Material hardship alters the diurnal rhythm of salivary cortisol

Similar documents
What Current Research Says About Measuring Cortisol and the HPA axis

Salivary Cortisol, Cortisol Awakening Response, and DHEA

There are marked social differences in coronary heart disease

No morning cortisol response in patients with severe global amnesia

Life-time socio-economic position and cortisol patterns in mid-life

Long Term Need for Recovery

PROLONGED PERCEIVED STRESS AND SALIVA CORTISOL IN A LARGE COHORT OF DANISH PUBLIC SERVICE EMPLOYEES: CROSS-SECTIONAL AND LONGITUDINAL ASSOCIATIONS

Adrenocortex Stress Response with Cortisol Awakening Response Interpretive Guide

From Stages to Patterns: Science-Based HPA Axis Assessment

PEER REVIEW HISTORY ARTICLE DETAILS TITLE (PROVISIONAL)

ANTH 260 Introduction to Physical Anthropology LAB

Individual differences in the diurnal cycle of salivary free cortisol: a replication of flattened cycles for some individuals

Modeling Cortisol Daily Rhythms of Family Caregivers of Individuals With Dementia: Daily Stressors and Adult Day Services Use

Developmental Histories of Perceived Racial/Ethnic Discrimination and Diurnal Cortisol Profiles in Adulthood: A 20-year Prospective Study

LMMG New Medicine Recommendation

Posttraumatic Stress Symptoms Related to Community Violence and Children s Diurnal Cortisol Response in an Urban Community-Dwelling Sample

The Cortisol Awakening Response and Autonomic Nervous System Activity During Nocturnal and Early Morning Periods

Psychobiological Processes: Pathways Linking Social Factors with Disease

Maternal Cortisol Levels and Behavior Problems in Adolescents and Adults with ASD

Child Psychiatry & Human Development ISSN X. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev DOI /s

Stress and the SES-Health Gradient: Getting Under the Skin. While the SES gradient in health is well established in many contexts, the physical

Socioeconomic Status and Health: A Role for Immune System Mechanisms?

There is growing interest in understanding the biological

Diurnal cortisol pattern of shift workers on a workday and a day off

SOCIOECONOMIC POSITION, STRESS, AND CORTISOL IN RELATION TO WAIST CIRCUMFERENCE IN AFRICAN AMERICAN AND WHITE WOMEN

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Title: Life satisfaction moderates the impact of socioeconomic status on diurnal cortisol slope

Personality and physiological reactions to acute psychological stress in a large cohort of middle aged men and women

Cumulative Effect of Depressive Symptoms Predicts Diurnal Cortisol Pattern in the Oldest-Old

Cortisol (Sheep) ELISA Kit

Relationship of nighttime arousals and nocturnal cortisol in IBS and normal subjects. Miranda Bradford. A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment

Epidemiology of Chronic Fatigue Syndrome (CFS)

Dyadic Study of Cancer Patients & Caregivers: Depressive Symptoms and Stress Biomarkers

Original Paper. The Dehydroepiandrosterone Awakening Response as a Possible Index of Subjective Sleep Quality

Rhythm Plus- Comprehensive Female Hormone Profile

Social Network Cultivation and Diurnal Cortisol Profiles in Healthy Chinese Elders

Social and Emotional Influences on Physiological Stress in Infants, Children and Adolescents

Latent state trait modeling of children s cortisol at two points of the diurnal cycle

STATISTICS & PROBABILITY

Brief Report: The Temporal Relationships Between Sleep, Cortisol, and Lung Functioning in Youth with Asthma

The HPA-axis and immune function in burnout Mommersteeg, Paula; Heijnen, C.J.; Kavelaars, A.; van Doornen, L.J.P.

