ELECTRONIC APPENDIX. This is the Electronic Appendix to the article

Similar documents
Experimental evolution under hyperpromiscuity in Drosophila melanogaster

Relative Effectiveness of Mating Success and Sperm Competition at Eliminating Deleterious Mutations in Drosophila melanogaster

Remating in Drosophila melanogaster: an examination of the trading-up and intrinsic male-quality hypotheses

ELIMINATION OF MUTANT TYPES IN SELECTION EXPERIMENT BETWEEN WILD TYPE AND MUTANT EYE COLOUR IN DROSOPHILA ANANASSAE

Sex in Space! Does Sexual Selection Accelerate or Hinder Adaptation?

ASSOCIATION BETWEEN SEX RATIO DISTORTION AND SEXUALLY ANTAGONISTIC FITNESS CONSEQUENCES OF FEMALE CHOICE

SEXUAL SELECTION AND IMMUNE FUNCTION IN DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER

Edinburgh Research Explorer

GENETIC DRIFT IN ANTAGONISTIC GENES LEADS TO DIVERGENCE IN SEX-SPECIFIC FITNESS BETWEEN EXPERIMENTAL POPULATIONS OF DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER

Linkage Mapping in Drosophila Melanogaster

READING ASSIGNMENT GENETIC ANALYSIS OF DROSOPHILA POPULATIONS I. HOW DO MITOSIS AND MEIOSIS COMPARE?

COMPARATIVE BEHAVIOURAL ANALYSIS OF MATING BETWEEN YELLOW AND WILD TYPE DROSOPHILA OF MELANOGASTER SPECIES GROUP

Drosophila melanogaster. Introduction. Drosophila melanogaster is a kind of flies fruit fly that is widely used in genetic

Rare male mating advantage in Drosophila melanogaster.

Chapter 15: The Chromosomal Basis of Inheritance

Male Remating in Drosophila ananassae: Evidence for Interstrain Variation in Remating Time and Shorter Duration of Copulation during Second Mating

Inheritance of Aldehyde Oxidase in Drosophila melanogaster

This thesis is protected by copyright which belongs to the author.

The Determination of the Genetic Order and Genetic Map for the Eye Color, Wing Size, and Bristle Morphology in Drosophila melanogaster

61A the flies were mass-mated in half-pint culture bottles containing the usual

Lab 5: Testing Hypotheses about Patterns of Inheritance

Lab 7 Heredity. Is there a fly in here?

IDENTIFYING THE INFLUENCE OF SELECTIVE EPISODES AND MECHANISMS ON REPRODUCTIVE SUCCESS IN TRIBOLIUM CASTANEUM AND DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER

Plasticity in reproductive effort of male dung flies. (Scatophaga stercoraria) as a response to larval density. P. STOCKLEY and N. J.

A gene is a sequence of DNA that resides at a particular site on a chromosome the locus (plural loci). Genetic linkage of genes on a single

The Modern Genetics View

Chapter 15: The Chromosomal Basis of Inheritance

Time allowed: 2 hours Answer ALL questions in Section A, ALL PARTS of the question in Section B and ONE question from Section C.

2. Evolution under Artificial Selection. Oil Content in Corn

SPERM SURVIVAL IN FEMALE STALK-EYED FLIES DEPENDS ON SEMINAL FLUID AND MEIOTIC DRIVE

Population Dynamics of the Wolbachia Infection Causing Cytoplasmic Incompatibility in Drosophila melanogaster

Measuring the effect of inbreeding on reproductive success in a population of fruit flies (Raleigh)

Sexual Selection and Condition Dependence in the Fruit Fly, Drosophila melanogaster

This document is a required reading assignment covering chapter 4 in your textbook.

Essential Question: How do living things inherit their genetic characteristics?

11.1 Genetic Variation Within Population. KEY CONCEPT A population shares a common gene pool.

Unit 4 Genetics. 3. Categorize the following characteristics below as being influenced by genetics or the environment.

EVOLUTION MICROEVOLUTION CAUSES OF MICROEVOLUTION. Evolution Activity 2.3 page 1

eyestalks make quick turns more difficult, we might expect there to be a trade-off between attracting mates and flight.

Activity 15.2 Solving Problems When the Genetics Are Unknown

Biology Department, University of North Carolina, CB# 3280, Coker Hall, Chapel Hill, North Carolina

Take a look at the three adult bears shown in these photographs:

Experiment 1. The aim here is to understand the pattern of

Repeated mating in a lek-mating insect, Drosophila melanogaster.

Biology 164 Laboratory

Parasitism of mating and non-mating males and body mass of Enallagma hageni

Patterns of Inheritance

RNAi strategies in support of mosquito SIT applications

Genetic analysis of sex-chromosome arrangement in Drosophila americana; a laboratory exercise for undergraduate or advanced placement students.

