pii: jc

Similar documents
Drowsiness is an unstable, rapidly fluctuating state of reduced SCIENTIFIC INVESTIGATIONS

Light treatment for sleep disorders: consensus report. IV. Sleep phase and duration disturbances.

Daytime Exposure to Bright Light, as Compared to Dim Light, Decreases Sleepiness and Improves Psychomotor Vigilance Performance

Driver Sleepiness Assessed by Electroencephalography - Different Methods Applied to One Single Data Set

Fatigue and Circadian Rhythms

The Use of Bright Light in the Treatment of Insomnia

Circadian photoreception in humans: More than meets the eye

Understanding Sleep Regulatory Processes to Improve Waking Performance

Taking the Obesity and Diabetes Risk out of Light at Night

Sleep, Fatigue, and Performance. Gregory Belenky, M.D. Sleep and Performance Research Center

November 24, External Advisory Board Members:

BUS DRIVERS WORKING HOURS AND THE RELATIONSHIP TO DRIVER FATIGUE

Circadian and wake-dependent modulation of fastest and slowest reaction times during the psychomotor vigilance task

Rodney Heller LC, CLEP Senior Lighting Designer & Lighting Evangelist Energy Performance Lighting Cottage Grove, WI

Who s Not Sleepy at Night? Individual Factors Influencing Resistance to Drowsiness during Atypical Working Hours

Shift Work: An Occupational Health and Safety Hazard. Sandra Buxton, BA (Hons) This thesis is presented for the degree of Master of Philosophy

Fatigue Management Awareness

Sleep and Sleep Stages Regulation

Sleep Medicine 13 (2012) Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect. Sleep Medicine. journal homepage:

on EARTH! Restless Nights...

Scheduling of sleep/darkness affects the circadian phase of night shift workers

The Efficacy of a Restart Break for Recycling with Optimal Performance Depends Critically on Circadian Timing

Clinical Trial Synopsis TL , NCT#

PERCLOS: AN ALERTNESS MEASURE OF THE PAST. Circadian Technologies, Inc., Stoneham, Massachusetts, USA

COMPARISON OF WORKSHIFT PATTERNS ON FATIGUE AND SLEEP IN THE PETROCHEMICAL INDUSTRY

Sleep Deprivation, Fatigue and Effects on Performance The Science and Its Implications for Resident Duty Hours

Translating Fatigue Research into Technologic Countermeasures

Quantitative measurements of sleepiness

Circadian variation of EEG power spectra in NREM and REM sleep in humans: Dissociation from body temperature

Circadian period and the timing of melatonin onset in men and women: predictors of sleep during the weekend and in the laboratory

Introduction. What is Shiftwork. Normal Human Rhythm. What are the Health Effects of Shiftwork? Blue Light

DROWSY DRIVER MONITOR AND WARNING SYSTEM

Survey about Sleepiness and Adaptation to Night- Shift Workers in Metallurgy Industry

The Effects of a Short Daytime Nap After Restricted Night Sleep

The Relationships between Sleep-Wake Cycle and Academic Performance in Medical Students

Fatigue neurophysiology in professional and non-professional drivers Abstract Introduction Methods Subjects

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

A MEASURE OF STRONG DRIVER FATIGUE.

Adaptation of performance during a week of simulated night work

HSE information sheet. Guidance for managing shiftwork and fatigue offshore. Offshore Information Sheet No. 7/2008

The Hidden Dangers of Fatigue

Controlled Patterns of Daytime Light Exposure Improve Circadian Adjustment in Simulated Night Work

IN ITS ORIGINAL FORM, the Sleep/Wake Predictor

Understanding drivers motivation to take a break when tired

Comparison between subjective and actigraphic measurement of sleep and sleep rhythms

A Reliability Study of The Turkish Version of The Morningness-Eveningness Questionnaire

THE DANGERS OF DROWSY DRIVING. The Costs, Risks, and Prevention of Driver Fatigue

Relations Between Performance and Subjective Ratings of Sleepiness During a Night Awake

Diagnostic Accuracy of the Multivariable Apnea Prediction (MAP) Index as a Screening Tool for Obstructive Sleep Apnea

Hetlioz (tasimelteon)

Subjective sleepiness is a sensitive indicator of insufficient sleep and impaired waking function

Ageing and the circadian and homeostatic regulation of human sleep during forced desynchrony of rest, melatonin and temperature rhythms

Effects of a rotating-shift schedule on nurses vigilance as measured by the Psychomotor Vigilance Task

SLEEP DISORDERS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Division of Sleep Medicine; Department of Neurology Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

Dr Alex Bartle. Medical Director Sleep Well Clinic Christchurch

Evaluation of In-Car Countermeasures to Sleepiness: Cold Air and Radio

Comparing performance on a simulated 12 hour shift rotation in young and older subjects

Power Density in Theta/Alpha Frequencies of the Waking EEG Progressively Increases During Sustained Wakefulness

EVALUATION OF SLEEP IN INDIVIDUALS WITH GLAUCOMA By: Dana Bakir M.Sc. Candidate Supervised by: Dr. Olga Overbury and Dr.

Circadian Period and the Melatonin Rhythm in Men and Women: Predictors of Sleep during the Weekend and in the Laboratory

Virtual Mentor American Medical Association Journal of Ethics November 2009, Volume 11, Number 11:

Teenagers: Sleep Patterns and School Performance

Index. sleep.theclinics.com. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

September 26 28, 2013 Westin Tampa Harbour Island. Co-sponsored by

TOP 10 LIST OF SLEEP QUESTIONS. Kenneth C. Sassower, MD Sleep Disorders Unit Massachusetts General Hospital for Children

Definition 1: A fixed point iteration scheme to approximate the fixed point, p, of a function g, = for all n 1 given a starting approximation, p.

Facts about Sleep. Circadian rhythms are important in determining human sleep patterns/ sleep-waking cycle

A Novel Approach to Eliminating Jetlag Using Natural Ingredients

Effects of Shift and Night Work in the Offshore Petroleum Industry: A Systematic Review

AOS1 How do levels of consciousness affect mental processes and behaviour? An Overview

Sleep and Sleep Hygiene in an Occupational Health & Safety Context

Get on the Road to Better Health Recognizing the Dangers of Sleep Apnea

Fatigue Management. Sample Only

HUMAN FATIGUE RISK SIMULATIONS IN 24/7 OPERATIONS. Rainer Guttkuhn Udo Trutschel Anneke Heitmann Acacia Aguirre Martin Moore-Ede

Ultrashort Sleep-Wake Cycle: Timing of REM Sleep. Evidence for Sleep-Dependent and Sleep-Independent Components of the REM Cycle

A Pre-Screening Questionnaire to Predict Non-24-Hour Sleep-Wake Rhythm Disorder (N24HSWD) among the Blind

41 st Annual AAGBI Linkman Conference Birmingham. Dr Kathleen Ferguson Honorary Treasurer AAGBI

Shift Work, Sleep, Health, Safety, and Solutions. Prof Philippa Gander PhD, FRSNZ Sleep/Wake Research Centre Massey University

Slow eyelid closure as a measure of driver drowsiness and its relationship to performance

Overview. Surviving shift work. What is the circadian rhythm? Components of a Generic Biological Timing System 31/10/2017

Circadian Rhythms in Children and Adolescents

Priorities in Occupation Health and Safety: Fatigue. Assoc. Prof. Philippa Gander, PhD Director, Sleep/Wake Research Centre

Fatigue at sea Lützhöft, M., Thorslund, B., Kircher, A., Gillberg, M.

Driver Alertness Detection Research Using Capacitive Sensor Array

Figure 1. Source localization results for the No Go N2 component. (a) Dipole modeling

Managing Sleep to Sustain Performance

Section 53 FATIGUE MANAGEMENT

Effects of caffeine on alertness as measured by infrared reflectance oculography

FATIGUE MANAGEMENT & MITIGATION

Does Exogenous Melatonin Improve Adaptation to Night Shift Work in Residents?

