The moderating impact of temporal separation on the association between intention and physical activity: a meta-analysis

Similar documents
The Theory of Planned Behaviour and dietary patterns: a systematic review and meta-analysis

Problem solving therapy

Traumatic brain injury

Results. NeuRA Treatments for internalised stigma December 2017

Results. NeuRA Family relationships May 2017

Results. NeuRA Hypnosis June 2016

Animal-assisted therapy

Results. NeuRA Mindfulness and acceptance therapies August 2018

Results. NeuRA Worldwide incidence April 2016

Distraction techniques

NeuRA Obsessive-compulsive disorders October 2017

Results. NeuRA Motor dysfunction April 2016

Results. NeuRA Forensic settings April 2016

Method. NeuRA Biofeedback May 2016

Meta-Analysis. Zifei Liu. Biological and Agricultural Engineering

Transcranial Direct-Current Stimulation

NeuRA Sleep disturbance April 2016

Theory of Planned Behavior and Adherence in Chronic Illness: A Meta-Analysis

The Meta on Meta-Analysis. Presented by Endia J. Lindo, Ph.D. University of North Texas

Results. NeuRA Treatments for dual diagnosis August 2016

Canterbury Christ Church University s repository of research outputs.

NeuRA Schizophrenia diagnosis May 2017

Results. NeuRA Herbal medicines August 2016

Results. NeuRA Maternal infections April 2016

The Efficacy of Paroxetine and Placebo in Treating Anxiety and Depression: A Meta-Analysis of Change on the Hamilton Rating Scales

Design and Analysis Plan Quantitative Synthesis of Federally-Funded Teen Pregnancy Prevention Programs HHS Contract #HHSP I 5/2/2016

NeuRA Decision making April 2016

In many healthcare situations, it is common to find

This is a repository copy of Role of affective attitudes and anticipated affective reactions in predicting health behaviors.

Results. NeuRA Essential fatty acids August 2016

Meta-Analysis and Subgroups

Study Data Excluded Reason for Exclusions

What is indirect comparison?

Introduction to Meta-Analysis

The QUOROM Statement: revised recommendations for improving the quality of reports of systematic reviews

Systematic reviews and meta-analyses of observational studies (MOOSE): Checklist.

This is a repository copy of Applying the Reasoned Action approach to understanding health protection and health risk behaviors.

Fixed Effect Combining

C2 Training: August 2010

NeuRA Schizophrenia diagnosis June 2017

How to Conduct a Meta-Analysis

18/11/2013. An Introduction to Meta-analysis. In this session: What is meta-analysis? Some Background Clinical Trials. What questions are addressed?

Keywords: Internet, obesity, web, weight loss. obesity reviews (2010) 11,

Method. NeuRA Cholinesterase inhibitors August 2016

Learning from Systematic Review and Meta analysis

A Short Primer on Power Calculations for Meta-analysis

Impact of attitudes, social norms, perceived behavioural control and intention on risky behaviour on Facebook: A regression analysis

Free Will and Agency: A Scoping Review and Map

Integrating Emotion and the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Consumers Activism in the Internet Web site

The detection and management of pain in patients with dementia in acute care settings: development of a decision tool: Research protocol.

University of Bristol - Explore Bristol Research. Peer reviewed version. Link to published version (if available): /bmsp.

This is an electronic reprint of the original article. This reprint may differ from the original in pagination and typographic detail.