Chronotype and cortisol awakening response (CAR). The influence of the chronotype on the awakening response of cortisol in the morning

Social determinants, health and healthcare outcomes 2017 Intermountain Healthcare Annual Research Meeting

Research linking workplace factors with cardiovascular disease

The Effect of Patient Neighbourhood Income Level on the Purchase of Continuous Positive Airway

Application of Local Control Strategy in analyses of the effects of Radon on Lung Cancer Mortality for 2,881 US Counties

Day-to-day dynamics of experience cortisol associations in a population-based sample of older adults

Relationships between Self-Rating of Recovery from Work and Morning Salivary Cortisol

Diurnal Cortisol Rhythms: Social Determinants and Role as a Risk, State or Scar Marker for Major Depressive Disorder in Youth

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Effects of a Mindfulness Based Intervention on Diurnal Cortisol in Cancer Survivors

Salivary Cortisol and Binge Eating Disorder in Obese Women After Surgery for Morbid Obesity


INSECURITY. Food. Though analyses at the regional and national levels

Self-esteem, cortisol reactivity, and depressed mood mediated by perceptions of control

ABSTRACT PARENTAL HOSTILITY AND PARENT STRESS PHYSIOLOGY: THE MODERATING ROLE OF CHILD EFFORTFUL CONTROL

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Economics of obesity

Statistics and Probability

Diurnal Pattern of Reaction Time: Statistical analysis

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Eat Weight Disord. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 January 23.

STUDIES show age-related changes in the pattern of. Diurnal Cortisol and Functional Outcomes in Post-Acute Rehabilitation Patients.

INTERNAL VALIDITY, BIAS AND CONFOUNDING

Relationship between body mass index and length of hospital stay for gallbladder disease

Early-life adversity and cortisol response to social stress: a meta-analysis

Prevalence of abandoned homes in the neighborhood and thymic function

Adrenocortex Stress Profile with Cortisol Awakening Response Support Guide

Multiple Linear Regression (Dummy Variable Treatment) CIVL 7012/8012

Sarah Y. Liu. A Thesis. The Department. of Psychology. Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Sarah Yu-Tsu Liu, 2014

bivariate analysis: The statistical analysis of the relationship between two variables.

A Comparison of Saliva & Wet Urine for Hormone Measurements

Socioeconomic status risk factors for cardiovascular diseases by sex in Korean adults

Adjusting for mode of administration effect in surveys using mailed questionnaire and telephone interview data

Chapter 1: Exploring Data

Chapter 14: More Powerful Statistical Methods

Giving Up on Unattainable Goals: Benefits for Health?

Changes in Number of Cigarettes Smoked per Day: Cross-Sectional and Birth Cohort Analyses Using NHIS

The Effects of Maternal Alcohol Use and Smoking on Children s Mental Health: Evidence from the National Longitudinal Survey of Children and Youth

The Impact of Relative Standards on the Propensity to Disclose. Alessandro Acquisti, Leslie K. John, George Loewenstein WEB APPENDIX

Awakening cortisol responses are influenced by health status and awakening time but not by menstrual cycle phase

Comprehensive assessments of neuroendocrine status. Salivary Sex Steroid Hormones and Adrenal/HPA Axis Function

Social Conflict and Cortisol Regulation

TEST REPORT # U. Patient Name: Sleep Balance Patient Phone Number: TEST NAME RESULTS 07/30/18 RANGE

Patterns of adolescent smoking initiation rates by ethnicity and sex

SCATTER PLOTS AND TREND LINES

Cortisol circadian rhythms and stress responses in infants at risk of allergic disease

BIOPSYCHOSOCIAL FACTORS: WITH AN EMPHASIS ON PSYCHOSOCIAL

DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS ARE ASSOCIATED WITH HIGHER LEVELS OF PEAK PLASMA GLUCOSE CONCENTRATIONS IN HISPANICS WITH METABOLIC SYNDROME.

Linkages Between Employment Patterns and Depression Over Time: The Case of Low-Income Rural Mothers

Longitudinal Relationships between Self efficacy, Post traumatic Distress and Salivary Cortisol among Motor Vehicle Accident Survivors

County-Level Small Area Estimation using the National Health Interview Survey (NHIS) and the Behavioral Risk Factor Surveillance System (BRFSS)

Is socioeconomic position related to the prevalence of metabolic syndrome? Influence of

In Search of a Salient Marker of Immune Function for Population Health Research

Determination of Appropriate Sampling Time for Job Stress Assessment: the Salivary Chromogranin A and Cortisol in Adult Females

Cortisol (serum, plasma)

THE EFFECT OF THE ODOR OF ISOVALERIC ACID AND JAPANESE CYPRESS ON HUMAN IMMUNE AND ENDOCRINE SECRETION A PRERIMINALY STUDY 1.