Are Students with a Negative Impression on Shift Work Morning-Typed?

MORNINGNESS-EVENINGNES05S QUESTIONNAIRE Self-Assessment Version (MEQ-SA) 1. Name: Date:

MICROSLEEP EPISODES AND RELATED CRASHES DURING OVERNIGHT DRIVING SIMULATIONS

Circadian phase delay using the newly developed re-timer portable light device

Prevention of drowsy driving by means of warning sound

Most people need to sleep about 8 hours each night. This is especially true for college students, since the deep sleep that occurs early in the night

CHAPTER. Summary, Conclusion and Recommendations

Circadian Rhythms and Sleep. Rapid Shift in Sleep Time and Acrophase of Melatonin Secretion in Short Shift Work Schedule

Sleep and Body Temperature in "Morning" and "Evening" People

Transcription:

SCIENTIFIC INVESTIGATIONS pii: jc-00268-14 http://dx.doi.org/10.5664/jcsm.5086 Ocular Measures of Sleepiness Are Increased in Night Shift Workers Undergoing a Simulated Night Shift Near the Peak Time of the 6-Sulfatoxymelatonin Rhythm Suzanne Ftouni, PhD 1 ; Tracey L. Sletten, PhD 1 ; Christian L. Nicholas, PhD 2 ; David J. Kennaway, PhD 3 ; Steven W. Lockley, PhD 1,4,5 ; Shantha M.W. Rajaratnam, PhD 1,4,5 1 School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Clayton, Victoria, Australia; 2 Sleep Research Laboratory, Melbourne School of Psychological Sciences, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Victoria, Australia; 3 Robinson Research Institute, School of Paediatrics and Reproductive Health, University of Adelaide, Adelaide, South Australia, Australia; 4 Division of Sleep and Circadian Disorders, Departments of Medicine and Neurology, Brigham and Women s Hospital, Boston, Massachusetts; 5 Division of Sleep Medicine, Department of Medicine, Harvard Medical School, Boston, Massachusetts Study Objectives: The study examined the relationship between the circadian rhythm of 6-sulphatoxymelatonin (amt6s) and ocular measures of sleepiness and neurobehavioral performance in shift workers undergoing a simulated night shift. Methods: Twenty-two shift workers (mean age 33.4, SD 11.8 years) were tested at approximately the beginning (20:00) and the end (05:55) of a simulated night shift in the laboratory. At the time point corresponding to the end of the simulated shift, 14 participants were classified as being within range of 6-sulphatoxymelatonin (amt6s) acrophase defined as 3 hours before or after amt6s peak and 8 were classified as outside amt6s acrophase range. Participants completed the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS) and the auditory psychomotor vigilance task (apvt). Waking electroencephalography (EEG) was recorded and infrared reflectance oculography was used to collect ocular measures of sleepiness: positive and negative amplitude/velocity ratio (PosAVR, NegAVR), mean blink total duration (BTD), the Shift work has become a ubiquitous part of labor practices due to the increasing pressure for 24-hour provision of goods and services. This drive for round-the-clock productivity, however, has resulted in negative consequences to health, safety, and performance. 1 Such consequences have been welldocumented in a number of occupational sectors in which peak functioning during the night shift is critical, for example, commercial transport, mining, emergency services, and health care. 2 The impairment to alertness that occurs during night shift work is mainly attributed to misalignment of the imposed sleep-wake cycle from the internally generated circadian (~24-hour) rhythm of sleepiness, 3 often against a background of chronic sleep deficiency and prolonged periods of wakefulness. Alertness and neurobehavioral performance peak when the circadian pacemaker promotes wakefulness during the daytime, and decline markedly during the night, particularly between 03:00 and 06:00, 4 as the circadian pacemaker strongly promotes sleep. 5 The deleterious effect of the circadian peak in sleep propensity occurring during the night shift is further percentage of eye closure (%TEC), and a composite score of sleepiness levels (Johns Drowsiness Scale; JDS). Results: Participants who were tested within amt6s acrophase range displayed higher levels of sleepiness on ocular measures (%TEC, BTD, PosAVR, JDS), objective sleepiness (EEG delta power frequency band), subjective ratings of sleepiness, and neurobehavioral performance, compared to those who were outside amt6s acrophase range. Conclusions: The study demonstrated that objective ocular measures of sleepiness are sensitive to circadian rhythm misalignment in shift workers. Keywords: oculometrics, circadian misalignment, shift work, performance, sleepiness Citation: Ftouni S, Sletten TL, Nicholas CL, Kennaway DJ, Lockley SW, Rajaratnam SM. Ocular measures of sleepiness are increased in night shift workers undergoing a simulated night shift near the peak time of the 6-sulfatoxymelatonin rhythm. J Clin Sleep Med 2015;11(10):1131 1141. BRIEF SUMMARY Current Knowledge/Study Rationale: The research was conducted to examine the relationship between circadian phase and ocular measures of sleepiness and neurobehavioral performance in a sample of shift workers. Study Impact: The research demonstrates that ocular measures of sleepiness are significantly elevated when shift workers are tested near the peak of the urinary melatonin metabolite (6-sulphatoxymelatonin; amt6s) rhythm, compared to when tested outside the amt6s peak range. These findings suggest that ocular measures of sleepiness may support alertness monitoring in shift workers. exacerbated by accumulated (homeostatic) sleep pressure that results from poor sleep quality and inadequate sleep duration. These are caused by the sleep episode occurring at an inappropriate circadian phase, 6 and can often prolong the duration of the wake episode. The interaction between circadian and homeostatic factors, particularly at the end of a night shift, amplifies the risk of accidents and injuries. 7 Increased sleepiness at the end of a night shift, and the consequent increase in 1131 Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015