Superior sperm competitors sire higher-quality young

The Sexually Antagonistic Genes of Drosophila melanogaster

Genetics WS Part 7 Name Part 7: Incomplete Dominance or Codominance

Two hierarchies. Genes Chromosomes Organisms Demes Populations Species Clades

THE INFLUENCE OF LIGHT ON GENE FREQUENCY CHANGES IN LABORATORY POPULATIONS OF EBONY AND NON-EBONY DROSOPHILA MELANOGASTER M. E.

Science 1.9 AS WORKBOOK. Working to Excellence

Parental age effects on the female fitness traits in Drosophila melanogaster

Bio 102 Practice Problems Mendelian Genetics and Extensions

Laboratory. Mendelian Genetics

Sperm Precedence of Irradiated Mediterranean Fruit Fly Males (Diptera: Tephritidae)

A test of quantitative genetic theory using Drosophila effects of inbreeding and rate of inbreeding on heritabilities and variance components #

Meiosis. Prophase I But something else happens: each chromosome pairs up with the other member of its pair... Prophase I Chromosomes become visible...

Natural and experimental evolution of sexual conflict within Caenorhabditis nematodes

Biology of Breeding: Considerations for maximizing genetic diversity of breeding groups

Gallery Walk. Fundamentals of Genetics

Using Drosophila melanogaster in middle and high school classrooms

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

The intestinal microbiota of tephritid fruit flies as a potential tool to improve rearing and the SIT

Genetics Practice Test

The prevalence and persistence of sigma virus, a biparentally transmitted parasite of Drosophila melanogaster

Associations Between Sperm Competition and Natural Variation in Male Reproductive. Genes on the Third Chromosome of Drosophila melanogaster.

The sight of a feather in a peacock s tail, whenever I gaze at it, makes me sick. --Charles Darwin 1887

MAGENTA-ALPHA-A THIRD FREQUENTLY MUTATING CHAR- females, and the gene for miniature-a was found to be mutable in

Genetics and Reproduction Test Review Sheet. Number of Parents 2 Parents 1 Parent

Name Class Date. Review Guide. Genetics. The fundamental principles of genetics were first discovered by. What type of plant did he breed?.

Chromosomal inheritance & linkage. Exceptions to Mendel s Rules

Figure 1: Transmission of Wing Shape & Body Color Alleles: F0 Mating. Figure 1.1: Transmission of Wing Shape & Body Color Alleles: Expected F1 Outcome

Potato Head Genetics Gina Ford & Jennifer Hladun Twelve Bridges Middle School Lincoln California

Proximal mechanisms of the sex ratio and clutch size responses of the wasp Nasonia vitripennis to parasitized hosts

Name Date Class. Main Idea. Human traits are controlled by single genes with two alleles, single genes with... a. b. c.

TEST NAME:review TEST ID: GRADE:07 Seventh Grade SUBJECT:Life and Physical Sciences TEST CATEGORY: My Classroom

Unit 1 Biological Diversity Topic 1.1 Examining Diversity. Text p. 3-15

April, 19rg. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN NO CROSSING OVER IN THE MALE OF DROSOPHILA OF GENES IN THE SECOND AND THIRD PAIRS OF CHROMOSOMES. T. H. MORGAN.

Rapid evolution towards equal sex ratios in a system with heterogamety

Good resources. Why are females choosy? Good resources. Resources from males can tip the scales of relative investment

Good resources. Why are females choosy? Good resources. Good resources direct benefits Good genes indirect benefits

Sexual selection. 1) Sexual dimorphism. 2) Variation in mating success. 3) Sexual selection. 4) Female choice based on male ornaments

SEX. Genetic Variation: The genetic substrate for natural selection. Sex: Sources of Genotypic Variation. Genetic Variation

Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Introduction. Sexual selection Classification

Picture 1. Two stalk-eyed flies rearing/extending forearms in battle. Photo credit: Sam Cotton.

GENETICS - CLUTCH CH.2 MENDEL'S LAWS OF INHERITANCE.