Households: the missing level of analysis in multilevel epidemiological studies- the case for multiple membership models

NHS Diabetes Prevention Programme (NHS DPP) Non-diabetic hyperglycaemia. Produced by: National Cardiovascular Intelligence Network (NCVIN)

Stress: The Good, Bad, and the Ugly Part One. Catherine Nelson, Ph.D. University of Utah

Impacts of Early Exposure to Work on Smoking Initiation Among Adolescents and Older Adults: the ADD Health Survey. David J.

Transcription:

Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Epidemiological Association International Journal of Epidemiology 2005;34:1138 1143 The Author 2005; all rights reserved. Advance Access publication 10 June 2005 doi:10.1093/ije/dyi120 OTHER ORIGINAL PAPERS Material hardship alters the diurnal rhythm of salivary cortisol Nalini Ranjit, 1 * Elizabeth A Young 2 and George A Kaplan 3 Accepted 18 May 2005 Background In the quest for biological mechanisms underlying socioeconomic differences in health outcomes, attention has turned to the role of the hypothalamicpituitary-adrenocortical axis. As there is some evidence that both acute and chronic stress raise cortisol levels, and material hardship is a stressor, we examined the relationship of chronic material hardship with salivary cortisol levels over the day. Methods Results The data are from a survey of a sample of poor women aged 18 54. Up to four repeated measures of salivary cortisol levels were obtained from 188 women in this sample and modelled as a diurnal profile. Self-reports of a variety of sources of material hardship over the preceding year were combined into a single scale. Specific dimensions of the subjects cortisol profiles were compared across levels of material hardship. Salivary cortisol varied over the day, and by level of reported material hardship. Upon awakening, salivary cortisol levels were comparable across hardship levels. But soon after waking, women at low levels of hardship experienced both a significantly sharper morning surge and subsequently a sharper decline in salivary cortisol (16.0 and 9.5 nmol/l/h) than women with high hardship levels (5.9 and 4.3 nmol/l/h). These differences in cortisol diurnal pattern tended to be related in a dose-response way to levels of material hardship. Conclusions Material hardship among poor women is associated with changes in the diurnal rhythms of cortisol, particularly in the waking response, which is blunted in women with high levels of hardship. Keywords Cortisol rhythm, chronic stress, material hardship, regression splines, awakening cortisol response, socioeconomic Discussions of associations between adverse material conditions and a variety of health outcomes have, in recent years, turned to the biological pathways underlying these associations. This emphasis on biological pathways brings epidemiologists closer to understanding how social structural conditions influence health, 1 while at the same time addressing concerns about confounding and reverse causation that may underlie findings of positive associations between material/psychosocial 1 Center for Social Epidemiology & Population Health, Department of Epidemiology, School of Public Health, University of Michigan, MI, USA. 2 Department of Psychiatry and Mental Health Research Institute, University of Michigan, MI, USA. 3 Center for Social Epidemiology & Population Health, Department of Epidemiology, School of Public Health, University of Michigan, MI, USA. * Corresponding author. E-mail: nranjit@umich.edu conditions and ill health. 2 One widely invoked formalization of a set of biological pathways is the allostatic load hypothesis, 3 which draws links between a stressful environment and physical disease via the physiological sequelae of repeated activation of the neuro-endocrine system. Of particular interest in this formulation is the role of stress in modulating the release of cortisol. Cortisol is diverse in its physiologic effects, 4 and glucocorticoid receptors are found in the cells of almost all tissues in the body. A wide array of cardiovascular, metabolic, immunologic, and homeostatic functions are regulated by cortisol. Thus, excessive glucocorticoid action is believed to have a role in the development of insulin resistance, 5 and is associated with other cardiovascular risk factors such as central obesity 6 and hypertension. 7 Prolonged exposure to cortisol has been postulated to result in a reduction of cortisol s ability to inhibit the action of pro-inflammatory cytokines. 8 Studies have 1138