S Ftouni, TL Sletten, CL Nicholas et al. attentional lapses and likelihood of falling asleep, 8 is a major risk factor for motor vehicle crashes. 8,9 The risk of motor vehicle crash is substantially elevated between 02:00 and 07:00. 9 Sleepiness monitoring systems are often recommended as a component of comprehensive Fatigue Risk Management Systems, 10 although validation studies are limited. The most common technologies assessing sleepiness in real-time utilize ocular metrics. Recent studies have assessed the efficacy of ocular metrics as practical tools in providing accurate, realtime, continuous assessment of alertness level. 11,12 Ocular measures, such as blink durations, are associated with the onset of sleep or microsleeps, and are observed in sleep-deprived individuals. 13 Such blinks exhibit unique properties, including slowing of the closing and reopening phases of a blink, which reflect the sleep/wake-related changes by the central nervous system (CNS). 13 These ocular parameters have also been found to be sensitive to variations in circadian phase and increased homeostatic sleepiness over time, 11 suggesting potential utility as a real-time sleepiness monitoring system in occupational and on-road settings. Hence, the current study employed the use of an ocular-based sleepiness detection system (Optalert) to assess sleepiness levels in a shift working population during a simulated night shift, following a series of night shifts worked in the real world. The study aimed to examine the relationship between ocular measures of sleepiness and circadian phase in shift workers. Specifically, sleepiness and neurobehavioral performance were examined in those who were within range of the acrophase of the urinary metabolite of melatonin 6-sulphatoxymelatonin (amt6s) at the end of the simulated night shift, compared to those who outside amt6s acrophase range. METHODS Ethical Approval The study was approved by the Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee. All participants provided written informed consent. The data presented are part of a larger, multicenter study examining the effects of a novel light intervention on alertness and neurobehavioral performance in night shift workers. Participants and Pre-Study Conditions A total of 27 healthy shift workers (9M, 18F) aged 31.8 ± 11.2 years (mean ± standard deviation [SD], range 18 64) completed the study at the Monash University Sleep and Circadian Medicine Laboratory. Participants were required to be regular night shift workers, working 5 night shifts per month including 2 consecutive night shifts. Night shifts were defined as shifts with 6 h on duty between 22:00 and 08:00, and with a maximum scheduled shift length of 12 hours. All participants were initially screened for use of medications or drugs of abuse, and any history of medical or psychiatric disorders. Participants were screened for sleep apnea using the Berlin Questionnaire 14 and the Multivariate Apnea Prediction (MAP) Questionnaire. 15 Participants were deemed as high risk of sleep apnea if they screened positive on 2 categories on the Berlin Questionnaire and positive on the MAP Apnea Index. Participants were ineligible if they reported transmeridian travel in the past month. Participants completed a number of subjective questionnaires including the Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) 16 ; Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index (PSQI) 17 ; and the Morningness/Eveningness Questionnaire (MEQ). 18 The laboratory-controlled simulated night shift was scheduled immediately following a sequence of 2 consecutive night shifts of each individual s normal shift work schedule. For 1 to 3 weeks before entering the laboratory for the simulated night shift, participants completed a daily sleep/work hours log and wore a wrist actigraphy device (Actiwatch-L, Mini-Mitter, Inc., Bend, OR). Sleep diary data were used to define the time in bed interval for actigraphic sleep analysis, whereby, if self-reported bedtime was 30 min prior to or after a sustained substantial reduction in activity, then bedtime was modified to the start of the sustained substantial reduction in activity; if self-reported wake time was 30 min prior to or after a sustained substantial increase in activity, then wake time was modified to the start of the sustained substantial increase in activity. Participants were asked to refrain from use of any prescription or non-prescription medications throughout the pre-study monitoring and simulated shift, and abstain from any recreational drugs for at least a month prior to the study. Participants were also required to abstain from alcohol (at least 24 h), caffeine and nicotine (12 h) prior to admittance to the sleep laboratory. Compliance was verified by urine toxicology upon admission to the laboratory. For 48 h prior to the beginning of the simulated night shift, participants collected approximately 4-hourly sequential urine samples (8-hourly during sleep episodes) for measurement of the major urinary metabolite of melatonin, 6-sulphatoxymelatonin (amt6s). The final urine sample was taken in the laboratory at the beginning of the simulated night shift. After each collection, participants recorded the volume of the sample and the time of urine collection. A 5-mL urine aliquot was frozen ( 20 C) and subsequently analyzed by amt6s radioimmunoassay 19 at the Adelaide Research Assay Facility, University of Adelaide using reagents purchased from Stockgrand, Ltd., University of Surrey (Guildford, UK). The amt6s intra-assay coefficient of variation (CV) was 7.2% and the inter-assay CVs were 23%, 7%, and 11% at 3.6 ng/ml, 15.6 ng/ml, and 29.6 ng/ml, respectively. The minimum detectable concentration was 0.5 ng/ml. Participants were asked to continue their own schedule in the 48 h prior to the shift, and no instructions were given to restrict light exposure. Shift work occupation categories 20 were as follows; health professionals (n = 7, 26%); machine and stationary plant operators (4, 18%); hospitality workers (3, 11%); hospitality, retail and service managers (3, 11%); business, human resource and marketing professionals (2, 9%); other (5, 18%); 3 (14%) participants occupations were unknown. Simulated Night-Shift Protocol The simulated night shift occurred in a sleep laboratory free of time cues: participants had no access to windows, clocks, television, radio, internet, or telephone, and were continually supervised by trained staff. Participants were instructed to arrive at the laboratory at 17:30. Testing began at 19:00 and Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015 1132

Ocular Measures of Sleepiness Assessed in Night Shift Workers Table 1 Ocular measures derived from Optalert sleepiness technology. Johns Drowsiness Scale Score (JDS) JDS is a composite score of ocular measures used to calculate an individual s level of sleepiness in real time, providing a minute-to-minute Johns Drowsiness Scale (JDS) rating (see Johns et al. 21 ). JDS is a continuous scale with scores ranging from 0 to 10 (very alert to very drowsy, respectively). The commercially available system is designed to emit auditory warnings when individuals reach a JDS score of 4.5 to 4.9 (cautionary level of sleepiness), and a score of 5.0 or above (critical level of sleepiness), which is associated with an increased risk of severe lane excursions on a driving simulator. 21,60,61 Positive Amplitude/Velocity Ratio (PosAVR) Ratio of the maximum amplitude to the maximum velocity of the closing phase of a blink. 21 Percentage of time with eyes closed (%TEC) Percentage of time that the eyes are deemed closed in each minute. The eyes are deemed closed when the velocity of the eyelid movement following the closing of the eyelid drops below the velocity threshold, and is deemed closed until the velocity increases back above this threshold indicating the beginning of the re-opening of the eyelids. Negative Amplitude/Velocity Ratio (NegAVR) Ratio of the maximum amplitude to the maximum velocity of the re-opening phase of a blink. 21 Blink total duration (BTD; seconds) The mean total blink duration of all blinks over the course of 1 min. Total blink duration includes the duration of the closing, closed, and re-opening phases of each blink. ended at 08:15 the following day, and participants were required to remain awake while supervised. Between 19:00 and 23:00, and from 07:00 to 08:15, participants were exposed to standard room lighting at eye level (vertical plane). Between 23:00 and 07:00, participants were randomly allocated to 1 of 2 fluorescent lighting conditions: 4,000 K or 17,000 K (Philips Bright Light Devices, HF3305, The Netherlands). Lighting illuminance levels were maintained between 100 to 150 lux across the simulated shift. Only data collected from the 4,000 K light group are reported herein. For the duration of the simulated shift participants were asked to remain seated in the center of the room in front of a computer which was used to administer each test battery, except for scheduled brief toilet breaks provided every 2 hours. Participants completed hourly cognitive test batteries from 19:00 to 01:00, a test at 03:00, and then hourly tests from 05:00 to 08:00. In between test batteries participants were permitted to read or watch selected movies. Data presented here were collected during the beginning (20:00) and end (05:55) of the simulated night shift. Infrared Oculography Participants eye and eyelid movements were monitored by infrared reflectance (IR) oculography (Optalert) 21 to record drowsiness levels continuously during the simulated night shift. IR transducers fitted to spectacle frames were positioned towards the top eye lid to measure the relative velocity of the opening and closing phase of the eye lid, and duration of each blink. Optalert glasses can be worn with contact lenses, and also support prescription spectacles. However, none of the participants in our study required prescription lenses. The system provides 13 ocular measures sampled each minute, of which the following were selected based on previous work 21 27 (detailed description of each measure provided in Table 1): positive and negative amplitude/velocity ratio of each blink (PosAVR, NegAVR), Johns Drowsiness Scale (JDS) score, the percentage of time with eyes closed (%TEC), and mean blink total durations (BTD). For all measures, higher values indicate higher levels of sleepiness. The commercially available system is designed to emit auditory warnings; however, these warnings were disabled in the present research study, such that the system was used purely to monitor alertness state. Sleepiness and Performance Assessments Subjective sleepiness was rated using the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS; 9-point scale from 1 very alert to 9 very sleepy, fighting sleep ) 28 by pressing the appropriate number on a computer keyboard when prompted. Sustained attention was assessed using a 10-min auditory psychomotor vigilance task (apvt). 29,30 An auditory stimulus was presented at random intervals (1 9 sec); the participant was asked to respond to the sound by pressing a button as quickly as possible. No simultaneous visual stimulus was presented. The auditory version of the PVT was used as it was more appropriate for administration during a period of light exposure within the larger protocol (data not included here), and to maintain consistency with our previous studies. 12 The apvt is a validated measure of sustained attention and a reliable index of objective sleepiness 30,31 and correlates with visual PVT during sleep deprivation. 30 Waking EEG Recordings EEG and electro-oculogram (EOG) recordings were made continuously throughout the simulated night shift using a portable, wireless, ambulatory, polysomnographic recorder (Siesta, Compumedics Ltd., Victoria, Australia). EEG electrodes were positioned according to the International 10/20 System and recorded at 8 sites (F3, F4, C3, C4, P3, P4, O1, and O2) against a Cz reference and subsequently digitally re-referenced to linked mastoids (M1 and M2). Only data from C3 are reported here. EOG was recorded with electrodes placed 2 cm above and below lateral canthus of the left and right eye, respectively. EEG and EOG data were sampled at 512 Hz and electrode impedances were < 10 kω. Raw EEG time series were imported as European Data Format (EDF) files into Curry 7 software (Compumedics Neuroscan, Victoria, Australia) and were digitally 1133 Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015