Performance of the Trim and Fill Method in Adjusting for the Publication Bias in Meta-Analysis of Continuous Data

NeuRA Ventricular system August 2016

Meta-Analysis De-Mystified: A Step-by-Step Workshop

Choice of axis, tests for funnel plot asymmetry, and methods to adjust for publication bias

Evaluating the results of a Systematic Review/Meta- Analysis

An Empirical Study on Causal Relationships between Perceived Enjoyment and Perceived Ease of Use

Testing the Efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior to Explain Strength Training in Older Adults

Quality and Reporting Characteristics of Network Meta-analyses: A Scoping Review

Adolescent Regular Moderate-to-Vigorous Physical Activity

Reporting and methods in systematic reviews of comparative accuracy

Evidence based urology in practice: heterogeneity in a systematic review meta-analysis. Health Services Research Unit, University of Aberdeen, UK

A Systematic Review of the Efficacy and Clinical Effectiveness of Group Analysis and Analytic/Dynamic Group Psychotherapy

System and User Characteristics in the Adoption and Use of e-learning Management Systems: A Cross-Age Study

REDUCING FRIDAY ALCOHOL CONSUMPTION AMONG MODERATE, WOMEN DRINKERS: EVALUATION OF A BRIEF EVIDENCE-BASED INTERVENTION

An Empirical Study of the Roles of Affective Variables in User Adoption of Search Engines

Understanding Tourist Environmental Behavior An Application of the Theories on Reasoned Action Approach

Utah Cost of Crime. Mental Health Court (Adults): Technical Report. December 2012

Method. NeuRA Positive symptoms June 2017

PDRF About Propensity Weighting emma in Australia Adam Hodgson & Andrey Ponomarev Ipsos Connect Australia

Correlation and Regression

Empirical assessment of univariate and bivariate meta-analyses for comparing the accuracy of diagnostic tests

1 Hedmark University College, Elverum, Norway. 2 University of Agder, Kristiansand, Norway

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

NeuRA Schizophrenia diagnosis October 2017

EFFECT OF IMPLEMENTATION INTENTIONS TO CHANGE BEHAVIOUR: MODERATION BY INTENTION STABILITY 1

Supplementary materials

Intention to consent to living organ donation: an exploratory study. Christina Browne B.A. and Deirdre M. Desmond PhD

Robert M. Jacobson, M.D. Department of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine Mayo Clinic, Rochester, Minnesota

Systematic Reviews and Meta- Analysis in Kidney Transplantation

Critical appraisal: Systematic Review & Meta-analysis

Research Synthesis and meta-analysis: themes. Graham A. Colditz, MD, DrPH Method Tuuli, MD, MPH

Meta-Analysis of the Theories of Reasoned Action and Planned Behavior 1

Meta-analysis in sport and exercise research: Review, recent developments, and recommendations

How to interpret results of metaanalysis

n The ACA Online Library is a member s only benefit. You can join today via the web: counseling.org and via the phone: x222.

P S. Synthesizer 1.0: A varying-coefficient meta-analytic tool ZLATAN KRIZAN

Feng-Yi Lai, RN, MSN, Instructor Department of Nursing, Shu-Zen College of Medicine and Management, Asphodel Yang, RN, PhD, Associate Professor

Using the construct of perceived autonomy support to understand social influence within the theory of planned behavior

Expectation-Confirmation Model of Information System Continuance: A Meta-Analysis

Downloaded from:

Objectives. Search strategy

This is a repository copy of The temporal stability and predictive validity of affect-based and cognition-based intentions.

Arthur Sone-Wai Li, B. Soc. Sc. A report submitted as a partial requirement for the degree of. Division of Psychology, School of Medicine

Developing and Testing Hypotheses Kuba Glazek, Ph.D. Methodology Expert National Center for Academic and Dissertation Excellence Los Angeles

Meta-Analysis and Publication Bias: How Well Does the FAT-PET-PEESE Procedure Work?