DIURNAL RHYTHM OF SALIVARY CORTISOL 1139 consistently demonstrated enhanced cortisol and corticotropin levels in the peripheral blood of drug-free depressives. 9 Alterations of HPA-axis functioning have also been demonstrated in persons with post-traumatic stress disorder. 10 In view of this wide array of effects, it is possible that cortisol is part of the pathway between social conditions and health outcomes. In this study, we explore the consequences of chronic economic stress, operationalized as material hardship, for HPA activity, measured by levels of salivary cortisol over the course of a day. While it has been consistently demonstrated that acute stress raises the level of cortisol, 11,12 findings on the effect of chronic stress on cortisol profiles are mixed, as are findings related to chronic economic stress. Lower cortisol concentrations have been observed among women of lower-grade socioeconomic status (SES), 13 but among low SES women, high job-demands are associated with higher cortisol levels. 14 Another study has shown a positive correlation of cortisol levels with perceived stress, but lowered cortisol secretion among subjects with high levels of burnout. 15 A recent review summarizes a series of related findings suggesting that a variety of chronic stress-related bodily disorders, including burnout, chronic fatigue syndrome, and chronic pelvic pain are associated with lowered, rather than elevated cortisol. 16 Another review, focusing on children experiencing early adversity, suggests that such hypocortisolism may be limited to awakening levels of cortisol. 17 The current analysis extends this literature in two ways. First, we use a comprehensive measure of economic stress, material hardship, that allows us to examine gradations in the cortisol response to stress. Second, we use new approaches to summarize the diurnal cortisol profile that allow us to compare specific features of the cortisol response within women and across levels of stress. Materials and methods The participants in this study are drawn from the longitudinal Women s Employment study (WES), based on a random sample of single mothers, about 50% black and 50% white, aged 18 54, with children receiving cash benefits as of February 1997, in an urban county in Michigan in February 1997. In June 2000, a special health supplement was administered to a random sub-sample (n = 298) of participants in the third wave of the WES survey. Cortisol measures Up to four salivary cortisol samples were obtained from each respondent, using Salivettes. The first measure was obtained during a clinic or home visit, where the respondent was provided with three labelled collection tubes and directions for obtaining subsequent salivary samples at home. Respondents were instructed to collect all three samples on one day, with the first sample collected upon awakening, the second 30 min later and before eating or brushing their teeth, and the third at bedtime. They were instructed to record the time of sample collection for each sample and to delay breakfast until after the second sample was collected. The collection protocol was designed to capture, with as few measures as possible, critical aspects of the diurnal variation in cortisol, including the morning awakening increase and sharp decline, as well as the more gradual decline to the evening nadir. Samples were returned by mail and frozen at 20 C until assay, at which time they were thawed and spun prior to assay. They were assayed using DPC Coat-a-Count tubes using 200 ml of saliva per tube following manufacturer s directions. Inter-assay variability was 10%. Material hardship measures Our measure of chronic economic stress in this population was operationalized as material hardship. The material hardship scale comprised a series of eight questions asking whether the participant had each experienced a number of economic hardships in the 12 months preceding the survey, covering a range of life activities. Items were binary responses to questions on housing, such as evictions and homelessness; questions on daily living, such as gas or electricity shut-offs, availability of phone facilities and sharing of household expenses; and questions on affordability of basic health resources, such as doctor visits, and winter clothing. These items are similar to those used in previous studies of poor populations. 18,19 Measures of material hardship are widely used to examine the well-being of low-income families, and are considered preferable to traditional income-based poverty measures, in that they employ direct indicators of consumption and physical living conditions. 20 A composite score, ranging from 0 to 8, and comprising the sum of the eight items, all equally weighted, was used to characterize the overall level of material hardship. Statistical analysis Analyses presented in this paper are restricted to the 188 respondents (62.9% of the original sample) for whom at least one valid cortisol measure was obtained, where a valid measure is defined as one that includes both a value for cortisol as well as the time of collection. Graphical visualizations of the cortisol profile were done using loess-based smoothers to minimize noise (Figure 1). The graphs provided a basis for fixing the inflection points as knots for regression spline models. Random effects models with linear splines were used to estimate and profile the entire pattern of cortisol variation over the day. The advantages of using these methods for modelling cortisol rhythms have been described elsewhere. 21 Such models account for the correlation of measures within respondents, can accommodate varying numbers of measures per respondent, and exploit the considerable variability in collection times observed in such data. Respondents diurnal profiles are characterized by stylized profiles of cortisol employing four distinct phases of change over the day, modelled as regression splines. Thus, we are able to examine the slope of each of the four segments and describe these separately. Interactions of material hardship and the regression splines were used to model cortisol profiles over the day as a function of chronic stress. All models were adjusted for possible confounders, including age, waking time of the respondent, smoking status (current smoker vs not), body mass index (BMI), and race. Confounders were included in the models on the basis of significant bivariate associations. Measures of whether or not the respondent had smoked, or eaten, immediately prior to collection of the salivary sample, and pregnancy status of the respondent were tested and not found to significantly alter the level or shape of the cortisol profile;