S Ftouni, TL Sletten, CL Nicholas et al. band-pass filtered (0.3 30 Hz, 24 db/octave). EEG data were sampled during the 3-min Karolinska Drowsiness Test (KDT), 28 during which participants were instructed to fixate on a 5-cm black dot on a computer screen and remain as still as possible. Data Analysis Cosinor analysis of amt6s values were used to determine acrophase time or peak of amt6s. 32 Participants were classified into groups based on whether they were within range of the amt6s acrophase, which coincides approximately with the peak in the circadian rhythm of sleep propensity. 33 The range of amt6s acrophase was defined based on a review by Arendt 34 describing the biological night as the hours between amt6s onset and offset and refined based on Lockley et al., 35 who reported the mean and 2-SD range of normal amt6s acrophases as 4.2 ± 2.9 h. Therefore, in the current study, participants were classified as being within amt6s acrophase range if the final testing session 05:55 occurred ± 3 h from the time of their amt6s acrophase as measured in the 24 h prior to entering the laboratory. The conservative estimate used here is aimed at isolating the 6-h time interval in which sleepiness is expected to be highest. For apvt measures, reaction times < 100 ms and > 10,000 ms were excluded as false starts and distractions, respectively. 30,36 Mean apvt reaction time (RT) and number of lapses (responses > 500 ms) for each 10-min test were calculated. apvt RT scores were transformed with an inverse (1/ RT) function, and square root was applied to apvt lapses. 37 Ten-minute recordings of each ocular measure were extracted coincident with the 10-min apvt. Ocular measures recorded during apvt were selected for analysis to reduce variance across participants, as they were instructed to remain still with eyes directed at the screen ahead and concentrate on the task. Ocular measures recorded during the KDT could not be used in the analysis as participants were instructed to blink as little as possible during the 3-min task. As ocular measures are derived from eye blinks, ocular measures recorded during this task may be an inaccurate representation of the participants sleepiness level. Ocular data were cleaned based on signal quality 11 ; data were retained when > 50% of each 1-min epoch displayed a clear blink signal. Waking EEG signals were derived from C3 referenced to linked mastoids during each KDT. ECG artifacts were removed using subtraction of a single component derived from a principal components analysis of an average ECG artifact across all electrodes. Two-second epochs containing muscle or eye blink artifacts were discarded from further analysis (< 5% of all epochs). Artifact-free 2-sec epochs were subjected to offline spectral analysis using a fast-fourier transformation and a 5% Hanning window (Curry 7 software, Compumedics Neuroscan, Victoria, Australia). Power spectral data were analyzed between 0.5 and 30 Hz to remove any high and low frequency artifacts with outliers greater/less than two standard deviations from the mean being removed. EEG power values were summed across consecutive frequency bins (0.5 Hz resolution) within 4 frequency bands (delta [0.5 4.0 Hz], theta [4.1 8.0 Hz], alpha [8.1 12.0 Hz], and beta [14.1 30 Hz]). Summed frequency band values were then averaged across 2-sec epochs within each KDT for final analysis. IBM SPSS Statistics 20.0 (IBM Corp., Somers, NY, USA) was used for statistical analysis. A mixed design ANCOVA was conducted to assess the interaction of circadian group (participants ± 3 h of amt6s acrophase range and those who were tested outside amt6s acrophase range) across 2 time points (baseline 20:00, and end of shift 05:55) on participants ocular measures, apvt performance, subjective sleepiness, and EEG. Participants age, body mass index (BMI), and time in bed (TIB) in the 24 h prior to the simulated shift were used as a priori covariates in the analysis: age-related changes in the timing of circadian drive have been found 38 ; BMI increases the risk of obstructive sleep apnea 39 ; and time in bed in the past 24 h was included to control for variation in homeostatic sleep pressure while assessing differences in circadian phase. To confirm that the assumption of equal variance was not violated, Levene test for Equality of Variances was set at > 0.05 for all analyses. RESULTS Data Retention Five of 27 participants had poor quality amt6s rhythm as determined by cosinor analysis of the 24-h rhythms across the 48-h collection period and visual inspection of the consistency in the pattern of the 24-h rhythm; data were consequently excluded from analyses. The remaining 22 participants exhibited a significant cosinor fit (α set 0.10 40 ; 91% were p < 0.05). As expected, no participants were within amt6s acrophase range at the baseline testing session (Figure 1). The end-shift testing session occurred ± 3 h of amt6s acrophase for 14/22 participants (hours relative to amt6s acrophase: mean = 0.17 ± SD 0.59 h; range = 2.83 to 2.73 h), while 8 participants were tested outside of this range (hours relative to amt6s acrophase: mean = 6.08 ± SD 3.00 h; range = 17.92 to 5.35 h). Of the 22 participants with amt6s data, one had insufficient ocular data at the baseline test due to poor signal quality, and the 21:00 test was subsequently substituted as the baseline. Three additional participants had missing ocular data at the 05:55 scheduled test and therefore data from the 05:00 test were substituted. A further 6 participants were excluded from ocular analyses; one participant had a weak signal throughout testing and, as a result, an insufficient amount of blinks were recorded; 2 participants did not have sufficient baseline ocular data for comparison; and 3 participants had a large amount of noise at all time points causing blinks to be unclear and data unreliable. A total of 16 participants were therefore included in the ocular analyses (within amt6s acrophase range, n = 9; outside amt6s acrophase range, n = 7). One participant did not have EEG recorded during the 20:00 scheduled test; EEG data during the next KDT (21:00) test was substituted as the baseline time point for all measures. One participant did not have sufficient EEG data collected throughout simulated shift KDTs and thus excluded from EEG analyses. Overall, data during the baseline test occurred at a mean time of 20:12 (SD ± 00:18 h), while the mean end-of-shift test occurred at 05:50 (SD ± 00:19 h). Participants work shifts prior to the simulated shift started on average at 21:03 (SD ± 01:50 h) and ended at 06:08 (SD ± 01:16 h). Table 2 displays participant details. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015 1134

