Research Note. Orientation Selectivity in the Cat's Striate Cortex is Invariant with Stimulus Contrast*

Similar documents
Monocular and Binocular Mechanisms of Contrast Gain Control. Izumi Ohzawa and Ralph D. Freeman

Dual Inhibitory Mechanisms for Definition of Receptive Field Characteristics in Cat Striate Cortex

The Influence of Contrasts on Directional and Spatial Frequency Tuning in Visual Cortex Areas 17/18 of the Cat

Nonlinear processing in LGN neurons

Temporal-frequency tuning of cross-orientation suppression in the cat striate cortex

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 8 November 1972)

Spectrograms (revisited)

Scotopic contrast sensitivity in infants evaluated by evoked potentials

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Disinhibition Outside Receptive Fields in the Visual Cortex

Motion direction signals in the primary visual cortex of cat and monkey

Nonlocal origin of response suppression from stimulation outside the classic receptive field in area 17 of the cat

Adaptation to contingencies in macaque primary visual cortex

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

Are primate lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) cells really sensitive to orientation or direction?

Simplification of responses of complex cells in cat striate cortex: suppressive surrounds and feedback inactivation

Effects of urethane on the response properties of visual cortical neurons in young adult and old cats

Local Disparity Not Perceived Depth Is Signaled by Binocular Neurons in Cortical Area V1 of the Macaque

Spectro-temporal response fields in the inferior colliculus of awake monkey

Neural Correlates of Perceived Brightness in the Retina, Lateral Geniculate Nucleus, and Striate Cortex

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Contrast discrimination with sinusoidal gratings of different spatial frequency

Input-speci"c adaptation in complex cells through synaptic depression

compared to electrophysiological studies on X (sustained) and Y (transient) flickering stimuli (lines and gratings) on the contrast threshold

Experience-dependent recovery of vision following chronic deprivation amblyopia. Hai-Yan He, Baisali Ray, Katie Dennis and Elizabeth M.

Information Conveyed by Onset Transients in Responses of Striate Cortical Neurons

SPATIAL FREQUENCY (CPD) Contrast evoked potentials in strabismic and anisometropic amblyopia. DENNIS M. LEVI 10; 6 :

Functional Maturation of the Macaque s Lateral Geniculate Nucleus

Visual Cortex Neurons of Monkeys and Cats: Temporal Dynamics of the Spatial Frequency Response Function

Selectivity and Spatial Distribution of Signals From the Receptive Field Surround in Macaque V1 Neurons

Vision Research 49 (2009) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Vision Research. journal homepage:

Binocularly Deprived Cats: Binocular Tests of Cortical Cells Show Regular Patterns of Interaction

The Neural Representation of Speed in Macaque Area MT/V5

Early and Late Mechanisms of Surround Suppression in Striate Cortex of Macaque

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4.

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

The Emergence of Contrast-Invariant Orientation Tuning in Simple Cells of Cat Visual Cortex

MULTIUNIT ACTIVITY OF THE CAT LATERAL GENICULATE NEURONS EVOKED BY MOVING LIGHT PATTERN OF VARIABLE INTENSITY

Supplementary Information for Correlated input reveals coexisting coding schemes in a sensory cortex

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

THE INFLUENCE EXERTED BY NEMBUTAL ON REACTIVITY OF THE SUPERIOR COLLICULUS NEURONS

Relative Contributions of Burst and Tonic Responses to the Receptive Field Properties of Lateral Geniculate Neurons in the Cat

Primary Visual Pathways (I)

Comment by Delgutte and Anna. A. Dreyer (Eaton-Peabody Laboratory, Massachusetts Eye and Ear Infirmary, Boston, MA)

Neural Basis for Stereopsis from Second-Order Contrast Cues

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017

PHYSIOLOGICAL OPTICS

Neuronal responses to plaids

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings

Different Roles for Simple-Cell and Complex-Cell Inhibition in V1

Sum of Neurally Distinct Stimulus- and Task-Related Components.

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

A Tutorial Essay on Spatial Filtering and Spatial Vision

Membrane Potential and Conductance Changes Underlying Length Tuning of Cells in Cat Primary Visual Cortex

The Nature of V1 Neural Responses to 2D Moving Patterns Depends on Receptive-Field Structure in the Marmoset Monkey

Organization of Binocular Pathways: Modeling and Data Related to Rivalry

(Received 28 May 1974)

Emanuel Todorov, Athanassios Siapas and David Somers. Dept. of Brain and Cognitive Sciences. E25-526, MIT, Cambridge, MA 02139

Does image movement have a special nature for neurons in the cat's striate cortex?

