Focus of Attention Instructions Impact on Pitching Accuracy Among College Baseball Pitchers

Similar documents
Switching Focus of Attention Mid-trial and its Effect on Stability During a Balance Task

Focus of Attention and Verbal Instructions: Strategies of Elite Track and Field Coaches and Athletes

The Effects of Attentional Focus and Skill Level in Performance of Badminton Long Service

An individual s focus of attention can have a considerable

To link to this article:

An External Focus of Attention Enhances Isometric Wall Sit Endurance Time: A Quantitative and Qualitative Analysis of the Attentional Focus Effect

Version of record first published: 23 Jan 2013.

O ne of the most meaningful ABSTRACT

Poor Quality of Instruction Leads to Poor Motor Performance Regardless of Internal or External Focus of Attention

ATTENTIONAL FOCUS DOES NOT IMPACT AGILITY PEFORMANCE AMONGST DIVISION I WOMEN COLLEGIATE TENNIS PLAYERS

A Paradigm Shift in Patient Education Are We Making Cueing Too Complicated?

The Effects of Vision and Internal and External Focus of Attention on Balance in a Yoga Pose

Effect of Static Stretching or Foam Rolling on Hamstrings Range of Motion and Strength

The Effects of Internal and External Focus of Attention on Balance and Dart Throwing: A Dual Task Approach

Often within a sport-training environment,

The effect of task-relevant and task-irrelevant attentional cues and skill level on performance and knee kinematics of standing long jump

The Effect of Task Difficulty and Instructions for Focus of Attention on Performing a Balance Task

The Effect of Internal vs. External Focus of Attention Instructions on Countermovement Jump Variables in NCAA Division I Baseball Players

AGES 16 / 18 AND UNDER Age Appropriate Strength and Conditioning LESSON WORKBOOK DARRYL NELSON

Faculty of Physical Education & Sport Sciences, Diala University. Seif El Din Watheq :

DEVELOPING SHOT PUT GLIDE TECHNIQUE

TRAINING GUIDE G O L F P E R F O R M A N C E T R A C K

Plyometrics. Ankle Bounces. Bounding. Butt Kuck

St. Joseph Rayong School Course Outline 1st Semester P5 Curriculum - Physical Education ( )

The Golf Swing Speed Challenge (103) - First 3 Weeks

Golfers Stretch. for Success by Performing Exercises using the Body Bar Flex

Age Determination. The athlete will compete the age they will be on December 31 of the year State Testing is held.

The placebo effect of preworkout drink on muscular strength

Journal of Motor Behavior Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:

P ERFORMANCE CONDITIONING. Appling National Jr. Team Programming to Your Situation. Off-Bike Sprinting Power Improvement: CYCLING

Plyometric Training Routine

CHERRY Creek Baseball. Fall workout CHERRY CREEK BASEBALL STRENGTH TRAINING

Dumbarton Academy. Physical Education. Standard Grade Course. Skills and Techniques Booklet

TRAINING GUIDE S H O U L D E R H E A L T H & P E R F O R M A N C E P A R T N E R W I T H

Full Body (medicine ball) Saggital Front Reach

An external focus of attention enhances manual tracking performance

Some Tips to Get Started When starting a strengthening program, keep some guidelines in mind:

Planning for Physical

Relationship between Body Core Stabilization and Athletic Function in. Football, Basketball and Swimming Athletes

POST-THROWING RECOVERY WORK

Grade 7 Life Skills: Physical Education Term 4

About Clayton Beatty & Total Surfing Fitness

Physical Education Skills Progression. Eden Park Primary School Academy

Prevent injuries before getting on the field

Interval Throwing Program

CORE EXERCISES INTRODUCTION NOTES FREQUENCY PAGE 1 OF 9. Trainer: Chad Benson

return to sports after injury IMPROVING STRENGTH, POWER, AND AGILITY

Cub Scout Den Meeting Outline

President s Day Pitching Camp

Best Practice: SPORTS

Day 5. Leg Raises. Throw downs. Side Crunches. Core/Abdominals

UNDERSTANDING THE IMPORTANCE OF MENTAL IMAGERY USING YOUR IMAGINATION IN YOUR ROUTINE by Patrick J. Cohn and Lisa Cohn

Grade 10. Warm-up and cool-down exercises. The warm-up. 1 Warm-up exercise 1: Light jog. 2 Warm-up exercise 2: Alternating pace

Warm-Up and Stretching Exercises


Lower Body Plyometric Exercises

The following exercises were developed by Tim Manson, Sylvester Walters and Doug Christie of TERNION*.