Ocular Measures of Sleepiness Assessed in Night Shift Workers Figure 1 Relationships between testing times and circadian phase in each participant. Melatonin urinary metabolite amt6s acrophase of each participant (open diamond symbols) overlaid with start of shift baseline test times (open circle symbols), and end of shift test time: those tested outside amt6s acrophase range (open square symbols) and those tested ± 3 hours of amt6s acrophase range (closed square symbols). Error bars displayed represent ± 3 hours from the time of amt6s acrophase, which according to previous work 34,35 is the expected time of the circadian nadir of alertness. Ocular Measures and Sleepiness All ocular measures (%TEC, BTD, PosAVR, JDS) except NegAVR displayed a significant time by circadian group interaction (see Table 3). At the completion of the simulated shift, the mean %TEC values of participants ± 3 h of amt6s acrophase range increased more than 10-fold from 0.73% (SD ± 1.0) to 8.01% (± 1.8) (Figure 2; Figure S1, supplemental material), whereas participants outside amt6s acrophase range had only a minor increase from baseline to the end-of-shift (F 1,11 = 6.00, p = 0.032). JDS scores of participants ± 3 h of amt6s acrophase range significantly increased to nearly double their baseline value, from 2.5 (± 0.7) to 4.9 (± 0.6), at the end-of-shift in comparison to baseline, while participants outside amt6s acrophase range displayed only a slight increase across time points (F 1,11 = 7.50, p = 0.019). A similar result was observed in BTD between the 2 groups (F 1,11 = 9.86, p = 0.009). A significant increase in PosAVR values from baseline levels was also observed in participants ± 3 h of amt6s acrophase range (F 1,11 = 20.36, p = 0.001). NegAVR did not show significant interaction or main effects. Performance and Subjective Sleepiness KSS displayed a significant time by circadian group interaction (F 1,17 = 14.74, p = 0.001; Figure 3). Mean subjective sleepiness scores of participants ± 3 h of amt6s acrophase range increased from 4.3 (SD ± 0.6) to 7.6 (± 0.5) across the simulated shift, while subjective sleepiness of participants who were outside amt6s acrophase range decreased marginally at the end of the shift from 5.2 (± 0.8) to 5.0 (± 0.7). Similar results were observed in the apvt RT scores, which showed a significant time by circadian group interaction (F 1,17 = 7.86, p = 0.012; Figure 3; Figure S1). RT scores decreased over time in participants tested outside the amt6s acrophase range, while RT scores increased at the end of the simulated shift for participants who were tested within amt6s acrophase range. apvt lapses followed a similar trend (F 1,17 = 3.49, p = 0.079). EEG Assessment of Alertness Significant interaction of time and circadian group was detected only in the EEG delta frequency band (0.5 4.0 Hz) (F 1,13 = 4.77, p = 0.048). Participants within amt6s acrophase range showed an increase in the delta frequency from baseline to the end-of-shift test, compared to those outside amt6s acrophase range (Figure 4). DISCUSSION This study demonstrated that ocular measures of sleepiness (%TEC, BTD, PosAVR, JDS) show predictable increases across a simulated night shift when shift workers are assessed ± 3 hours of amt6s acrophase range, compared to those outside the amt6s acrophase range. Similar results were observed in measures of subjective sleepiness (KSS), objective sleepiness (EEG delta activity), and attentional performance (apvt). We observed considerable inter-individual variability in circadian phase, as assessed by the timing of the peak in the amt6s rhythm; the end-of-shift assessment of sleepiness and 1135 Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015

S Ftouni, TL Sletten, CL Nicholas et al. Table 2 Participant descriptives. End of Shift Test ± 3 h of amt6s Acrophase End of Shift Test Outside ± 3 h of amt6s Acrophase p n % Mean ± SD n % Mean ± SD Demographics n 14 8 Age 32.1 13.5 35.6 8.5 0.52 Sex (M, F) 9, 5 6, 2 BMI 23.7 3.2 26.9 1.5 0.02 Sleep-wake ESS 7.7 4.4 11.4 4.4 0.07 10 4 28.6 4 50.0 PSQI 5.9 2.5 7.4 1.6 0.11 5 8 57.1 7 87.5 MEQ 53.0 8.6 45.1 11.2 0.08 Sleep History Wake time (24 hours prior; h) 13:55 01:56 13:46 01:52 0.87 TIB 24 h (h) 6.6 1.3 6.3 1.2 0.61 Mean TIB 7 days (h) 6.3 1.6 6.7 1.9 0.37 Sleep efficiency 24 h (%) 79.5 9.8 82.6 5.0 0.42 Mean sleep efficiency 7 days (%) 77.6 7.5 74.4 10.0 0.43 Hours awake at baseline (h) 6.3 2.0 6.6 1.8 0.70 Work schedule history Mean shift start time (prior to simulated shift; h) 21:10 01:53 20:52 01:51 0.72 Mean shift end time (prior to simulated shift; h) 06:05 01:21 06:13 01:13 0.82 Number of consecutive night shifts prior 3.3 1.7 4.6 2.2 0.13 Total hours worked 30.7 16.3 44.7 23.6 0.12 Sleep history was based on sleep logs and verified with actigraphy. No participants napped (< 1 h) in the 24 h prior to the simulated night shift. BMI, body mass index; ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale (range 0 24; higher scores indicate greater state-sleepiness); PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Inventory (range 0 21; higher scores are indicative of greater sleep difficulty); MEQ, Morningness/Eveningness Questionnaire (range 16 86; higher scores indicate more morning chronotype); h, hours; TIB, time in bed; SD, standard deviation. Bold type, p < 0.05. Table 3 Table of analyses. Group Time Group Time n F df p Partial η 2 F df p Partial η 2 F df p Partial η 2 Ocular measures JDS 9, 7 0.658 1,11 0.435 0.056 0.689 1,11 0.424 0.059 7.504 1,11 0.019 0.406 PosAVR 9, 7 12.051 1,11 0.005 0.523 3.359 1,11 0.094 0.234 20.355 1,11 0.001 0.649 %TEC 9, 7 0.760 1,11 0.402 0.065 0.008 1,11 0.928 0.001 6.001 1,11 0.032 0.353 NegAVR 9, 7 0.704 1,11 0.419 0.060 0.274 1,11 0.611 0.024 3.013 1,11 0.110 0.215 BTD 9, 7 2.251 1,11 0.162 0.170 0.314 1,11 0.587 0.028 9.857 1,11 0.009 0.437 Neurobehavioral performance PVT RT 14, 8 0.014 1,17 0.908 0.001 0.247 1,17 0.625 0.014 7.855 1,17 0.012 0.316 PVT Lapses 14, 8 0.344 1,17 0.565 0.020 0.989 1,17 0.334 0.055 3.494 1,17 0.079 0.170 Subjective Sleepiness KSS 14, 8 0.821 1,17 0.378 0.046 8.849 1,17 0.008 0.342 14.741 1,17 0.001 0.464 EEG measures Delta power 12, 6 0.366 1,13 0.555 0.027 1.930 1,13 0.188 0.129 4.766 1,13 0.048 0.268 Theta power 12, 8 0.073 1,15 0.791 0.005 0.759 1,15 0.397 0.048 0.059 1,15 0.811 0.004 Alpha power 13, 6 0.008 1,14 0.931 0.001 0.417 1,14 0.529 0.029 1.756 1,14 0.206 0.111 Beta power 12, 7 0.141 1,14 0.713 0.010 1.343 1,14 0.266 0.088 0.209 1,14 0.655 0.015 n = tested within MT6s acrophase range, tested outside amt6s acrophase range; participants age, body mass index, and time in bed in the 24 hours prior to the simulated shift were used as a priori covariates in the analysis. Bold type, p < 0.05. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015 1136

Ocular Measures of Sleepiness Assessed in Night Shift Workers Figure 2 Ocular measures assessed across a simulated night shift, between circadian groups. Circadian group was categorized based on whether each participant was tested ± 3 hours of the melatonin urinary metabolite (amt6s) acrophase. (A) Mean scores at baseline 20:00 time point, and end of simulated shift time point 05:55. (B) Mean change from baseline scores. Asterisks indicate significant interaction (p < 0.05). Error bars represent standard deviation. JDS, Johns Drowsiness Scale; PosAVR; positive amplitude/velocity ratio; TEC%, percentage of time with eyes closed; NegAVR, negative amplitude/velocity ratio; BTD, blink total duration. 1137 Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015