Mechanisms of stimulus feature selectivity in sensory systems

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System

We (1 4) and many others (e.g., 5 8) have studied the

Retinal DOG filters: high-pass or high-frequency enhancing filters?

Substructure of Direction-Selective Receptive Fields in Macaque V1

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

BY KENJIRO YAMANA AND YOSHIHIRO TOH* Department of Biology, Faculty of Science, Kyushu University, Fukuoka 812, Japan. Accepted 27 May 1987 SUMMARY

The Visual System. Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004

Inhibition, Spike Threshold, and Stimulus Selectivity in Primary Visual Cortex

Receiver operating characteristic (ROC) analysis of neurons in the cat's lateral geniculate nucleus during tonic and burst response mode

Stimulus dependence of neuronal correlation in primary visual cortex of the macaque Adam Kohn* and Matthew A. Smith

Thalamo-Cortical Relationships Ultrastructure of Thalamic Synaptic Glomerulus

2/3/17. Visual System I. I. Eye, color space, adaptation II. Receptive fields and lateral inhibition III. Thalamus and primary visual cortex

Linguistic Phonetics Fall 2005

Attention March 4, 2009

The Role of Thalamic Population Synchrony in the Emergence of Cortical Feature Selectivity

background depends upon the direction of background movement. When background contrasts.

Cellular Mechanisms Underlying Stimulus-Dependent Gain Modulation in Primary Visual Cortex Neurons In Vivo

The response properties of primary visual cortical (V1) neurons

Linguistic Phonetics. Basic Audition. Diagram of the inner ear removed due to copyright restrictions.

Spectral Characteristics of Phase Sensitivity and Discharge Rate of Neurons in the Ascending Tectofugal Visual System

Morton-Style Factorial Coding of Color in Primary Visual Cortex

The origin of the oblique effect examined with pattern adaptation and masking

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision

Receptive fields and suppressive fields in the early visual system

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

COGS 101A: Sensation and Perception

Effects of Light Stimulus Frequency on Phase Characteristics of Brain Waves

Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

The processing of feature discontinuities for different cue types in primary visual cortex

High Visual Contrast Sensitivity in the Young Human Infant

Comparison Among Some Models of Orientation Selectivity

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones

Neural Recording Methods

Modulation of Cutaneous Cold Receptor Function by Electrolytes, Hormones and Thermal Adaptation

Measurement and modeling of center-surround suppression and enhancement

The Receptive-Field Organization of Simple Cells in Primary Visual Cortex of Ferrets under Natural Scene Stimulation

Transcription:

Exp Brain Res (1982) 46:457-461 9 Springer-Verlag 1982 Research Note Orientation Selectivity in the Cat's Striate Cortex is Invariant with Stimulus Contrast* G. Sclar and R.D. Freeman School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720, USA Summary. Extracellular responses of single units in striate cortex of the cat were studied quantitatively. Sinusoidal gratings were used as stimuli and the variables of interest were orientation and contrast. Specifically, we wanted to determine if orientation tuning was dependent on contrast. Of 45 cells studied in detail, two basic types of contrast-response pattern were observed, but most patterns were intermediate between these extremes. In one type, responses increased approximately linearly with log contrast while in the second, saturation was found at low contrast levels. For all these cells, orientation tuning characteristics were independent of contrast. An additional observation, made from 14 cells, was that stimuli presented at non-optimal orientations can suppress responses to below the general maintained discharge levels. In eight of these cases, the inhibition was clearly contrast-dependent. Key words: Cat visual cortex - Orientation tuning - Contrast Introduction The relationships between stimulus contrast and responses of visuai neurons deserve considerable attention because they must be of fundamental importance in visual function. In X-cells of the retina and LGN, responses appear to be linear at low contrasts, but they vary logarithmically at high contrast levels (Mallei and Fiorentini 1973; Robson 1975). In visual cortex, responses are reported to be * Supported by US National Institutes of Health Grant EY01175 and Research Career Development Award EY00092 to R.D. Freeman. G. Sclar received support from National Institutes of Health Training Grant EY07043 Offprint requests to: R.D. Freeman, MD (address see above) linear for about a log unit above threshold until saturation levels are reached, but conclusions from these observations are limited by considerable variability in the data (Tolhurst et al. 1981). What is clear is that visual neurons do not respond in some simple proportional manner as stimulus contrast is varied. Contrast-response functions may vary considerably with location in the visual pathway, cell type, and absolute contrast levels. One would predict, therefore, that absolute contrast level should affect measurements of other stimulus parameters (Robson 1975). For example, orientation or spatial or temporal tuning properties of cortical cells should vary with contrast level. Specifically, for cells that show saturation, the degree of selectivity for these parameters should be underestimated. We report here a study designed to test the above hypothesis. We determined the orientation-tuning of cortical cells using sinusoidal gratings of various contrasts. Orientation selectivity was chosen as the variable to be measured because it appears to be the only function which is first elaborated in visual cortex, and in addition, it is a very stable property of cortical cells (Henry et al. 1973; Rose and Blakemore 1974; Hammond et al. 1975). Methods Adult cats (2-4 kg), which were premedicated with injections of atropine and Acepromazine, were anesthetized with halothane while a femoral vein was cannulated. Anesthesia was continued with sodium thioamyl and a tracheal cannula was positioned. A small cranial hole at Horsley-Clarke P4 near the midline was made, and the dura was reflected. The animal was paralyzed with gallamine triethiodide (10 mg/kg 9 h -1) and artificially ventilated with a mixture of N20/02/C02 (75 : 24 : 1). Sodium thioamyl was added as required during the experiment so that the EEG contained high amplitude low frequency peaks interspersed with high frequency waves, which we took to indicate an adequate state 0014-4819/82/0046/0457/$ 1.00