SCHOOL WIDE LINKS: Athletics Day

2018 NWC 05/06 Soccer Conditioning Packet

BLACK PEAR TRUST SUBJECT PLAN - PE

REMEMBER GOOD POSTURE DURING ALL YOUR EXERCISES, AVOID SLOUCHING AS YOUR CURRENT PROGRAM BECOMES EASY SLOWLY INCREASE:

Technical Progression Workshop. Throws Shot Put, Discus and Javelin

On The Road. Training Manual

Day 4. Throw downs. Vertical Flutter Kicks. Side Crunches. Core/Abdominals

World Wide WoD Gym Resolutions WoD Standards v2.1

CERTIFIED WEIGHTLIFTING PERFORMANCE COACH. Notebook THE SQUAT

LEVEL II TEACHING SKILL ANALYSIS. January 2010 Page 1


Pre and Post pool sessions. County Camps

6 Th Fiep European Congress / 6. fiep europski kongres. Mir Hamid Salehian

Hockey Canada. 8.0 Injury Prevention Techniques. 8.1 General Principles of Conditioning

BC Alpine Fitness Testing Field Protocols Revised June 2014

Why Movement Experiences at U6 Impact a Soccer Career

PGYVC Volleyball Circuit Athletic Plan

How to use the training schedule by level: If you are a beginning/any level climber - Follow the program as written

RJT. Pupil Task Cards: Jumping JUMP THROW RUN

THE EFFECTS OF VARIED MODALITIES OF TRAINING ON PERFORMANCE VARIABLES OF SPEED, FLEXIBILITY AND EXPLOSIVE POWER OF MALE COLLEGIATE CRICKET PLAYERS


(*Judges notes: We will have 4 working platforms, and 2 warm-up platforms. Warm-Up platforms will be outside, weather permitting)

OFF-ICE. Plyometrics and Agilities. The USA Hockey Coaching Education Program is presented by REVISED 6/15

Physiotherapy Services. Gross Motor Skills

Twitch Trainer. 8-Week Training Protocol. Ryan Steenberg, BS, CSCS, CGFI-3, NG360

Developing Pole Vault Technique

Introduction to Training Beginning Athletes. Beginning Athletes 19/04/2018. Fundamental Movement Skills for Beginning Athletes. Training Progressions

Athletic Development in the Nursery

Level 3 Physical Education HOMEWORK. Name: House:

11+ FOR KIDS MANUAL A WARM-UP PROGRAMME FOR PREVENTING INJURIES IN CHILDREN S FOOTBALL

How To Fly High With Plyometrics

1. Handspread should be about shoulder width apart 1. Follow thru with the plant by reaching upward 2. Front hand thumb under pole (knuckles facing

Acceleration. Emirates Arena 24/25 September Jared Deacon

Introduction to Biomechanics

Pole Vault INTRODUCING THE POLE VAULT

30/30 Challenge Program Overview

INVESTIGATION OF MANAGEMENT MODELS

Fast Feet You cannot play rugby without it!



Body Bar FLEX. Stretching Exercises for GOLF. by Gordon L. Brown, Jr. for Body Bar, Inc.

POWERBAG. Powerbag Video / DVD Series TRAINING GUIDE

National Strength Program for Young Players

Transcription:

Southern Illinois University Carbondale OpenSIUC Research Papers Graduate School Summer 7-4-2011 Focus of Attention Instructions Impact on Pitching Accuracy Among College Baseball Pitchers Brian Solemsaas Southern Illinois University Carbondale, solemsaasb04@siu.edu Follow this and additional works at: http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/gs_rp Recommended Citation Solemsaas, Brian, "Focus of Attention Instructions Impact on Pitching Accuracy Among College Baseball Pitchers" (2011). Research Papers. Paper 150. http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/gs_rp/150 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at OpenSIUC. It has been accepted for inclusion in Research Papers by an authorized administrator of OpenSIUC. For more information, please contact opensiuc@lib.siu.edu.