S Ftouni, TL Sletten, CL Nicholas et al. Figure 3 Self-rated sleepiness and auditory PVT (apvt) assessed across simulated night shift, between circadian groups. Circadian group was categorized based on whether each participant was tested ± 3 hours of the melatonin urinary metabolite (amt6s) acrophase. (A) Mean scores at baseline 20:00 time point, and end of simulated shift time point 05:55. (B) Mean change from baseline scores. Error bars represent standard deviation. Asterisks indicate significant interaction (p < 0.05). KSS, Karolinska Sleepiness Score; apvt, auditory psychomotor vigilance task; RT, reaction time. performance occurred at a time ranging from 7.2 h before to 6.0 h after the amt6s peak. This variability in circadian phase across the simulated night shift may be the result of behavioral, biological, and/or environmental influences. Participants had worked a variable number of night shifts before entering the laboratory, therefore resulting in variability in sleep-wake history and light-dark exposure. We did not, however, observe significant differences in shift or sleep-wake history between participants in the two circadian phase groups, or between shift workers with an earlier phase (i.e., amt6s acrophase before end of shift test; n = 10) versus those with a later phase (i.e., amt6s acrophase after end of shift test; n = 12; p > 0.05), suggesting that these factors are unlikely to account for the observed differences in circadian phase. Measures of blink duration (BTD), amplitude/velocity ratio (PosAVR), percentage of time with eyes closed (%TEC), and a composite measure of these variables (JDS) displayed larger increases in sleepiness across the simulated night shift in those who were tested ± 3 hours of amt6s acrophase range, compared to those tested outside this range. The increased rate of accumulation of sleepiness across the night shift in those tested within amt6s acrophase range likely reflects the interactive effects of circadian and homeostatic influences on sleepiness. For example, sleep efficiency was relatively low in our participants, most likely because sleep is scheduled during the daytime when the circadian pacemaker is promoting wakefulness, 41 thus resulting in chronic sleep deficiency. Ocular metrics have previously demonstrated their sensitivity to the fluctuations in sleepiness due to homeostatic and circadian processes. 11,23 In line with the oculometric measures of sleepiness, subjective sleepiness (KSS), neurobehavioral impairment (apvt), and objective sleepiness (EEG) also increased at the end of the simulated night shift in participants ± 3 hours of amt6s Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015 1138

Ocular Measures of Sleepiness Assessed in Night Shift Workers acrophase range. All EEG power frequency bands showed a trend towards increasing across the simulated night shift in both circadian groups, suggesting overall general accumulation of sleepiness across the shift in all shift workers. However, participants who ended their shift within the range of amt6s acrophase displayed significantly higher delta power compared to those who were outside amt6s acrophase range. This suggests a higher sleep propensity when tested around the amt6s peak, over and above the effects of elevated homeostatic sleep pressure caused by chronic sleep deficiency and prolonged wakefulness. Similar findings were observed in a study by Torsvall and Åkerstedt, 42 who assessed subjective and objective sleepiness in night working train drivers. The study showed more rapid increase in self-reported sleepiness and EEG alpha, theta, and delta activity during the night journey. 42 The current study extended these findings by showing that working near the amt6s peak substantially influences the degree of sleepiness observed at the end of a night shift. The findings of the study provide further evidence of the utility of oculometrics as objective measures of sleepiness in shift workers. It is acknowledged, however, that the assessment of neurobehavioral impairment (apvt) occurred under controlled laboratory conditions and hence the extent to which findings may be extrapolated to field settings is unknown. It should be noted that 18% (5/27) of the initial sample had missing phase data. Furthermore, six of the 22 participants with circadian data (59% of the initial sample) had ocular data deemed to be of poor quality and were excluded, further limiting the sample size. It should be noted, however, that while the collection procedure meant that ocular measures were sometimes limited by signal quality, the measures themselves remain sensitive to circadian and homeostatic variations in alertness. 11,12,27 Careful consideration should be given to how oculometric data are collected in future studies. Nonetheless, several aspects of the study were ecologically valid. First, night shift workers were recruited to the study, and assessed in this protocol following a sequence of night shifts in the real-world. Second, the oculographic system (Optalert) we utilized is marketed and increasingly used as a technology to support fatigue risk management in occupational settings. A major limitation in the assessment of driver sleepiness is the lack of widely accepted, real-time methods. Measures that rely on offline processing or repeated neurocognitive testing are not practical in this context. While subjective assessments of sleepiness have been found to be accurate in the laboratory, their relationship to objective sleepiness measures has not been shown reliably in applied settings. 43,44 Hence, selfratings of sleepiness have limited value in the workplace compared to objective sleepiness measures, which do not rely upon the accuracy of an individual s introspection. In-vehicle sleepiness-detection systems provide the driver with immediate notification of their current alertness state and risk of performance impairment and attentional failure in the future, potentially preventing sleepiness-related motor vehicle crashes. The present study adds to the growing literature showing the utility of oculometrics in sleepiness measurement technologies, specifically by showing the sensitivity of these metrics to reduced alertness due to the circadian nadir. amt6s profiles in the field may be impacted by light suppression of melatonin synthesis. While this may be a potential Figure 4 Change EEG power from baseline test to end of shift test between circadian groups. Circadian group was categorized based on whether each participant was tested ± 3 hours of the melatonin urinary metabolite (amt6s) acrophase (mean ± standard deviation). EEG was grouped into the following frequency bands: delta (0.5 4.0 Hz), theta (4.1 8.0 Hz), alpha (8.1 12.0 Hz), and beta (14.1 30 Hz). Asterisks indicate significant interaction (p < 0.05). Error bars represent standard deviation. limitation to the research, the method is well-established in the literature as a suitable real-world assessment of circadian phase, of which the techniques and protocols used here have been applied successfully in several prior studies of shift workers on North Sea oil rigs, 45 49 in shift working nurses, 50 52 in shift workers living in Antarctica, 53,54 and in many clinical populations and experimental protocols. 35,55 58 To assess the potential impact of light on amt6s, in a previous study we compared amt6s with urinary cortisol (which is much less confounded by light) in a subset of sighted individuals (n = 7) and found a strong relationship between the phase estimates (average Pearson r ± SD, 0.98 ± 0.01, p < 0.05). 40 The study is limited by the short time lag between the estimation of phase and performance assessment in the laboratory (up to 24 hours). The findings of the study highlight the importance of effective alertness management strategies during the night shift work and the drive home at around the circadian nadir. Assessing whether or not an individual is working near the circadian peak during a night shift appears to be an important component of safety risk management. Crowley et al. found that performance, sleepiness, and mood are improved in participants who show circadian entrainment to a simulated night shift, compared to those who do not show entrainment, and thus are working at an adverse circadian phase. The authors reported significant associations between the magnitude of circadian entrainment and performance levels. 59 The current study supports the findings of Crowley and colleagues, and further demonstrates that in a sample of shift workers performing a simulated night shift after a series of night shifts in their 1139 Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015