458 G. Sclar and R.D. Freeman: Invariance of Orientation Tuning with Stimulus Contrast of anesthesia. Peak expired CO2 was held at around 4.5%. Temperature was maintained at about 37.5 ~ C, and EKG and heart rate were monitored. Prior to recording, topical neo-synephrine and atropine solutions were applied, and contact lenses with 3 mm artificial pupils were placed on the corneas. Spectacle lenses were used in addition to attain conjugacy, between the retinae, a tangent screen and a display TV screen. The screens were 57 cm from the eyes. A reversible ophthalmoscope was used to project retinal landmarks onto the tangent screen. Sinusoidal gratings, displayed on the TV tube, were bright (300 cd/m 2) and relatively large (a 10 ~ circular aperture was used to mask the screen). Contrast was linear to values of at least 85%. These gratings were generated by a microcomputer under control of a minicomputer, which was used to run the experiments. Tungsten-in-glass microelectrodes were advanced into striate cortex through a layer of agar over the brain surface and a wax covering sealed the chamber. Action potentials from individual cells were isolated extracellularly and conventional methods were used to amplify and display impulses by audio and visual devices. Spikes were viewed on a display CRT and a window discriminator selected impulses to be fed to the input buffer of a computer. Small electrolytic lesions were made along each electrode track. At the end of the experiment, animals were deeply anaesthetized and perfused with formalin. Subsequent histological analysis was performed to establish recording sites. Once a cell was isolated, its receptive fields were plotted manually. Ocular dominance, orientation limits, direction selectivity and velocity characteristics were determined subjectively. A front surface mirror was then positioned to place the receptive field of the dominant eye in the center of the TV screen. The other eye was occluded. Short quantitative trials were then run, prior to the main experiment, to determine preferred orientation, spatial, and temporal frequencies. A response versus contrast curve was then obtained, using the preferred stimulus values. The main experiment was typically run over a range of contrasts of around 0.82 log units. The highest and lowest contrasts generally used were 80% and 2-10%, respectively. The central measurements required responses to a stimulus set comprised of 28 or 30 variables (4-5 contrasts at equal intervals along a log scale, as a function of 6-7 orientations). All stimulus conditions were randomly interleaved. Included in the set was a null or blank presentation consisting of a field of uniform luminance, i.e., zero contrast to measure maintained discharge levels. Each stimulus was presented ten times, for 4 s per trial, and trials were preceded by a 1-s foreperiod during which no stimulus was presented. Responses to the stimuli were accumulated in 256 bin histograms representing 2 s. Responses to the gratings were Fourier transformed, to obtain the amplitude at the D.C. level (zero frequency component) or the first harmonic of the temporal modulation rate used. The larger of these values was used to specify responses. Results and Discussion We have obtained complete data, as described above, for 45 cells. For each cell, a given set of data may be analyzed from two points of view: by examining the effects of contrast upon orientation tuning, or by observing the effects of orientation on contrast-response curves. Examples of the first type of analysis are shown in Fig. la and c. In Fig. la, orientation tuning curves are given for contrast levels of 10%, 20%, 40%, and 80%. Some degree of saturation is evident between the upper two curves (40% and 80%) but below that, response is an approximately linear function of log contrast. Note that all four curves peak at around the same orientation. Responses from a different type of cell are shown in Fig. lc. Once again, four contrast levels were used (10%, 20%, 40%, and 80%). In this case, saturation occurs at much lower values, and responses are only slightly reduced at a contrast of 10%. What is striking here is that, relatively, saturation applies to all orientations tested, in the sense that no increase in response is found as contrast is increased. Using responses of these same two cells, the second form of analysis is illustrated in Fig. lb and d. In Fig. lb contrast-response functions are given at optimal (top curve) and sub-optimal (lower six curves) orientations. For all orientations, responses vary approximately linearly with log contrast, and slopes decrease monotonically with reduced peak response levels. Contrast-response functions for the other cell, shown in Fig. ld, also vary approximately linearly with log contrast, but in this case, slopes are relatively flat for all orientations. Once again, this demonstrates the remarkably uniform relative saturation at suboptimal as well as optimal orientations. The orientation tuning curves and contrast-response functions shown in Fig. 1 represent two basic patterns that we observed. Response patterns of most cells were generally intermediate between these two types. Differentiation could not be made on the basis of receptive field classification. We classified 19 Fig. 1. a, c Orientation tuning curves are shown for two simple ceils from which responses were obtained to presentations of sinusoidal gratings at the following contrast levels: 10% (open circles), 20% (asterisks), 40% (crosses), 80% (filled circles). In these and succeeding graphs, dashed lines indicate spontaneous discharge levels. Response patterns of these cells are quite different, but in both cases preferred orientation and bandwidth remained constant over the range of contrasts tested. The data of a and c are replotted in b and d, respectively, to illustrate the different response patterns of the two cells. Now, contrast, on a log axis, is the variable and response functions are plotted for the seven orientations shown on the left. Preferred orientation is at the top and successively lower curves represent increasing angular distances from the optimal value. Note that the functions in b are approximately linear but the slopes change markedly from steep at the top to shallow or fiat on the bottom. The functions in d, on the other hand, are nearly all flat indicating response saturation at relatively low contrasts. Summaries of data from these cells and all others studied are given in e and f. For each cell, preferred orientations and orientation tuning characteristics (half-widths at half-heights) were noted for all contrast levels. Then the slopes were computed of linear regression fits of preferred orientation and tuning widths as functions of contrast. The distributions of these slopes for orientation and tuning are shown in the histograms of e and f, respectively. Most slopes are close to zero indicating that these functions are relatively constant as contrast is varied