FOCUS OF ATTENTION INSTRUCTIONS IMPACT ON PITCHING ACCURACY AMONG COLLEGE BASEBALL PITCHERS by: Brian Solemsaas B.S., Upper Iowa University, 2009 A Research Paper Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Master of Science in Education degree Department of Kinesiology in the Graduate School Southern Illinois University Carbondale August 2011

RESEARCH PAPER APPROVAL FOCUS OF ATTENTION INSTRUCTIONS IMPACT ON PITCHING ACCURACY AMONG COLLEGE BASEBALL PITCHERS By Brian O. Solemsaas A Research Paper Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master s of Science in Education Department of Kinesiology Approved by: Jared M. Porter, Ph.D Julie A. Partridge, Ph.D Graduate School Southern Illinois University Carbondale July 4, 2011

AN ABSTRACT OF THE RESEARCH PAPER OF BRIAN SOLEMSAAS, for the Master s of EDUCATION degree in Kinesiology, presented on JULY 4th, 2011 at Southern Illinois University Carbondale. TITLE: FOCUS OF ATTENTION INSTRUCTIONS IMPACT ON PITCHING ACCURACY AMONG COLLEGE BASEBALL PITCHERS MAJOR PROFESSOR: Jared Porter, Ph.D Previous research has consistently demonstrated that using an external focus of attention rather than an internal focus of attention enhances motor skill learning and performance. The purpose of this study was to examine whether using different focus of attention (i.e. internal or external) influenced pitching accuracy. It was hypothesized that highly skilled baseball pitchers utilizing an external focus of attention would display greater pitching accuracy when compared to trials performed following instructions that were designed to direct attention internally. Participants (N=11) completed 60 trials under internal and external conditions, 20 trials per day over a six day period for a total of 120 trials. A univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted for absolute error and constant error. Results of this study did not support the experimental hypothesis, and findings were not consistent with the predictions of the constrained action hypothesis (Wulf et al., 2001). i

DEDICATION This paper is dedicated to Coach Dan Cal Callahan. He was a mentor and a friend who always supported me in my research and in coaching. He gave me the opportunity to work with the team and I am very thankful for that. I try every day to do things right. Thank you for being such a great man and coach, you are truly missed. ii

TABLE OF CONTENTS PAGE ABSTRACT... i DEDICATION... ii LIST OF FIGURES... iv Introduction... 1 Method... 8 Results... 12 Discussion... 14 REFERENCES... 19 VITA... 22 iii

LIST OF FIGURES FIGURE PAGE Figure 1... 9 Figure 2... 12 Figure 3... 13 iv

1 INTRODUCTION Over the years, pitchers have thrown bullpens in order to practice throwing mechanics and to work on their accuracy. A bullpen is a session used by pitchers to throw off a mound in order to practice throwing various pitches with the goal of improving accuracy. While athletes throw these bullpens, coaches provide verbal instructions and feedback to pitchers in an attempt to improve their overall performance. Although mechanical issues associated with pitching have been researched to give pitchers the most efficient way to deliver the ball (House, 2000), little has been done to investigate the influence verbal instructions have on pitching. It is critical for pitching coaches to identify the differential effects various types of verbal instructions have on pitching accuracy. There have been numerous studies that have shown that what participants focus their conscious attention on has an impact on their motor skill performance (Shea & Wulf, 1999; Wulf, Hoess, & Prinz, 1998; Wulf, Lauterbach, & Toole, 1999). Specifically, performance benefits have been greatest when participants use an external focus of attention (e.g., attention directed to the movement effect on the environment) compared to an internal focus of attention (e.g., attention to the movements themselves) (Wulf, et al., 1999). An example of an internal focus while completing a bench press would be to have the participant focus on extending the elbows when executing the lift; while an example of an external focus would be to have the participant focus on pushing the bar away from the chest. One of the first studies to empirically investigate the efficacy of attentional focus was conducted by Wulf and colleagues in 1998. Sixteen volunteers were randomly

2 assigned to either the internal focus or the external focus of attention group for a balance task using a stabilometer. Participants were instructed to place their feet on the platform so that the tip of each foot touched one of the red markers located on the stabilometer platform. Both groups practiced the task for two consecutive days. Learning was assessed in a retention test on the third day. The internal focus group participants were instructed to focus on their feet and to try and keep them at the same height, whereas participants in the external focus condition were instructed to focus on the red markers and to keep the markers at the same height. What they found was that there were no advantages for the external focus condition during practice; however during the retention test the external focus condition was more effective for learning than the internal focus condition. A reason for this is their attempts to consciously control the movement while in the internal focus condition actually interfered with automatic control processes. Wulf et al. (1998) suggested that instructions given to learners while they are practicing a motor skill can have a decisive influence on learning. Instructions related to the performer s body movements (internal focus) are not always optimal and can degrade performance (Wulf et al., 1998). Giving instructions that cause the performers to focus on the effects the movements have on the environment (external focus) can be much more effective for learning a motor skill. Advantages for learning a skill where attention is focused on the movement s effect rather than on the movement itself has been shown to be beneficial in many tasks such as golf (Bell & Hardy, 2009; Wulf, & Su, 2007), balancing (Wulf, 2008; Wulf, McNevin, & Shea, 2001; Wulf, Shea, & Park, 2001), basketball (Weiss, Reber, & Owen, 2008), dart throwing (Marchant, Clough, & Crawshaw, 2007; Weiss et al., 2008),