S Ftouni, TL Sletten, CL Nicholas et al. normal environment, performance is impaired and sleepiness increased when assessments occur near the circadian nadir of alertness. While the study used a relatively small sample, and thus limiting statistical power and the ability to generalize across the population, the study conducted in a sample of actual shift workers contributes to the body of knowledge examining the consequences of working near the amt6s acrophase. Targeted interventions are warranted to manage the negative impact on occupational safety and performance due to circadian disruption, and the consequential sleep deficiency, associated with shift work. 1,7 The mismatch between the circadian pacemaker, the sleep-wake cycle, and environmental cues has been found to have serious impacts on performance and safety, 2 particularly when there is a need to perform during the biological night. Future studies assessing ocular measures of sleepiness should investigate inter- and intra-individual variation in these measures, to provide greater insight into their utility and validity. In conclusion, shift workers who are working within range of the amt6s acrophase display increased sleepiness and performance impairment during a simulated night shift. The study is the first to demonstrate sleepiness-related ocular responses to the circadian nadir of alertness in a sample of shift workers, and contributes to the ongoing effort to develop real-time sleepiness measures as a part of occupational risk management systems. ABBREVIATIONS BTD, blink total duration CNS, central nervous system CV, coefficient of variation EEG, electroencephalogram EOG, bilateral electrooculogram ESS, Epworth Sleepiness Scale JDS, Johns Drowsiness Scale KDT, Karolinska Drowsiness Test MEQ, Morningness/Eveningness Questionnaire NegAVR, negative amplitude/velocity ratio PosAVR, positive amplitude/velocity ratio PSQI, Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index PVT, psychomotor vigilance task TEC, time with eyes closed REFERENCES 1. Akerstedt T, Wright KP. Sleep loss and fatigue in shift work and shift work disorder. Sleep Med Clin 2009;4:257 71. 2. Barger LK, Lockley SW, Rajaratnam SMW, Landrigan CP. Neurobehavioral, health, and safety consequences associated with shift work in safety-sensitive professions. Curr Neurol Neurosci Rep 2009;9:155 64. 3. Akerstedt T. Work hours, sleepiness and the underlying mechanisms. J Sleep Res 1995;4(S2):15 22. 4. Dijk DJ, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Circadian and sleep/wake dependent aspects of subjective alertness and cognitive performance. J Sleep Res 1992;2:112 7. 5. Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. Body-temperature is elevated during the rebound of slow-wave sleep following 40-h of sleep-deprivation on a constant routine. J Sleep Res 1993;3:117 20. 6. Cohen DA, Wang W, Wyatt JK, et al. Uncovering residual effects of chronic sleep loss on human performance. Sci Transl Med 2010;2:14ra3. 7. Folkard S, Lombardi DA. Modeling the impact of the components of long work hours on injuries and accidents. Am J Ind Med 2006;49:953 63. 8. Lyznicki JM, Doege TC, Davis RM, Williams MA. Sleepiness, driving, and motor vehicle crashes. JAMA 1998;279:1908 13. 9. Horne JA, Reyner LA. Sleep related vehicle accidents. BMJ 1995;310:565 7. 10. Gander P, Hartley L, Powell D, et al. Fatigue risk management: organizational factors at the regulatory and industry/company level. Accid Anal Prev 2011;43:573 90. 11. Anderson C, Chang AM, Sullivan JP, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA. Assessment of drowsiness based on ocular parameters detected by infra-red reflectance oculography. J Clin Sleep Med 2013;9:907 20. 12. Ftouni S, Sletten TL, Howard ME, et al. Objective and subjective measures of sleepiness and their associations with on-road driving events in shift workers. J Sleep Res 2013;22:58 69. 13. Stern JA, Walrath LC, Goldstein R. The endogenous eyeblink. Psychophysiology 1984;21:22 33. 14. Netzer NC, Stoohs RA, Netzer CM, Clark K, Strohl KP. Using the Berlin Questionnaire to identify patients at risk for the sleep apnea syndrome. Ann Intern Med 1999;131:485 91. 15. Maislin G, Pack AI, Kribbs NB, et al. A survey screen for prediction of apnea. Sleep 1995;18:158 66. 16. Johns MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep 1991;14:540 5. 17. Buysse DJ, Reynolds CF 3rd, Monk TH, Berman SR, Kupfer DJ. The Pittsburgh Sleep Quality Index: a new instrument for psychiatric practice and research. Psychiatry Res 1989;28:193 213. 18. Horne JA, Ostberg O. A self-assessment questionnaire to determine morningness-eveningness in human circadian rhythms. Int J Chronobiol 1976;4:97 110. 19. Aldhous ME, Arendt J. Radioimmunoassay for 6-sulphatoxymelatonin in urine using an iodinated tracer. Ann Clin Biochem 1988;25:298 303. 20. Australian Bureau of Statistics. Australian and New Zealand Standard Classification of Occupations. New Zealand: Australian Bureau of Statistics, 2009. 21. Johns MW, Tucker A, Chapman R, Crowley K, Michael N. Monitoring eye and eyelid movements by infrared reflectance oculography to measure drowsiness in drivers. Somnologie 2007;11:234 42. 22. Häkkänen H, Summala H, Partinen M, Tiihonen M, Silvo J. Blink duration as an indicator of driver sleepiness in professional bus drivers. Sleep 1999;22:798 802. 23. Caffier PP, Erdmann U, Ullsperger P. Experimental evaluation of eye-blink parameters as a drowsiness measure. Eur J Appl Physiol 2003;89:319 25. 24. Picot A, Charbonnier S, Caplier A, Vu NS. Using retina modelling to characterize blinking: comparison between EOG and video analysis. Mach Vis Appl 2012;23:1195 208. 25. Åkerstedt T, Ingre M, Kecklund G, et al. Reaction of sleepiness indicators to partial sleep deprivation, time of day and time on task in a driving simulator - The DROWSI project. J Sleep Res 2010;19:298 309. 26. Vadeby A, Forsman A, Kecklund G, Åkerstedt T, Sandberg D, Anund A. Sleepiness and prediction of driver impairment in simulator studies using a Cox proportional hazard approach. Accid Anal Prev 2010;42:835 41. 27. Ftouni S, Rahman SA, Crowley KE, Anderson C, Rajaratnam SM, Lockley SW. Temporal dynamics of ocular indicators of sleepiness across sleep restriction. J Biol Rhythms 2013;28:412 24. 28. Åkerstedt T, Gillberg M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci 1990;52:29 37. 29. Dinges DF, Pack F, Williams K, et al. Cumulative sleepiness, mood disturbance, and psychomotor vigilance performance decrements during a week of sleep restricted to 4-5 hours per night. Sleep 1997;20:267 77. 30. Jung CM, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA, Wright KP. Comparison of sustained attention assessed by auditory and visual psychomotor vigilance tasks prior to and during sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res 2011;20:348 55. 31. Lisper H, Kjellberg A. Effects of 24-hour sleep deprivation on rate of decrement in a 10-minute auditory reaction-time task. J Exp Psychol 1972;96:287 90. 32. Nelson W, Tong YL, Lee JK, Halberg F. Methods for cosinor-rhythmometry. Chronobiologia 1979;6:305 23. 33. Arendt J. Shift work: coping with the biological clock. Occup Med 2010;60:10 20. 34. Arendt J. Melatonin: characteristics, concerns, and prospects. J Biol Rhythms 2005;20:291 303. 35. Lockley SW, Skene DJ, Arendt J, Tabandeh H, Bird AC, Defrance R. Relationship between melatonin rhythms and visual loss in the blind. J Clin Endocr Metab 1997;82:3763 70. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015 1140