1140 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF EPIDEMIOLOGY Cortisol, nmol/l 20 19 18 17 16 15 14 13 12 11 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 Hours since waking Figure 1 Plot of salivary cortisol measures obtained over the course of the day from 188 respondents. Loess-smoothed plot based on 631 salivary cortisol measures, obtained from 188 respondents in sample Table 1 Cortisol sample collection details by level of hardship Low Moderate High hardship hardship hardship (n = 85) (n = 49) (n = 54) Mean number of 3.3 (3.1 3.5) 3.7 (3.4 3.9) 3.1 (2.9 3.4) measures (95% CI) Proportion with four 57.7 73.4 53.7 measures Mean waking time 7:36 am 7:30 am 7:03 am Median time difference 5 10 10 between waking and 1st measure (minutes) Median time difference 31 34 31 between 1st and 2nd measure (minutes) Median time difference 13:45 13:12 13:23 between 2nd morning measure and bedtime measure (hours:minutes) Median hours between 10:49 10:25 10:40 waking and clinic sample accordingly, they were not included in the final models. Random effects were assumed for the intercept (awakening value of cortisol), and for up to three of the slopes parameters depicting phases of the profile during the day. Variants of the material hardship measure were tested to demonstrate robustness as well to identify the best specification of the effect. Results Women who provided cortisol measures were similar to nonrespondents on a variety of measures, including age and race composition, physical health barriers, and level of material hardship. While the range of the hardship measure as constructed is from 0 8, the measure is heavily skewed towards the right, with ~45% of the cortisol sample having a value of 0, and 95% of respondents scoring 3 or less on this scale. For analyses utilizing the hardship scale as an ordinal categorical measure, cutpoints were set at 0, 1, and 2 or more, corresponding to 45, 26, and 29% of the respondents respectively. Of the 188 women with at least one valid cortisol measure, 61% had four measures, while 83% had at least three measures, and 8.5% (16) women provided only one measurement. The small group of women who had only one measurement had a higher mean level of chronic stress than women with two or more cortisol measurements (1.5 vs 0.96), but this difference was not statistically significant (P 0.18). Table 1 presents cortisol collection details by level of hardship. There is little evidence that the number, sequence or timing of measurements was systematically related to level of hardship. Waking time appears to be higher among women with high levels of hardship, but the difference is small and not statistically significant (P 0.21). Higher levels of hardship are also associated with slight delays in the timing of the first morning measurements; again, these differences are not significant. No differences by hardship are apparent for the other collection times. The observed differences in waking time and time of collection of first sample was taken into account in specification and testing of the spline models. As per protocol, collection times were clustered in the first 1.5 h after awakening (49% of all samples), and in the period after 12 h (23% of all samples). In contrast, a large majority of the clinic samples were taken between 8 and 11 h after waking. Thus, the inclusion of the clinic sample allowed for a reasonably uniform distribution of samples over the rest of the day, but did not alter inferences in the critical hour after waking. The range of cortisol in the final sample was 0.012 109.3 nmol/l, with a mean of 10.8 and a SD of 10.1. By visual inspection, cortisol levels rise sharply from an awakening level of about 11 nmol/l for ~30 45 min after awakening (Phase 1), followed by a rapid decline for approximately the next half hour (Phase 2), and a slower decline over the rest of the day (Phase 3). Visual inspection suggests another inflection point at ~14 h after awakening, at which a very slight increase in cortisol levels ensues; accordingly, this point was set to mark the beginning of a final phase (Phase 4). A regression-spline model confirmed that at each of these points, there was a significant change in slope from the previous phase. Thus, the knots (or inflection points) for the stress model were fixed at 30 min past awakening, 75 min past awakening, and 14 h past awakening. The fit of this regression model was robust to small changes (within 10 min before or after) in the selected inflection points. Table 2 presents model-based estimates of the waking level of cortisol (intercept), and slopes of cortisol change for hardship levels of 0 and (on a scale of 1 to 8) for a unit difference on the hardship scale. The underlying model regresses diurnal profiles of salivary cortisol against material hardship levels, controlling for age, race, BMI, smoking status, and time of waking. The intercepts are calculated for a hypothetical black woman aged 35 with BMI of 32, and waking time set at 7:30. The slope (phase) values (in nmol/l/h) describe rate of change in cortisol levels over the course of the day. Waking levels of cortisol, represented by the intercept, do not differ by level of hardship.