Influences of social power and normative support on condom use decisions: a research synthesis

NeuRA Essential fatty acids August 2016

Method. NeuRA Negative symptoms April 2016

Transcription:

PSYCHOLOGY, HEALTH & MEDICINE, 2016 VOL. 21, NO. 5, 625 631 http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13548506.2015.1080371 The moderating impact of temporal separation on the association between intention and physical activity: a meta-analysis Máirtín S. McDermott a, Rajeev Sharma a, Megan Andrews b, Shahriar Akter c, Donald Iverson d, Peter Caputi b, Tim Coltman c and Murad Safadi a a School of Computing and Information Technology, Faculty of Engineering and Information Sciences, University of Wollongong, Northfields Avenue, Wollongong, New South Wales 2522, Australia; b School of Psychology, Faculty of Social Sciences, University of Wollongong, Northfields Avenue, Wollongong, New South Wales 2522, Australia; c School of Management, Operations and Marketing, Faculty of Business, University of Wollongong, Northfields Avenue, Wollongong, New South Wales 2522, Australia; d Faculty of Health, Arts and Design, Advanced Manufacturing and Design Centre, 453-477 Burwood Road Hawthorn, 3122 Victoria, Australia ABSTRACT Previous meta-analyses have estimated that the intention behaviour association in physical activity (PA) is large in magnitude. However, these prior meta-analyses have also revealed a large degree of heterogeneity, suggesting the presence of moderating variables. This study examines the impact of one such moderator, testing the hypothesis that the magnitude of the association between intention and behaviour decreases as the temporal separation between the two increases. A systematic literature search was used to identify published and unpublished studies that met the inclusion criteria. A random-effects meta-regression was conducted to test the study hypothesis. A total of 78 journal articles and 11 unpublished dissertations were identified, yielding 109 effect sizes. The mean number of weeks between the measurement of intention and behaviour was 5.4 (SD = 6.6, range =.43, 26). The average correlation between intention and behaviour was r = 0.51. In line with theoretical predictions, temporal separation was a significant moderator of the intention behaviour correlation (B =.014, p <.001) and explained 24% of the between-study variance. This result remained unchanged when entered simultaneously with several control variables. The results of this analysis have important implications both for researchers and for intervention designers aiming to increase rates of PA. ARTICLE HISTORY Received 21 April 2015 Accepted 3 August 2015 KEYWORDS Intention; behaviour; physical activity; temporal separation; meta-analysis Introduction Increasing the intention to act is an important aim of many behaviour change interventions, such as those aiming to increase levels of physical activity (PA). This is due to intention CONTACT Máirtín S. McDermott mairtin@uow.edu.au Supplemental data for this article can be accessed at http://dx.doi.10.1080/13548506.2015.1080371. 2015 Taylor & Francis

626 M. S. MCDERMOTT ET AL. being viewed as the variable most proximal to behaviour by several commonly used models. Previous meta-analyses have estimated the intention behaviour (I-B) association to be between r =.45 and.51 (Hagger, Chatzisarantis, & Biddle, 2002; McEachan, Conner, Taylor, & Lawton, 2011). However, those meta-analyses have also revealed a large degree of heterogeneity, suggesting that the I-B association needs to be examined for the presence of moderating variables. The period of temporal separation between the measurement of intention, typically assessed using mixed measures of participants self-ratings of intention, expectation and desire (Armitage & Conner, 2001), and the measurement of behaviour, also typically assessed using self-reports (McEachan et al., 2011), has been argued to be an important moderator of the I-B relationship in PA (Downs & Hausenblas, 2005; McEachan et al., 2011). Increasing periods of temporal separation are considered likely to attenuate the strength of the I-B association as with longer time periods an individual will have more opportunities to be exposed to new experiences and information that might lead to a change in intention (Ajzen & Madden, 1986). This may be a particularly critical issue for PA, as the health benefits of exercise accrue only after individuals have engaged in that behaviour regularly over a sustained period of time. Prior research and its limitations Two previous meta-analyses have examined the moderating effect of temporal separation on the I-B association in PA (Downs & Hausenblas, 2005; McEachan et al., 2011). Although these studies produced findings that were largely consistent with theoretical predictions, both are limited from both a methodological and practical standpoint. From a methodological perspective, the studies have two key limitations. First, neither study employed control variables whilst analysing the effect of temporal separation. Second, both studies employed subgroup analysis ( one week, one week to one month, one month to one year and 5 weeks, >5 weeks, respectively) to investigate differences in the magnitude of I-B associations across potentially arbitrary, researcher-defined categories of temporal separation. This latter point is also limiting from a practical standpoint, as it prevents these studies from providing a reliable estimate of the magnitude of the moderating effect, i.e. an estimate of the rate at which the predictive validity of intention falls over time. The current meta-analysis, therefore, aims to build on prior research by addressing the above limitations. Specifically, by employing random-effects meta-regression to test the impact of temporal separation defined as a continuous variable, whilst controlling for the effects of variables found previously to moderate the I-B association in PA: participant age, publication type and the methods used to measure behaviour (Downs & Hausenblas, 2005; Hagger et al., 2002; McEachan et al., 2011). Methods Selection criteria and study identification The preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses (PRISMA (Moher, Liberati, Tetzlaff, & Altman, 2009)) guidelines were followed throughout the design and conduct of this systematic review. We included studies published in English where the target