3 juggling (Zentgraf & Munzert, 2009), standing long-jump (Porter, Ostrowski, Nolan, & Wu, 2010), and weight lifting (Vance, Wulf, Tollner, McNevin, & Mercer, 2004). The benefits of adopting an external focus of attention are explained by the constrained action hypothesis (Wulf et al., 2001). This hypothesis states that participants trying to consciously control one s movements constrain the motor system by interfering with automatic motor control processes that would normally regulate the movement. Focusing on the movement effect, on the other hand, allows the motor system to more naturally self-organize, unconstrained by the interference caused by conscious control. This lack of interference results in more effective motor performance and learning (Wulf et al., 2001). For example, in a study by Wulf et al. (2001), participants balanced on a stabilometer, while probe reaction times (RTs) were taken to measure the cognitive demands required under external and internal attentional focus conditions. External focus participants demonstrated faster probe RTs compared to participants utilizing an internal focus of attention. These results suggest using an external focus of attention is less cognitively demanding, thus allowing the neurological system to process information more rapidly. If an external focus does lead to improved performance, why do most coaches typically give instructions about what the performer s bodies are doing during the performance of a skill (Porter, Wu, & Partridge, 2010)? Novices are guided to be aware of movement cues and what the body parts are doing while performing, while motor skills performed by highly skilled athletes appear to be performed automatically (Singer, Lidor, & Cauraugh, 1994). In other words, the benefits of this automaticity may depend on the skill level of the athlete. It was suggested by Bernstein (1996) that an external

4 focus of attention might be more beneficial for skilled athletes than less skilled athletes because the levels of automizations are different. Bernstein (1996) stated that motor skills are more highly automatized in expert athletes than in non-experts; and an internal focus of attention would essentially revert the athlete to a disrupting mode of control associated with less skilled performers. A study by Perkins-Ceccato et al. (2003) examined the effects of attentional focus in highly skilled golfers (average handicap of 4) compared to low-skilled golfers (average handicap of 26) under external focus (i.e., focus on hitting the ball as close to the target as possible) and internal focus conditions (i.e., focus on the movement form of the swing). What they found was that highly skilled golfers performed more effectively with external focus instructions, whereas the less-skilled golfers benefited more from internal focus instructions (Perkins-Ceccato et al., 2003). One question that still remained following the Perkins-Ceccato et al. (2003) study was whether the effectiveness of different focus conditions varied with higher levels of expertise? Wulf & Su (2007) sought to answer this question by using expert golfers in a pitch shot task. Six expert golfers from the University of Nevada Las Vegas golf team with average handicaps of 1.3 participated in this study. They were instructed to hit golf balls using their own clubs at a target 15 m away using an internal focus, external focus, and control conditions, performing 20 trials under each condition. In the internal focus condition, participants were instructed to focus on their arm motion. In the external focus condition, participants were instructed to focus on the club motion, and in the control condition participants were encouraged to use their normal focus of attention. Wulf & Su (2007) found that expert golfers performance benefited from instructions that induced an external focus. Not only was directing their attention to the club motion more effective