Drosophila melanogaster females change mating behaviour and. offspring production based on social context

SEX RATIO MANIPULATION BY THE PARASITOID WASP MUSCIDIFURAX RAPTOR IN RESPONSE TO HOST SIZE

r = intrinsic rate of natural increase = the instantaneous rate of change in population size (per individual). If r > 0, then increasing

Experimental transplant of guppies. Predator: Pike-cichlid; preys mainly on large guppies

April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice. Female choice

The effect of light exposure on Drosophila melanogaster survival

Ch 8 Practice Questions

Passing It On. QUESTION: How are inherited characteristics passed from parent to offspring? toothpicks - red and green

F the mating activity and fertility in Ilrosophilo melanogaster. NovITSKY

Name: PS#: Biol 3301 Midterm 1 Spring 2012

Transcription:

ELECTRONIC APPENDIX This is the Electronic Appendix to the article Assessing putative interlocus sexual conflict in Drosophila melanogaster using experimental evolution by Andrew D. Stewart, Edward H. Morrow and William R. Rice Proc. R. Soc. B (doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3182) Electronic appendices are refereed with the text; however, no attempt is made to impose a uniform editorial style on the electronic appendices.

Electronic Appendices Appendix A: These protocols and data show that live-yeast is a limiting resource to female reproduction and that female density does not significantly affect female fecundity. Appendix B: These protocols and data show that female mating success in initial mating vials is nearly complete. Appendix C: These protocols and data show that most females remate during their time in the male-female interaction vials. Appendix D: These protocols and data show that the relative fitness of brown-eyed (bw/bw) females and red-eyed (bw/bw + ) females are approximately equal, while those of browneyed and red-eyed males significantly differ. Appendix E: These protocols and data show that males do not significantly influence female fecundity by either consuming or fouling the limiting resource, live-yeast. 1

Appendix A To measure the effect of fly density on female fecundity we first determined whether or not the amount of live-yeast applied to the surface of the killed-yeast medium was a limiting resource to female lifetime reproduction. Five replicate adult competition vials each with 16 mated females (with no males) were constructed with 0, 0.5, 1.0, and 1.45 times the normal level of yeast used to propagate the LH M laboratory population (10 mg/16 females). A simple linear relationship was found between average female fecundity and the level of live-yeast provided, indicating that live-yeast was the major factor limiting female lifetime fecundity (Appendix A: Figure 1, black circles, r 2 = 99.3%, P = 0.0037). To test for an effect of density besides its influence on the amount of live-yeast available per capita, we constructed five additional replicate vials of 11 females per vial (45% more yeast available per capita per vial) and 32 females per vial (50% less yeast available per capita per vial). We found neither a significant difference between the average fecundity of females at double density with normal yeast level (blue triangle in Appendix A: Figure 1) and females at normal density with 50% less yeast, nor between females at normal density with 45% more yeast and females at 31% lower density with normal yeast level, such that per capita yeast availability was increased by 45% (red square in Appendix A: Figure 1; P > 0.05, Tukey-Kramer multiple comparisons test). These data indicate that the effect of changing density is the same as a corresponding change in the level of the limiting resource, live-yeast. There was, however, a non-significant trend toward increased density reducing female lifetime fecundity less than would be expected by changes in per capita yeast consumption alone (blue triangle in Appendix A: Figure 1). There was also a non-significant trend where decreased density resulted in lower lifetime female fecundity than would be expected by changes in per capita yeast consumption alone (red square in Appendix A: Figure 1). 2

We suspect that these patterns, if real, are probably due to flies spreading new yeast colonies on their feet to additional locations on the surface of the killed-yeast medium in the vials. This effect of density, over and beyond that of the consumption of live-yeast, would make our unprotected environment a conservative estimate of male-induced harm (see electronic Appendix E). Appendix A: Figure 1) Relationship between the quantity of live-yeast provided during culture and average female fecundity in Drosophila melanogaster from the base LH M population (16 females per vial; black circles). Also shown is the average female fecundity under double density (32 females per vial; blue triangle) and under 45% reduced density (11 females per vial; red square). All mean squares are depicted ± standard error. 3

Appendix B To determine the proportion of virgin females that mated during the initial mating stage (Figure 1, left) a separate control experiment was conducted. Without anesthesia, 16 virgin females (bw/bw) were combined with 24 males (bw/bw) and allowed to mate for 2 hours (as in the main experimental protocol). After the initial mating, all males were removed under brief anesthesia and discarded. Females were placed in male-female interaction vials (but without males) with limited yeast for two days. After two days, females were placed into individual oviposition vials for 18 hours, then discarded. After 6 days, all oviposition vials were scored for the presence/absence of larvae. This process was replicated 20 times for a total of 320 females. During the two-hour initial mating period we found that nearly all females were mated (96.8 ± 1.1% [mean ± SE]). 4