DIURNAL RHYTHM OF SALIVARY CORTISOL 1141 Table 2 Model-based estimates of salivary cortisol levels on waking and over the course of day, for a one unit contrast in hardship level Estimated effect of a unit Estimated Estimated increase in stress parameters for parameters for Difference in hardship level = 0 hardship level = 1 estimates for P-value for unit Estimate (SE) Estimate (SE) one unit (SE) difference Intercept (waking level) 11.33 (1.14) 11.07 (0.98) 0.26 (0.73) 0.7209 Phase 1 (0 30 min): period of 16.16 (2.20) 12.97 (1.81) 3.19 (1.71) 0.0642 morning rise Phase 2 (30 75 min): period of 9.88 (1.36) 7.95 (1.07) 1.93 (0.94) 0.0399 morning sharp decline Phase 3 (75 min 14 h): period of 0.59(0.10) 0.55 (0.07) 0.04 (0.06) 0.5609 slow decline over the rest of waking day Phase 4 (beyond 14 h): period of 0.35 (0.47) 0.58(0.33) 0.24 (0.32) 0.4660 change in last few hours before bedtime Age (centred at 35 years) 1.18 (0.46) 1.18 (0.46) The small decline in cortisol level ( 0.26 nmol/l) associated with a unit increase in hardship level is not significantly different from zero. The slope parameter of Phase 1 implies that cortisol levels rise at the rate of 16.12 nmol/l/h, in the first 30 min after awakening, for a person with a hardship level of 0. An increase in hardship level by one unit attenuates the steepness of this morning rise to 12.97 nmol/l/h. The difference in slopes during this rising phase, 3.19 nmol/l/h, is marginally significant (P = 0.0642). The slope parameters of Phase 2 imply a decline in cortisol, at the rate of 9.88 nmol/l/h for women with a hardship level of 0, and a slower decline, at 7.95 nmol/l/h for women with a hardship level of 1. The difference in slopes for a unit increase in stress in Phase 2 is significant (1.93 nmol/l/h, P = 0.04). There is no evidence that level of hardship moderates the period of slow decline described by Phases 3 and 4. Of the confounders examined, only age, BMI, and waking time were seen to be associated with the waking level of cortisol in these multivariate models. In addition to the fixed effects shown in the table, the intercept and the slopes for Phases 1 3 were allowed to vary randomly; the estimates obtained for these effects confirm that there is significant variability between individuals in each of these parameters. In an alternative specification, the material hardship variable was employed as an ordinal categorical variable, with three categories corresponding to approximate tertiles, as shown in Table 1. The predicted cortisol rhythms for women at the lowest and highest tertiles of stress are presented in Figure 2. There is no difference in the waking level of cortisol between women at the highest and lowest level of hardship (10.6 nmol/l as compared to 10.9 nmol/l). However, differences in cortisol profiles by hardship levels appear soon after waking. Women at low levels of hardship experience a sharp rise in the first halfhour after waking [16.0 nmol/l/h (CI: 11.3 20.7)], while this rise is blunted among women with high hardship levels [5.9 nmol/l/h (CI: 0 13.4; P-value for difference = 0.03)]. The blunted morning rise associated with hardship is associated with a significantly blunted morning decline as well. Under low hardship conditions, the rate of decline is 9.5 nmol/l/h Cortisol (nmol/l) 25 20 15 10 5 0 Low hardship level High hardship level 0 5 10 15 20 25 Hours since waking Figure 2 Fitted cortisol profiles for low and high levels of material hardship. Estimated values of cortisol for women at high and low levels of hardship obtained from a model regressing cortisol level against a 3-category ordinal variable describing material hardship, and controlling for age, race, BMI, smoking status, and time of waking (CI: 12.5 to 6.6); with high levels of hardship, the decline is slowed to 4.3 nmol/l/h (CI: 8.4 to 0.1), for a significant difference of 5.3 nmol/l/h (P = 0.02). This slower morning increase and decline in cortisol levels among highly stressed women results in their maintaining slightly (but not significantly) lower levels of cortisol over most of the rest of the day, despite the fact that waking levels of cortisol do not vary by level of hardship. There is some suggestion that high levels of stress are associated with elevations in evening cortisol, but the data are too sparse in this region to support a statistically significant conclusion. To test that these effects were robust, the model was re-estimated with more and less inclusive definitions of the basic hardship variable, as well as multiple specifications of the hardship variable used above, including collapsing the range at the upper end, and specifying it as an ordinal variable with two, three or four categories. The direction of association of the spline slope parameters with hardship level persisted across the models,