PSYCHOLOGY, HEALTH & MEDICINE 627 population was adults aged 18 65, without any current or former medical conditions; the target behaviour was operationalised as physical activity or exercise; and where reported outcomes included bivariate correlations between intention and PA measured at a subsequent time point. Studies in which participants received an intervention were excluded. We searched PsycINFO, MEDLINE, Web of Science, CINAHL, and ProQuest Dissertations and Theses. We also scanned the reference lists of previous meta-analyses incorporating data on the I-B relationship in PA. All titles and abstracts from the formal electronic searches were pre-screened by one review author (MSMcD) for possible inclusion. Those selected were then subject to full-text assessment by the same author, with the accuracy of inclusions checked by a second (MA). Data extraction We extracted correlation data, sample size and the period of temporal separation between intention and behaviour (in weeks). In line with previous reviews (Downs & Hausenblas, 2005; Hagger et al., 2002), we coded the age of participants as either 18 25 or 26 65 and publication type as either a journal article or dissertation. To control for risk of bias in included studies, we coded each correlation for the method used to measure behaviour. Following Sharma, Yetton, and Crawford (2009), a four-point ordinal scale ordered as archival (1), behaviourally continuous (2), behaviourally anchored (3) or perceptually anchored (4) was employed. Data analysis Calculation of the pooled mean effect size (r) was conducted using inverse-variance weighted random-effects meta-analysis (Borenstein, Hedges, Higgins, & Rothstein, 2009). We also estimated the heterogeneity across studies, using both the Q and I 2 statistics (Higgins, Thompson, Deeks, & Altman, 2003). To test for the moderating impact of temporal separation, we employed the protocol for random-effects meta-regression recommended by Borenstein et al. (2009). Visual inspection of scatterplots and examination of Cook s distance (Field, 2005) for each included effect size were used to identify outliers. Based on this analysis, one study was excluded. The remaining effect sizes had a maximum temporal separation of 26 weeks. All analyses were performed using Comprehensive Meta-Analysis Version 3.0 (Borenstein et al., 2014). Results The electronic search strategy retrieved 8517 unique records. A further 49 were identified through screening the reference lists of related meta-analyses. In total, 77 journal articles and 11 dissertations met the inclusion criteria. Details of the screening process can be seen in Figure 1. Data from seven studies were reported in more than one article. Relevant information was extracted from either as appropriate. Three articles reported data from two studies. Eighty-four studies were therefore included yielding 109 effect sizes between PA intention and behaviour with a total n = 31,870. A list of articles included in the meta-analysis is