Appendix C To estimate the level of remating in the male-female interaction vials, flies from the brown-eyed replica of the base population (LH M -bw) were used. On day-12 of its 2-week generation cycle 16 vials each containing 16 females were made. In 8 of the vials, we measured whether or not females had mated by the time that they were transferred to the male-female interaction vials. For each of these vials, the 16 brown-eyed females (bw/bw) were individually placed in oviposition vials for 18 hours then removed. Six days later we scored the vials for the presence/absence of larvae indicating that the female had mated and found that 95.3% of females were mated at the time of transfer to the male-female interaction vials. In the 8 remaining vials we measured the remate rate. In each vial, 16 red-eyed (bw + /bw + ) males from the LH M population were added to the 16 brown-eyed females (bw/bw). Any remating that occurred in these male-female interaction vials was apparent since any brown-eyed females that remate with these red-eyed males will produce red-eyed offspring. After 2-days, individual females were placed in oviposition vials for 18 hours, then removed. Twelve days later the offspring were screened for red-eyed flies. A total of 94.1% of the females produced at least one red-eyed offspring. After adjusting for the females that had not mated at the time of transfer, we estimate that 89.4 ± 2% (mean ± SE) of females remate in male-female interaction vials. Additionally, because of the high level of sperm displacement in D. melanogaster (approximately 85% in the LH M population) most offspring are sired from matings that occur in the male-female interaction vials. 5

Appendix D In order to determine the cause of the rise of the red allele in control populations, two experiments were conducted to determine the relative fitness between the brown eye-color and red eye color alleles when present in both males and females. Males: In the first experiment, we measured the fitness, within the context of sexual selection, of red-eyed males compared to brown-eyed males when they both competed for mating opportunities. A total of 16 replicate vials were assayed. In each replicate, 24 males (12 brown-eyed [bw/bw] and 12 red-eyed [bw + /bw]) were placed in an initial mating vial with 16 brown-eyed females (bw/bw) for 2 hours. Then, under light anesthesia, the number of males was reduced to 16 (8 each) and transferred with the 16 females to malefemale interaction vials, containing a limited amount of live-yeast. After the two days, females were transferred to individual oviposition vials for 18 hours, then removed. After twelve days all progeny were scored for eye color to determine male parentage. Females: In the second experiment, we measured the fitness, in the context of natural selection, of red-eyed females compared to brown-eyed females when they both competed for a limiting resource linked to reproductive success (live-yeast; see electronic Appendix A). A total of 20 replicate vials were assayed. In each replicate 16 females (8 red-eyed and 8 brown-eyed) were placed in an initial mating vial with 24 males (12 brown-eyed and 12 red-eyed) for two hours. Then, under light anesthesia, the number of males was reduced to 16 (8 each) and transferred with the 16 females to male-female interaction vials, 6

containing a limited amount (10 mg) of live-yeast. After the two days, females were transferred to individual oviposition vials for 18 hours, then removed. After twelve days all progeny were counted. We found that while there was no difference between the lifetime fecundity of brown-eyed females and red-eyed females (mean fecundity brown-eyed ± SE = 26.13 ± 1.21 vs. red-eyed = 26.71 ± 1.20; t paired = 0.493, d.f. 19, P = 0.627), there was a significant difference in mating success between brown-eyed and red-eyed males (mean proportion brown-eyed offspring ± SE = 0.682 ± 0.0151 vs. red-eyed = 0.318 ± 0.0151, where expected proportions are 0.75 vs. 0.25; t = - 4.516, d.f. = 15, P = 0.0004). We estimate that red-eyed males have a 27.2% competitive advantage over brown-eyed males. These data indicate the pleiotropic natural/sexual selection that we observed on the bw + allele was manifested through males but not through females. 7

Appendix E To determine if males can affect female fecundity in a non-direct manner (i.e., by eating or fouling the environment/live-yeast), we pretreated male-female interaction vials for 8 hours with either i) 16 males, ii) 16 females, or iii) no flies, and then removed them before the vials were used to culture 16 singly mated females in a manner otherwise identical to that used during the male-female interaction phase of the experiment. Sample sizes for each pretreatment were 12 vials of 16 females each. Average fecundities of females after feeding within the pretreated vials were (mean ± SE) i] 31.30 ± 0.71, ii] 21.86 ± 0.71, and iii] 31.16 ± 0.71, respectively. Pretreatment of the male-female interaction vials with 16 males had no measurable effect on female fecundity (no flies vs. 16 males, delta = -0.131 ± 1.003, t = -0.131, d.f. = 533, P = 0.896). In sharp contrast, pretreatment with 16 females produced a 30% reduction in the fecundity of females that subsequently fed in these vials (no flies vs. 16 females = 9.301 ± 1.002, t = 9.286, d.f. = 533, P < 0.000001). Interestingly, females eat all of the live-yeast within the first 24 hours, so the nearly one-third reduction in fecundity after an 8-hour exposure of live-yeast to competitor females is in accordance with expectations based upon the linear relationship between quantity of live-yeast and fecundity (see electronic Appendix A). 8