing Data

Case Report Evolution of Skin during Rehabilitation for Elephantiasis Using Intensive Treatment

Neuropsychology of reward learning and negative symptoms in schizophrenia

Recreational alcohol and brain. GP Study Day 12 th March 2019 Anya Topiwala

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI)

The Effect of Frontal Lobe Stress on Gambling Task Performance: Implications for. Understanding Addictive Behavior. Jared A. Rowland, M.S.

Research Article The Impact of the Menstrual Cycle on Perioperative Bleeding in Vitreoretinal Surgery

Concurrent validity of WAIS-III short forms in a geriatric sample with suspected dementia: Verbal, performance and full scale IQ scores

Research Article Urinary Catheterization May Not Adversely Impact Quality of Life in Multiple Sclerosis Patients

THE IMPACT OF EXECUTIVE FUNCTIONS AND EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE ON IOWA GAMBLING TASK PERFORMANCE: FOCUS ON RIGHT FRONTAL LOBE DAMAGE

Research Article Hb A1c Separation by High Performance Liquid Chromatography in Hemoglobinopathies

Correspondence should be addressed to Martin J. Bergman;

Transcription:

Behavioural Neurology 19 (2008) 59 63 59 IOS Press Age-related quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability Valeria Isella a,, Cristina Mapelli a, Nadia Morielli a, Oriana Pelati b, Massimo Franceschi b and Ildebrando Marco Appollonio a a Neuropsychology Unit, Neurology Section, S. Gerardo Hospital, Department of Neuroscience, University of Milano Bicocca, Monza, Italy b Neurology Department, IRCCS Multimedica, Castellanza, Italy Abstract. The frontal aging hypothesis predicts that brain senescence affects predominantly the prefrontal regions. Preliminary evidence has recently been gathered in favour of an age-related change in a typically frontal process, i.e. decision making, using the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), but overall findings have been conflicting. Following the traditional scoring method, coupled with a qualitative analysis, in the present study we compared IGT performance of 40 young (mean age: 27.9 ± 4.7) and 40 old (mean age: 65.4 ± 8.6) healthy adults and of 18 patients affected by frontal lobe dementia of mild severity (mean age: 65.1 ± 7.4, mean MMSE score: 24.1 ± 3.9). Quantitative findings support the notion that decision making ability declines with age; moreover, it approximates the impairment observed in executive dysfunction due to neurodegeneration. Results of the qualitative analysis did not reach statistical significance for the motivational and learning decision making components considered, but approached significance for the attentional component for elderly versus young normals, suggesting a possible decrease in the ability to maintain sustained attention during complex and prolonged tasks as the putative deficit underlying impaired decision making in normal aging. Keywords: Decision making, gambling task, aging, frontotemporal dementia 1. Introduction The frontal aging hypothesis [11] claims that brain senescence affects predominantly the frontal lobes. Provided that the ventromedial prefrontal cortex represents the putative neural substrate of decision making ability [4], an impairment in the ability to decide competently might be an important, life-relevant neuropsychological correlate of physiological aging. Recently, decision making competency in healthy elderly has been the subject of several studies [5 7, 12], which used a classical decision making test, the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) [1]. Overall, findings with respect to the effects of aging have been conflicting. Corresponding author: Dr. Valeria Isella, Clinica Neurologica, Ospedale S. Gerardo dei Tintori, Via Pergolesi 33, 20052, Monza (MI), Italy. Tel.: +39 2333676; Fax: +39 2333457; E-mail: valeria. isella@unimib.it. In addition to the traditional scoring methods of the IGT, a qualitative analysis has been devised lately [3], which allows to decompose the cognitive processes underlying the task according to an expectancy-valence learning model of decision making [2]. The analysis considers motivational, memory and attentional components and has been validated in a number of clinical populations [13]. In the present survey we replicated the design of the aforementioned studies, i.e. comparison of performance of young and old normal individuals at the Iowa Gambling Task, while adding the following: 1. a group of patients affected by mild frontotemporal dementia frontal variant, which may be considered as the pathological counterpart of frontal aging, and 2. qualitative analysis of performance at the IGT. Our aim was to try answering to the following questions: are healthy elderly as good as young individuals at making decisions? If not, are they comparable to mild frontal ISSN 0953-4180/08/$17.00 2008 IOS Press and the authors. All rights reserved