628 M. S. MCDERMOTT ET AL. Identification PsycINFO 2720 titles MEDLINE 3091 titles Web of Science 4424 titles CINAHL 1766 titles ProQuest 1217 titles Screening metaanalyses 49 titles Records after duplicates removed (n =8517) Screening Records screened (n = 8566) Records excluded (n = 7974) Eligibility Included Full-text titles retrieved for in-depth examination (n = 592) Titles included in metaanalysis (n = 88) Full-text titles excluded, (n = 504) Not healthy sample (n=107) No appropriate I-B metric reported (n=105) Participants aged <18 years (n=87) No prospective I-B association (n=77) Wrong behaviour (n=28) No assessment of PA behaviour (n=25) Participants aged >65 years (n=23) No assessment of intention (n=18) Received intervention (n=14) Not published in English (n=8) Could not locate (n=6) Insufficient item-level detail (n=3) Could not determine period of temporal separation (n=2) Excluded as an outlier (n=1) Figure 1. PRISMA flow chart. Table 1. Characteristics of included studies (n = 84). Participants % (n) University students 67.9 (57) Company employees 11.9 (10) Recruited from the community 7.1 (6) Other 7.1 (6) Random sampling 5.6 (5) Theoretical framework Theory of planned behaviour 77.4 (65) Multiple theories 14.3 (12) Health action process approach 4.8 (4) Other theories 3.6 (3) Temporal separation (mean (SD), range) 5.4 (6.6),.43 26 Sample size (mean (SD), range) 292.4 (271.7), 22 1582 available in Supplementary File 1. An overview of study characteristics is presented in Table 1. The random-effects meta-analysis showed that the average correlation between intention and behaviour was r =.51 (95% CI:.48,.54). Examination of the Q-statistic (Q = 1473.69,

PSYCHOLOGY, HEALTH & MEDICINE 629 Table 2. Meta-regression of the impact of temporal separation on the I-B association in PA with covariates. Variable Coefficient Standard error Z-value p-value 95% confidence interval Temporal separation (weeks).013.003 3.93.0001.020.007 Behaviour method type.125.034 3.68.0002.059.192 Publication type 0.070.060 1.18.2393.187.047 Participant age.003.002 1.14.2534.007.002 Intercept.459.120 3.83.0001.224.694 Notes: Temporal separation was entered in weeks. Behaviour method type was coded as system captured (1), behaviourally continuous (2), behaviourally anchored (3) or perceptually anchored (4). Multiple codings within measures were possible due to variation in method type across items. Publication type was coded either as a peer-reviewed journal article (1) or unpublished dissertation (2). Participant age was coded as 18 25 (1) or 26 65 (2). p <.001) and I 2 (92.67%) indicated significant heterogeneity, supporting the use of meta-regression to explore moderators such as temporal separation. The results of the meta-regression show that temporal separation in weeks was a significant moderator of the I-B correlation (B =.014, p <.001) explaining 24% of the betweenstudy variance in the I-B relationship. A breakdown of average correlations for each period of temporal separation can be seen in Supplementary File 2. This result remained unchanged when temporal separation was entered simultaneously with control variables (B =.013 [95% CI:.020,.007] p <.001) (see Table 2). Together, these variables accounted for 36% of the between-study variance in the I-B association. Discussion Identifying the precise nature of the relationship between behaviour and cognitive antecedents such as intention is of clear importance to those aiming to develop effective interventions for increasing rates of PA. The results of the current meta-analysis represent the most robust evidence to date in support of one of the key assumptions of the I-B relationship, specifically that the correlation between intention and behaviour decreases as the period of temporal separation between intention and behaviour increases. To our knowledge, the current meta-analysis is also the first to provide an estimate of the magnitude of the moderating effect of temporal separation. This study has some strengths and limitations. A comprehensive search strategy was employed to identify both published and unpublished studies that met the inclusion criteria. In addition, temporal separation was operationalized as a continuous variable, allowing for a more reliable determination of its moderating impact compared with those studies employing potentially arbitrary categories of time intervals. Finally, a robust examination of the impact of temporal separation was conducted whilst controlling for methodological, participant and study-level control variables. Against this, it is worth noting that despite controlling for key covariates, and the inclusion of a sample of studies that were largely homogenous in terms of participants and definitions of intention and behaviour, reported heterogeneity remained high (Q = 897.42, p <.001, I 2 = 88.63%) suggesting the presence of other moderators not accounted for in this analysis. Furthermore, the included studies restrict our ability to track how the I-B association develops beyond six months. We identified only one effect size where the period of temporal separation exceeded this time point, and this was excluded from the main analysis