G. Sclar and R.D. Freeman: Invariance of Orientation Tuning with Stimulus Contrast 459 80 80 64 64 48 48 32 32 16 16 (,0 244 236 227 219 211 ~12 194 38 38 ~ gg.el 24 24 18 18 12 12 6 8 Or-ien~as (Oegs) Con~r-as~ (7.) 25 25 r O q- 0 Z L E 15 5 (e) 15 5 (F) Fig. la-f ~-. 15 -. I 8. I ~, 15 ~-. 15 -. I e. I >. 15 Slope Slope

460 G. Sclar and R. D. Freeman: Invariance of Orientation Tuning with Stimulus Contrast CO (I/ 48 24 12 112 CL. 84 Lf~ 58 28 5 Ig 2g 4g (c) 4g 24 If 8 L' I Fig. 2a--d. Responses of four complex cells are given for gratings at the contrast levels indicated. Sweeps at three different orientations were used to obtain the functions shown for each cell. The orientations used were optimal (filled circles), moderately suboptimal (filled squares, +_ 25 ~ 31 ~ 39 ~, 65 ~ in a, b, c, and d, respectively) and substantially suboptimal (open circles, _+ 50 ~ 47 ~ 59 ~ and 83 ~ in a, b, c, and d, respectively). Note that contrast dependent inhibition was present in each case when the most inappropriate orientation was used, and that responses were below the spontaneous levels (dashed lines) as determined with blank stimuli (see Methods) cells as simple and 26 as complex but contrastresponse patterns were independent of these categories. In the case of the first pattern, Fig. la and b, the responses are approximately proportional to log contrast but the proportionality (slope) is a function of stimulus orientation. In the second (Fig. lc and d), the cell is relatively insensitive to changes in contrast at all orientations. However, in both these cases, inspection of Fig. 1 suggests that orientation tuning properties may remain relatively unaffected as contrast is changed. Returning to the hypothesis stated at the outset, we now consider these tuning properties in detail. We have determined preferred orientations and tuning characteristics at each contrast level, for the population of cells we studied. In accordance with previous procedures we calculated linear regression fits around peak responses to estimate preferred orientations and half-width at half-height values (Henry et al. 1973; Rose and Blakemore 1974). Linear regression fits were then computed between these quantities and contrast. If the data of Fig. la-d are typical, then the slopes of these regression fits should be quite flat. Slopes of zero would indicate that contrast changes have insignificant effects on orientation tuning and preference. The distributions of slopes for all 45 cells recorded are given in Fig. le and f. In Fig. le, the histogram represents slopes of regression fits to preferred orientation vs. contrast values. Data of Fig. lf, similarly, are regression fit slopes of tuning (halfwidth at half-height) characteristics versus contrast values. In both cases, it is clear that the histograms peak markedly around a slope of zero. For only two cells, tuning patterns were marginally different from zero (t = 15, p < 0.05; t = 14.3, p < 0.05) and, in these cases, tuning sharpness appears, counterintuitively, to decrease slightly as contrast increases. Aside from these exceptions, the results illustrated in the histograms suggest that orientation preference and tuning characteristics remain remarkably constant as contrast is altered. In addition to this central finding, one other feature of the data we analyzed is worth noting. Of the 45 cells we studied, 19 had substantial maintained discharge levels. For 14 of these cells, stimuli presented at nonoptimal orientations were inhibitory in that responses were clearly below maintained levels. For eight of these 14 cells, all of which had relatively high spontaneous levels (mean = 11.3 impulses/s, SD = 5.9), this inhibition was markedly contrast dependent. Typical responses from this group of cells, all classified as complex, are illustrated in Fig. 2. In summary, we have found that orientation preference and tuning characteristics of cortical cells are stable as contrast is varied. This holds over very wide ranges of contrast, nearly 2 log units, as well as near threshold levels. The implication of these results is that threshold and suprathreshold stimulus contrast levels should produce highly correlated responses of cortical cells. This appears to hold in the case of spatial frequency preference and bandwidth (Movshon et al. 1978). Our present findings do not support the hypothesis stated at the outset, that orientation tuning properties of cortical cells should vary with contrast. Perhaps the most intriguing finding of our experiments is related to the contrast-response functions that showed clear saturation. What is striking here is that relative saturation was evident at all orientations tested. These cells may receive a strong non-oriented excitatory input which causes saturation prior to the process of orientation selectivity, which is thought to be largely inhibitory (Creutzfeldt et al. 1974; Nelson and Frost 1978; Sillito 1979; Tsumoto et al. 1979). This could be due to saturation at the LGN as suggested in another study of cortical response functions (Henry et al. 1978). Cells which do not show saturation may have relatively weak excitatory input. Acknowledgements. We thank J. G. Robson for helpful discussions and A.B. Bonds for assistance with computer programming.

G. Sclar and R.D. Freeman: Invariance of Orientation Tuning with Stimulus Contrast 461 References Creutzfeldt OD, Kuhnt U, Benevento LA (1974) An intracellular analysis of visual cortical neurones to moving stimuli: Responses in a co-operative neuronal network. Exp Brain Res 21:251-274 Hammond P, Andrews DP, James CR (1975) Invariance of orientational and directional tuning in visual cortical cells of the adult cat. Brain Res 96:56-59 Henry GH, Bishop PO, Tupper RM, Dreher B (1973) Orientation specificity and response variability of cells in the striate cortex. Vision Res 13:1771-1779 Henry GH, Goodwin AW, Bishop PO (1978) Spatial summation of responses in receptive fields of single cells in cat striate cortex. Exp Brain Res 32:245-266 Maffei L, Fiorentini A (1973) The visual cortex as a spatial frequency analyzer. Vision Res 13:1255-1267 Movshon JA, Thompson ID, Tolhurst DJ (1978) Spatial and temporal contrast sensitivity of neurones in areas 17 and 18 of the cat's visual cortex. J Physiol (Lond) 283:101-120 Nelson JI, Frost BJ (1978) Orientation-selective inhibition from beyond the classical visual receptive field. Brain Res 139: 359-365 Robson JG (1975) Receptive fields: Spatial and intensive representation of the visual image. In: Carterette D, Friedman W (eds) Handbook of perception, vol 5. Academic Press, New York Rose D, Blakemore C (1974) An analysis of orientation selectivity in the cat's visual cortex. Exp Brain Res 20:1-17 Sillito A (1979) Inhibitory mechanisms influencing complex cell orientation selectivity and their modification at high resting discharge levels. J Physiol (Lond) 289:33-53 Tolhurst D J, Movshon JA, Thompson ID (1981) The dependence of response amplitude and variance of cat visual cortical neurones on stimulus contrast. Exp Brain Res 41:414M19 Tsumoto T, Eckart W, Creutzfeldt OD (1979) Modification of orientation sensitivity of cat visual cortex neurons by removal of GABA-mediated inhibition. Exp Brain Res 34:351-363 Received December 18, 1981 / Accepted February 24, 1982