1142 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF EPIDEMIOLOGY 20 15 High stress Moderate stress Low stress 10 Cortisol in nmol/l 5 0 5 10 15 Waking level in Phase 1 in Phase 2 in Phase 3 in Phase 4 Figure 3 Effects of differing levels of hardship on modelled estimates of awakening level and slopes of change in cortisol level during each phase although the statistical significance of the association (in terms of P-value) varied with specification. Figure 3 presents parameter estimates from one of these alternative specifications, which employed a more inclusive measure of hardship, ranging from 0 to 13, to permit finer gradations. The graded response of estimated parameters to hardship level evident in Figure 3 indicates that material hardship has a dose-response effect on the slopes of the morning rise and fall in cortisol levels, with the degree of attenuation of parameters dependent on the degree of hardship. Thus, while level of hardship is not associated with awakening levels of cortisol, it is evident that elevated hardship level is associated with blunting of both the morning Phase 1 (0 30 min after awakening) rise in cortisol as well as the subsequent Phase 2 (30 75 min after awakening) decline, with the degree of blunting related to the degree of stress. Over the rest of the day (after 75 min past awakening), the rate of decline in cortisol levels does not vary by hardship level. Gradation in cortisol response by level of hardship was evident in some specifications, where the lowest and highest levels of hardship were clearly separated; however, P-values did not attain significance in those models because of small sample sizes in the extreme hardship category. Discussion Examining the effects of chronic stress on cortisol is an exercise fraught with difficulties. The particular measure of stress employed may or may not impact on salivary cortisol levels, as the earlier review of the literature suggested. The process of obtaining repeated and carefully timed measures of cortisol within a laboratory setting may itself have consequences for the stress level of participants. The circadian rhythms of cortisol, the high degree of inter-individual variation, and the marked differences by age and sex in cortisol production all make for considerable difficulty in arriving at useful summary measures or credible conclusions regarding the sensitivity of cortisol secretion to chronic or acute exposures. The results of the analysis show differences in cortisol profiles by level of chronic economic stress, operationalized here by measures of material hardship. The difference in profile is most marked immediately after awakening (morning challenge), when both the cortisol rise at awakening and the decline subsequent to awakening are sluggish among women who are economically stressed, resulting in lower cortisol levels for these women in the morning. The lowered cortisol levels seen among highly stressed women over the rest of the day are a consequence of the blunting of the morning rise and decline. The results support the suggestion that the HPA axis adapts to chronic stress by downregulation, but it is in the waking response that this downregulation is most evident. A recent review 22 concludes that it is indeed the awakening response of cortisol that may have the most significance in terms of a link between psychosocial factors and physiological functioning. The study has some limitations. Since this is already a chronically stressed sample, the range of exposure observed here is limited, and the findings may not be generalizable outside this range, or may underestimate the magnitude of the differences in cortisol response seen in the population. The findings are limited to women; other studies 14 have shown male female differences in cortisol responses to chronic stress. Finally, on the basis of visual inspection of the data, the model assumed equal timing of phases across levels of stress. With larger samples, and similar statistical models, it should be possible to test variants of this assumption. The increasing number of population-based studies obtaining cortisol data provides an opportunity to apply these methods and test these findings more generally, and to re-evaluate their implications