60 V. Isella et al. / Age-related quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability dementia patients, i.e. do they present an equal level of impairment and similar changes in decision making heuristic? 2. Subjects and methods Normal participants were 80 community dwelling, healthy individuals with a MiniMental State Examination (MMSE) adjusted score 24; 40 young controls aged 20 to 35 and 40 elderly controls aged 50 or more. Demented participants were 18 subjects selected among outpatients referring to the Neuropsychology Services at S. Gerardo Hospital, in Monza, and Clinica Santa Maria, in Castellanza, Milan, and meeting standardised diagnostic criteria for frontal variant of fronto-temporal dementia [8]. Normals and patients were excluded if they had a current or past history of major psychiatric disorders (including depression), head trauma or alcohol or drug abuse. Only patients with a MMSE adjusted score 18 or a Clinical Dementia Rating score between 0.5 and 2 were included. Six frontal patients out of 18 (33.3%) had a CDR score of 0.5, seven (38.9%) had 1.0 and five (27.8%) had 2.0. The socio-demographic features and MMSE scores of the three study groups are shown in Table 1. As expected, MMSE scores were significantly worse for demented subjects, while they were overlapping for the two control groups. Although schooling was significantly higher for young controls than for the other two groups, we did not covariate for education when analysing experimental data, because educational level appeared to be highly correlated with age (Pearsons r coefficient = 0.60), so that covarying would have masked possible intergroup age-related differences. Besides, the total IGT score was unrelated with education (maximum Pearson s r coefficient = 0.36 obtained in the elderly controls group). The possibility that statistically significant differences might have been biased should anyway be taken into account. All demented subjects underwent a standard assessment including neurological, neuropsychological and neuroimaging examinations. Normal controls underwent the MMSE and a semistructured clinical interview for the verification of exclusion criteria. The manual version of the Iowa Gambling Task was administered by two neuropsychology trainees according to the instructions provided by Authors [1]. Briefly, the subject receives a budget of 4000 (facsimile bills) and is invited to choose one card at a time from four decks (A to D), for 100 trials. The four decks are differently arranged in terms of amount of wins, and frequency and amount of losses, so that A and B are disadvantageous decks (i.e. lead to net financial loss), while C and D are advantageous decks (i.e. lead to net financial gain). The subject must try to win as much money as possible, and, at the same time, to avoid losing money as far as possible. Quantitative IGT parameters consist in the net number of advantageous choices (selections from decks C and D minus selections from decks A and B), computed both for the whole 100 cards (total score) and for five successive blocks of 20 cards each (1 20, 21 40 and so on); this latter parameter is used in order to quantify the progressive change in selection across the task. Qualitative analysis [3] was performed for each participant with the original Matlab algorithm provided online by authors (http://mypage.iu.edu/ jbusemey/ home.html) and considers three parameters, each corresponding to a putative cognitive component of the decision making process. The motivational parameter refers to the relevance attributed to wins; it ranges from 0 to 1 and higher values indicate higher relevance. The learning parameter refers to memory for the outcome of prior choices; it ranges from 0 to 1 and higher values indicate more rapid forgetting. The sensitivity parameter refers to the sensitivity of choices to expectancies progressively formulated during the test and is deemed to depend on the level of concentration maintained throughout the task; it ranges from 5 to +5 and higher values indicate higher levels of concentration. Statistical analysis was performed using SPSS 14.0. Demographic and neuropsychological variables were analysed using chi-square test or one-way ANOVA, as appropriate. Results of the Iowa Gambling Task were also analysed using a repeated measures ANO- VA design, with card blocks [1 5] as within-subjects and group (young controls vs elderly controls vs frontal patients) as between-subjects variables. Besides, each participant was categorized as good or poor performer at the IGT, based on whether he/she made a significantly greater number of advantageous than disadvantageous choices (using the binomial test [9]); the proportion of good and poor performers within each study group was then compared. All tests were thresholded at a significance level of p<0.05. 3. Results The comparison among the three study groups for IGT scores are shown in Table 1.