630 M. S. MCDERMOTT ET AL. after being identified as an outlier. Based on the current analysis, however, which found that the I-B correlation decreased on average by.013 each week over the 26 weeks from the intercept (i.e. 0 weeks) of r =.459 (see Table 2), it seems unlikely that intention would continue to predict behaviour to a meaningful degree beyond this point. This is important since it takes time for the health benefits of PA to accrue. Each of these limitations represents a matter for further empirical research. In conclusion, the findings of this meta-analysis suggest that although the overall I-B association in PA is moderate, it is significantly moderated by temporal separation to the extent that predicted levels of the association are negligible at six months. Further research is required to determine the persistence of the association beyond this time point. Although the current analysis controlled for different potential sources of bias, reported heterogeneity remained high which indicates the presence of moderators not controlled for here. Metaanalyses, incorporating a broader range of moderators, may be required to gain a more complete understanding of the progression of the I-B relationship in PA over time. The implications of this meta-analysis are threefold: (a) to maximize predictive capacity of the I-B relationship, researchers should focus on predicting PA behaviour in the short term; (b) it might be prudent for those planning interventions to incorporate multiple top ups of intervention elements targeting intentions over time. This latter point is further supported by findings that a similar pattern appears to emerge in experimental studies (Webb & Sheeran, 2006), and (c) given the extent to which the magnitude of the I-B association decreases over time, the utility of intention as a predictor of meaningful levels of PA appears questionable. Disclosure statement No potential conflict of interest was reported by the authors. Funding This work was supported by Australian Research Council [grant number DP130100068]. ORCID McDermott Máirtín S. http://orcid.org/0000-0002-2640-3161 References Ajzen, I., & Madden, T. J. (1986). Prediction of goal-directed behavior: Attitudes, intentions, and perceived behavioral control. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 22, 453 474. Armitage, C. J., & Conner, M. (2001). Efficacy of the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analytic review. British Journal of Social Psychology, 40, 471 499. Borenstein, M., Hedges, L., Higgins, J., & Rothstein, H. (2014). Comprehensive meta-analysis. Version 3. Englewood, NJ: Biostat. Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T., & Rothstein, H. R. (2009). Introduction to meta-analysis. Chichester: Wiley. Downs, S. D., & Hausenblas, H. A. (2005). The theories of reasoned action and planned behaviour applied to exercise: A meta-analytic update. Journal of Physical Activity and Health, 2, 76 97. Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using SPSS (2nd ed.). London: Sage.

PSYCHOLOGY, HEALTH & MEDICINE 631 Hagger, M. S., Chatzisarantis, N. L. D., & Biddle, S. J. H. (2002). A meta-analytic review of the theories of reasoned action and planned behavior in physical activity: Predictive validity and the contribution of additional variables. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 24, 3 32. Higgins, J. P., Thompson, S. G., Deeks, J. J., & Altman, D. G. (2003). Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. British Medical Journal, 327, 557 560. McEachan, R. R. C., Conner, M., Taylor, N. J., & Lawton, R. J. (2011). Prospective prediction of health-related behaviours with the theory of planned behaviour: A meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review, 5, 97 144. Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., & Altman, D. G. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: The PRISMA statement. PLoS Medicine, 6, e1000097. Sharma, R., Yetton, P., & Crawford, J. (2009). Estimating the effect of common method variance: The method-method pair technique with an illustration from TAM research. MIS Quarterly, 33, 473 490. Webb, T. L., & Sheeran, P. (2006). Does changing behavioral intentions engender behavior change? A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 249 268.