DIURNAL RHYTHM OF SALIVARY CORTISOL 1143 for the assumed physiological sequelae of stress associated with chronic socioeconomic disadvantage. Acknowledgements Data collection and analysis were supported by an award from the National Institute for Child Health and Development (HD P50 HD38986) and by funds from the Michigan Initiative on Inequalities in Health to George A Kaplan. Major funding of the WES study was provided by the Charles Stewart Mott Foundation and the Joyce Foundation. Elizabeth A Young is funded by an award from the National Institute of Mental Health (MH01931). We thank the participants in the WES who so generously participated in the study. References 1 Brunner EJ. Towards a new social biology. In: Berkman L, Kawachi I (eds). Social Epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press, 2000, 349 67. 2 Macleod J, Davey Smith G. Psychosocial factors and public health: a suitable case for treatment? J Epidemiol Community Health 2003; 57:565 70. 3 McEwen BS. Protective and damaging effects of stress mediators. N Engl J Med 1998;338:171 79. 4 De Kloet ER. Hormones and the stressed brain. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2004; 1018:1 15. 5 Reynolds RM, Walker BR. Human insulin resistance: the role of glucocorticoids. Diabetes Obes Metab 2003;5:5 12. 6 Bjorntorp P. Do stress reactions cause abdominal obesity and comorbidities? Obes Rev 2001;2:73 86. 7 Whitworth JA, Brown MA, Kelly JJ, Williamson PM. Mechanisms of cortisol-induced hypertension in humans. Steroids 1995;60:76 80. 8 Miller GE, Cohen S, Ritchey AK. Chronic psychological stress and the regulation of pro-inflammatory cytokines: a glucocorticoid-resistance model. Health Psychol 2002;21:531 41. 9 Murphy BE. Steroids and depression. J Steroid Biochem Mol Biol 1991; 38:537 59. 10 Bonne O, Brandes D, Segman R, Pitman RK, Yehuda R, Shalev AY. Prospective evaluation of plasma cortisol in recent trauma survivors with posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res 2003;119: 171 75. 11 Dugue B, Leppanen E, Grasbeck R. The driving license examination as a stress model: effects on blood picture, serum cortisol and the production of interleukins in man. Life Sci 2001;68:1641 47. 12 Ockenfels MC, Porter L, Smyth J, Kirschbaum C, Hellhammer DH, Stone AA. Effect of chronic stress associated with unemployment on salivary cortisol: overall cortisol levels, diurnal rhythm, and acute stress reactivity. Psychosom Med 1995;57:460 67. 13 Steptoe A, Kunz-Ebrecht S, Owen N, Feldman PJ, Willemsen G, Kirschbaum C, Marmot M. Socioeconomic status and stress-related biological responses over the working day. Psychosom Med 2003; 65:461 70. 14 Kunz-Ebrecht SR, Kirschbaum C, Steptoe A. Work stress, socioeconomic status and neuroendocrine activation over the working day. Soc Sci Med 2004;58:1523 30. 15 Pruessner JC, Hellhammer DH, Kirschbaum C. Burnout, perceived stress, and cortisol responses to awakening. Psychosom Med 1999;61: 197 204. 16 Heim C, Ehlert U, Hellhammer DH. The potential role of hypocortisolism in the pathophysiology of stress-related bodily disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2000;25:1 35. 17 Gunnar MR, Vazquez DM. Low cortisol and a flattening of expected daytime rhythm: potential indices of risk in human development. Dev Psychopathol 2001;13:515 38. 18 Mayer SE, Jencks C. Poverty and the distribution of material hardship. J Hum Resour 1989;24:88 113. 19 Edin K, Lein L. Work, welfare, and single mother s economic survival strategies. Am Sociol Rev 1996;61:253 66. 20 Ouellette T, Burstein N, Long D, Beecroft E. Measures of material hardship: Final Report. Prepared for: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. http://aspe.hhs.gov/hsp/material-hardship04/ (June 6, 2005 date last accessed). 21 Ranjit N, Young EA, Raghunathan TE, Kaplan G. Modeling cortisol rhythms in a population-based study. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2005; 30:615 24. 22 Clow A, Thorn L, Evans P, Hucklebridge F. The awakening cortisol response: methodological issues and significance. Stress 2004;7:29 37.