V. Isella et al. / Age-related quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability 61 Table 1 Sociodemographic and neuropsychological features and gambling task parameters of the three study groups Variables Young controls Elderly controls Frontal dementia F value/x2 n =40 n =40 patients n =18 MEAN (SD) MEAN (SD) MEAN (SD) Age 27.9 (4.7) 65.4 (8.6) 65.1 (7.4) 333.6 Gender (% men) 38 45 56 1.7 Education (years) 13.1 (2.1) 8.7 (3.6) 7.5 (4.5) 26.1 Adjusted MMSE score 29.0 (1.5) 28.9 (0.9) 24.1 (3.9) 44.7 MEAN (SEM) MEAN (SEM) MEAN (SEM) IGT total score 14.2 (3.8) 7.1 (3.1) 2.2 (3.9) 3.9 Poor performers at the IGT (%) 57.5 80 88.9 8.1 IGT qualitative parameters: Motivation 0.47 (0.1) 0.62 (0.1) 0.44 (0.1) 1.9 Learning 0.21 (0.1) 0.36 (0.1) 0.36 (0.1) 2.0 Sensitivity 0.47 (0.3) 0.91 (0.4) 0.68 (0.8) 2.9 p <0.05 vs elderly controls, p<0.05 vs frontal dementia patients. LEGEND: SD = Standard Deviation, SEM = Standard Error of the Mean, MMSE = MiniMental State Examination, CDR = Clinical Dementia Rating, IGT = Iowa Gambling Task. Overall, one-way ANOVA for the total scores was significant and post-hoc tests showed a significantly lower score for demented subjects with respect to young normals and an intermediate score, not significantly different from that of the other two groups, for elderly controls. Repeated measures 3 (group) x 5 (block) ANOVA (Fig. 1) showed significant effects for block (F(4,380) = 7.133, p =0.000), group (F(2,95) = 3.934, p =0.023) and block x group interaction (F(8,380) = 3.198, p =0.002). Post-hoc analysis revealed that demented subjects showed a flat IGT score curve across blocks; young subjects chose significantly more advantageously than elderly subjects at the last block and than frontal patients at the last three blocks; elderly controls chose significantly more advantageously than frontal patients at the fourth block only. Chi-square comparison of the proportion of good and poor performers showed a significantly higher percent of good performers within the young control group with respect to the other two groups, which did not differ from each other. One-way ANOVA comparison of the three qualitative parameters was not significant for the motivational and learning parameters and approached significance for the sensitivity parameter (p =0.056); at post-hoc comparisons, sensitivity was lower for elderly normals with respect to young normals (p =0.021). 4. Discussion In the present study we compared the performance at a gambling task of young normals, elderly normals and patients with frontal neurodegeneration, with the aim to evaluate possible quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability related to frontal aging. Previous literature dealing with the effects of aging on decision making as assessed via the IGT has yielded controversial results. Wood et al. [12] evidenced a different cognitive strategic approach to the task for young and old healthy adults, but found no difference in their overall quantitative performance. MacPherson et al. [7] as well reported no adverse effect of aging on successful completion of the task (although the fact that their young participants performed rather poorly might have been a confounding factor). Conversely, Denburg et al. [5,6] found an age-related decline in decision making ability as assessed with the IGT. Such a decline concerned a substantial minority of their normal seniors and appeared to be unrelated to their sociodemographic, general health or neuropsychological status. Our findings are in line with these latter studies. IGT total scores did not differ significantly among the three study groups, probably due to large variances, but subgroup analysis revealed a significantly major proportion of poor performers among elderly normals and frontal patients. More precisely, the three study groups were substantially overlapping at the beginning of the task; from the second to the fourth block, a progressive increase in the number of advantageous choices took place for both control groups, not for demented patients; in the final block, young normals went on doing profitable selections, while seniors started to make a higher and higher number of disadvantageous choices and dropped down almost to the level of demented patients. As already stated in the methods section, educa-

62 V. Isella et al. / Age-related quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability 6 Choices C+D - choices A+B 4 2 0-2 YOUNG NORMAL CONTROLS ELDERLY NORMAL CONTROLS FRONTO-TEMPORAL PATIENTS -4 Blocks of cards p<0.05 vs young controls, p<0.05 vs elderly controls Fig. 1. Performance of the three study groups at the Iowa Gambling Task, with each block including 20 successive cards. The score is represented by the number of choices from advantageous decks (C and D) minus the number of choices from disadvantageous decks (A and B). A negative score indicates poor decision making. tional level was significantly higher for young than for old participants, but covarying was not carried out because it might have masked age-related discrepancies. Albeit a bias might still have derived from such a difference, we do not deem it sufficiently large to change our conclusions. According to qualitative findings, the elderly controls behaviour did not seem to be due to an alteration of the process of weighing positive feedbacks, neither to a defective learning of expected outcomes associated with the different choice options. Rather, it might have been related to some factor, most likely a progressive loss of concentration, which interfered with the consistent application of cards selection strategy followed by the subject during previous trials, leading to more erratic choices in the final blocks of cards. In fact, healthy seniors presented lower values of the sensitivity parameter with respect to young normals. Such a result, however, approached, but did not reach significance, and thus requires confirmation. The specificity of this attentional component to decision making would also warrant investigation. As to frontal demented individuals, no qualitative parameter was able to explain failure at the IGT, including sensitivity. However, an attentional deficit might still be a possible account for frontal patients performance, albeit not detected by the sensitivity parameter. As a matter of fact, sensitivity is related to a progressive loss of concentration towards the end of the task and would not signal a random pattern of choices possibly set up from the beginning of the test. To the best of our knowledge, there is only one study that used Busemeyer s algorithm in order to examine the qualitative features of performance of old and young normals at the IGT, yielding partially different results compared to the present ones [12]. In Woods et al. s studies two groups of subjects were equally successful at the task. Secondly, they appeared to apply different strategies: young participants placed more emphasis on memory, while a more proper appraisal of positive and negative feedbacks represented seniors strength (differences in the sensitivity parameter could not be evaluated independently from those obtained in the learning parameter). Dissimilarities in participants features possibly affecting the approach to the task, like age and educational level (which were both higher in Wood et al. s elderly sample), or the use of the computerized, rather than the manual, version of the test might perhaps partly account for such discrepancies, but mostly they remain unexplained. Likewise, the present results can be hardly reconciled with those of the one prior study dealing with decision making ability in the frontal variant of fronto-temporal dementia [10], where age-matched controls largely outperformed patients at the IGT. Overall, our findings suggest that healthy seniors undergo a decline in their ability to decide advantageously, performing at the IGT at an intermediate level with respect to young normals and patients with frontal dysfunction due to neurodegenerative pathology. The putative deficit underlying such a decline might involve the capacity to maintain sustained attention during com-