Ocular Measures of Sleepiness Assessed in Night Shift Workers 36. Basner M, Dinges DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep 2011;34:581 91. 37. Dinges DF, Kribbs NB. Performing while sleepy: effects of experimentallyinduced sleepiness In: Monk TH, ed. Sleep, sleepiness, and performance. Chichester, UK: Wiley, 1991:97 128. 38. Dijk DJ, Duffy JF, Czeisler CA. Contribution of circadian physiology and sleep homeostasis to age-related changes in human sleep. Chronobiol Int 2000;17:285 311. 39. van Amelsvoort LG, Schouten EG, Kok FJ. Duration of shiftwork related to body mass index and waist to hip ratio. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord 1999;23:973 8. 40. Barger LK, Sullivan JP, Vincent AS, et al. Learning to live on a Mars Day: fatigue countermeasures during the Phoenix Mars Lander Mission. Sleep 2012;35:1423 35. 41. Dijk DJ, Czeisler CA. Contribution of the circadian pacemaker and the sleep homeostat to sleep propensity, sleep structure, electroencephalographic slow waves, and sleep spindle activity in humans. J Neurosci 1995;15:3526 38. 42. Torsvall L, Åkerstedt T. Sleepiness on the job: continuously measured EEG changes in train drivers. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1987;66:502 11. 43. Anund A, Kecklund G, Kircher A, Tapani A, Åkerstedt T. The effects of driving situation on sleepiness indicators after sleep loss: a driving simulator study. Ind Health 2009;47:393 401. 44. Horne JA, Baulk SD. Awareness of sleepiness when driving. Psychophysiology 2004;41:161 5. 45. Barnes RG, Deacon SJ, Forbes MJ, Arendt J. Adaptation of the 6-sulphatoxymelatonin rhythm in shiftworkers on offshore oil installations during a 2-week 12-h night shift. Neurosci Lett 1998;241:9 12. 46. Barnes R, Forbes MJ, Arendt J. Shift type and season affect adaptation of the 6-sulphatoxymelatonin rhythm in offshore oil rig workers. Neurosci Lett 1998;252:179 82. 47. Gibbs M, Hampton S, Morgan L, Arendt J. Adaptation of the circadian rhythm of 6-sulphatoxymelatonin to a shift schedule of seven nights followed by seven days in offshore oil installation workers. Neurosci Lett 2002;325:91 4. 48. Gibbs M, Hampton S, Morgan L, Arendt J. Predicting circadian response to abrupt phase shift: 6-sulphatoxymelatonin rhythms in rotating shift workers offshore. J Biol Rhythms 2007;22:368 70. 49. Thorne H, Hampton S, Morgan L, Skene DJ, Arendt J. Differences in sleep, light, and circadian phase in offshore 18:00-06:00 h and 19:00-07:00 h shift workers. Chronobiology Int 2008;25:225 35. 50. Dumont M, Benhaberou-Brun D, Paquet J. Profile of 24-h light exposure and circadian phase of melatonin secretion in night workers. J Biol Rhythms 2001;16:502 11. 51. Benhaberou-Brun D, Lambert C, Dumont M. Association between melatonin secretion and daytime sleep complaints in night nurses. Sleep 1999;22:877 85. 52. Hansen AM, Garde AH, Hansen J. Diurnal urinary 6-sulfatoxymelatonin levels among healthy Danish nurses during work and leisure time. Chronobiology Int 2006;23:1203 15. 53. Midwinter MJ, Arendt J. Adaptation of the melatonin rhythm in human subjects following night-shift work in Antarctica. Neurosci Lett 1991;122:195 8. 54. Ross JK, Arendt J, Horne J, Haston W. Night-shift work in Antarctica: sleep characteristics and bright light treatment. Physiol Behav 1995;57:1169 74. 55. Deacon S, Arendt J. Posture influences melatonin concentrations in plasma and saliva in humans. Neurosci Lett 1994;167:191 4. 56. Skene DJ, Bojkowski CJ, Currie JE, Wright J, Boulter PS, Arendt J. 6-sulphatoxymelatonin production in breast cancer patients. J Pineal Res 1990;8:269 76. 57. Bearn J, Franey C, Arendt J, Checkley SA. A study of the effects of desipramine treatment alone and in combination with L-triiodothyronine on 6-sulphatoxymelatonin excretion in depressed patients. Br J Psychiatry 1989;155:341 7. 58. Deacon S, English J, Tate J, Arendt J. Atenolol facilitates light-induced phase shifts in humans. Neurosci Lett 1998;242:53 6. 59. Crowley SJ, Lee C, Tseng CY, Fogg LF, Eastman CI. Complete or partial circadian re-entrainment improves performance, alertness, and mood during night-shift work. Sleep 2004;27:1077 87. 60. Johns MW, Chapman R, Crowley K, Tucker A. A new method for assessing the risks of drowsiness while driving. Somnologie 2008;12:66 74. 61. Stephan K, Hosking S, Regan M, Verdoorn A, Young K, Haworth N. The relationship between driving performance and the Johns Drowsiness Scale as measured by the Optalert System. Melbourne: Monash University Accident Research Centre, 2006. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS The authors thank the research staff at the Sleep and Circadian Medicine Laboratory for their recruitment and data collection efforts, particularly, Ms. Jade Murray and Dr. Michelle Magee. SUBMISSION & CORRESPONDENCE INFORMATION Submitted for publication July, 2014 Submitted in final revised form April, 2015 Accepted for publication May, 2015 Address correspondence to: Shantha M.W. Rajaratnam, School of Psychological Sciences, Monash University, Building 17, Wellington Road, Clayton, 3800 VIC, Australia; Tel: + 61 3 9905 3934; Fax: + 61 3 9905 3948; Email: Shantha. Rajaratnam@monash.edu DISCLOSURE STATEMENT This was not an industry supported study. This research was supported by NHMRC project grant (#545871). Dr. Lockley has received consulting fees from Blackrock; Cowen & Co; Endurant Capital Management; Far West Capital Management; Fidelity; Frankel Group; Impax Laboratories; Kearney Venture Partners; Lazard Capital Markets; Naturebright; New Horizon Capital; Perceptive Advisors; Polar Capital; ResearchWorks Inc.; Thomas Jefferson University; Wyvern Funds; and has current consulting contracts with Carbon Limiting Technologies Ltd; Environmental Light Sciences, LLC; Headwaters Inc.; PlanLED; Delos Living LLC; Pegasus Capital Advisors LP; Wyle Integrated Science and Engineering; has received unrestricted equipment gifts from Bioilluminations LLC; Bionetics Corporation; and Philips Lighting; a fellowship gift from Optalert, Pty Ltd; advance author payment and royalties from Oxford University Press; payment for editing a textbook section from Elsevier; honoraria from the National Sleep Foundation; and for an article in the Wall Street Journal; honoraria plus travel, accommodation or meals for invited seminars, conference presentations or teaching from Brookline Adult Education; Brown University; Estee Lauder (2013-2014); Harvard University (CME); MediCom Worldwide, Inc.(CME); travel, accommodation and/or meals only (no honoraria) for invited seminars, conference presentations or teaching from 8th International Conference on Managing Fatigue; 14th Annual Tennessee Perfusion Conference; Cantifix; Connecticut Business & Industry Association Health and Safety Conference; Emergency Services Steering Committee; Harvard University; Hintsa Performance AG; Illuminating Engineering Society; Massachusetts General Hospital; Midwest Lighting Institute; New England College of Occupational and Environmental Medicine; Ontario Association of Fire Chiefs; Rio Tinto; UMass Memorial; University of Manchester; University of Texas Medical Branch; Woolcock Institute of Medical Research; ongoing investigator-initiated research grants from Biological Illuminations LLC; Respironics Inc.; and Vanda Pharmaceuticals Inc.; completed service agreements with Rio Tinto Iron Ore and Vanda Pharmaceuticals Inc.; two completed and one ongoing sponsor-initiated clinical research contracts with Vanda Pharmaceuticals Inc. Dr. Lockley also holds a process patent for the use of short-wavelength light for resetting the human circadian pacemaker and improving alertness and performance which is assigned to the Brigham and Women s Hospital per Hospital policy and has received revenue from a patent on the use of short-wavelength light, which is assigned to the University of Surrey. Dr. Lockley has also served as a paid expert witness on behalf of 8 public bodies and one union for arbitration and cases related to sleep, circadian rhythms, and work hours. Dr. Lockley also serves as a Theme Leader in the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. Dr. Rajaratnam has served as a consultant through his institution to Vanda Pharmaceuticals, Philips Respironics, EdanSafe, The Australian Workers Union, National Transport Commission, and Transport Accident Commission, and has through his institution received research grants and/or unrestricted educational grants from Vanda Pharmaceuticals, Takeda Pharmaceuticals North America, Philips Lighting, Philips Respironics, Cephalon, and ResMed Foundation, and reimbursements for conference travel expenses from Vanda Pharmaceuticals. His institution has received equipment donations or other support from Optalert, Compumedics, and Tyco Healthcare. He has also served as an expert witness and/ or consultant to shift work organizations. Dr. Rajaratnam also serves as a Program Leader in the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. Drs. Ftouni and Sletten serve as a Project Leaders in the Cooperative Research Centre for Alertness, Safety and Productivity. The other authors have indicated no financial conflicts of interest. All work was performed at the Sleep and Circadian Medicine Laboratory, Monash University, Australia. 1141 Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, Vol. 11, No. 10, 2015