V. Isella et al. / Age-related quantitative and qualitative changes in decision making ability 63 plex and prolonged decision making tasks. The lack of straightforward data in support for this hypothesis and discrepancies with respect to previous literature data, however, prompt further investigation of the issue. References [1] A. Bechara, A.R. Damasio, H. Damasio and S.W. Anderson, Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex, Cognition 50 (1994), 7 15. [2] J.R. Busemeyer and I.J. Myung, An adaptive approach to human decision making: learning theory, decision theory and human performance, Experimental Psychology: general 121 (1992), 177 194. [3] J.R. Busemeyer and J.C. Stout, A contribution of cognitive decision models to clinical assessment: decomposing performance on the Bechara gambling task, Psychological Assessment 14 (2002), 253 262. [4] L. Clark and F. Manes, Social and emotional decision-making following frontal lobe injury, Neurocase 10 (2004), 398 403. [5] N.L. Denburg, E.C. Recknor, A. Bechara and D. Tranel, Psychophysiological anticipation of positive outcomes promotes advantageous decision-making in normal older persons, International Psychophysiology 61 (2006), 19 25. [6] N.L Denburg, D. Tranel and A. Bechara, The ability to decide advantageously declines prematurely in some normal older persons, Neuropsychologia 43 (2005), 1099 1106. [7] S.E. MacPherson, L.H. Phillips and S. Della Sala, Age, executive function and social decision making: a dorsolateral prefrontal theory of cognitive aging, Psychology and Aging 17 (2002), 598 609. [8] D. Neary, J.S. Snowden, L. Gustafson, U. Passant, D. Stuss, S. Black, M. Freedman, A. Kertesz, P.H. Robert, M. Albert, K. Boone, B.L Miller, J. Cummings and D.F. Benson, Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: a consensus on clinical diagnostic criteria, Neurology 51 (1998), 1546 1554. [9] S. Siegel and N.J. Castellan, Nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences, 2nd ed., McGraw Hill, New York, 1988. [10] T. Torralva, C.M. Kipps, J.R. Hodges, L. Clark, T. Bekinschtein, M. Roca, M.L. Calcagno and F. Manes, The relationship between affective decision-making and theory of mind in the frontal variant of fronto-temporal dementia, Neuropsychologia 45 (2007), 342 349. [11] R.L. West, An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive aging, the International Neuropsychological Society 6 (2000), 727 729. [12] S. Wood, J. Busemeyer, A. Koling, K.R. Cox and H. Davis, Older adults as adaptive decision makers: evidence from the Iowa Gambling Task, Psychology and Aging 20 (2005), 220 225. [13] E. Yechiam, J.R. Busemeyer, J.C. Stout and A. Bechara, Using cognitive models to map relations between neuropsychological disorders and human decision making deficits, Psychological Science 16 (2005), 973 978.

MEDIATORS of INFLAMMATION The Scientific World Journal Gastroenterology Research and Practice Diabetes Research International Endocrinology Immunology Research Disease Markers Submit your manuscripts at BioMed Research International PPAR Research Obesity Ophthalmology Evidence-Based Complementary and Alternative Medicine Stem Cells International Oncology Parkinson s Disease Computational and Mathematical Methods in Medicine AIDS Behavioural Neurology Research and Treatment Oxidative Medicine and Cellular Longevity