5 than directing attention to their arm movements, it was also more effective than no attentional focus instructions (control condition). They also found novices benefited from using an external focus of attention, which is in contrast to what Perkins-Ceccato et al. (2003) found in their study. Previous research has shown the benefits of using an external focus of attention. A study conducted by Wulf and Dufek (2009) sought to replicate findings of previous research (Wulf et al., 2007) which showed increased jump height with an external focus, by examining possible differences in force production as a function of attentional focus. In Wulf and Dufek s (2009) study, participants were to jump as high as possible using a Vertec measuring device to record vertical jump-and-reach height. After each participant was warmed-up they performed 10 jumps under each of the internal and external focus conditions. In the internal focus condition, participants were to focus on the tips of their fingers, reaching as high as possible. In the external focus condition, participants were instructed to focus on the rungs of the Vertec measuring device, reaching as high as possible. For each jump, the highest rung that the participants touched was recorded. What the researchers found was jump-and-reach heights, center-of-mass (COM) displacement, impulse, and joint moments were greater when using an external focus of attention compared to an internal focus (Wulf & Dufek, 2009). In a two-experiment study conducted by Freudenheim, Wulf, Madureira, Pasetto, & Correa (2010), swimmers had greater swim speeds while using an external focus of attention. In Experiment 1, participants were required to swim one length in an outdoor swimming pool (16 m) using the front crawl stroke. They were instructed to swim as fast as possible, pushing off from the inside of the pool. Different groups were instructed to

6 focus on different aspects of the stroke, either on the arm stroke or the leg kick. In the internal focus condition, participants were asked to focus on pulling your hands back (arm stroke) or pushing the instep down (leg kick), Participants in the external focus conditions were instructed to focus on pushing the water back (arm stroke) or pushing the water down (leg kick) (Freudenheim, et al., 2010). Results of Experiment 1, indicated there were no differences in swim times between groups regarding the arm stroke versus the leg kick. However, participants swam faster when instructed to focus on moving the water back or down (external focus) as opposed to moving their limbs back or down (internal focus). Thus, the difference in the wording of the instructions resulted in a significant advantage for the external condition (Freudenheim, et al., 2010). The question remained about the benefits of an external focus compared to internal focus or control conditions (Wulf & Su, 2007; Wulf, et al., 1998; Wulf, Zachry, Granados, & Dufek, 2007). Previous studies in which control conditions were used almost exclusively found benefits of external focus instructions compared to both internal and control conditions. In Experiment 2 of the Freudenheim et al. (2010) study, participants were required to swim one length in an outdoor swimming pool (16 m) three times using the front crawl stroke. For each trial, participants were given different instructions. They were instructed to focus on pulling your hands back (internal focus), or pushing the water back (external focus), or they were not given any focus instructions (control condition). Because an external focus is assumed to promote automaticity and participants movement control could be assumed to be somewhat automatic already, one might have expected similar results under external focus and control conditions (Freudenheim et al., 2010). Results indicated that participants swam

7 faster in the external condition compared to both the internal focus condition and the control condition. Consistent with earlier studies (Wulf & Su, 2007; Wulf, et al., 1998; Wulf, et al., 2007), directing participants attention to the movement effect (water) resulted in superior performance compared to those directing attention to their body movements (hands), or no focus instructions (Freudenheim et al., 2010). As previously mentioned, there are numerous studies showing the benefits of using an external focus of attention for a variety of sport skills. One consideration that has not been established is whether or not benefits of an external focus of attention are observed in pitching a baseball. If the manipulation of verbal instructions can lead to increased accuracy for pitchers, then it is of great importance for pitching coaches to understand how to deliver instructions so they can enhance the pitcher s performance. Thus, the purpose of this study was to examine if using verbal instructions to elicit different attentional focus (i.e., internal or external) influenced pitching accuracy. This is important for practical reasons so that pitching coaches are able to utilize external focus techniques in practice and in games to help their pitchers throw with better accuracy. It was hypothesized that participants would be more accurate when they used an external focus of attention rather than an internal focus of attention.

8 METHOD Participants Eleven male college baseball players (M age= 19.55 years, SD=1.63; M height= 187.96 cm, SD=4.40; M weight= 83.91 kg, SD= 12.04) participated in this experiment. Three of the participants were left-handed throwers and eight were right-handed throwers. Originally there were thirteen pitchers, but two were unable to participate due to injuries. All participants signed an informed consent and completed a medical history questionnaire; both of these documents as well as the experimental methods were approved by the University s Institutional Review Board. Apparatus and Task Participants were instructed to throw a bullpen off a regulation NCAA approved mound located 18.44 m from home plate. The pitching mound had a height of 25.40 cm. Participants used a regulation baseball made by Rawlings (22.86 cm in circumference and 141.75 g in weight) (NCAA, 2011). This experiment was designed to take place outside on a dirt mound at Abe Martin Field where the SIUC baseball team plays scheduled games. Due to inclement weather only two bullpen sessions were thrown outside off of a dirt mound. The other four bullpen sessions were thrown inside off of a turf covered mound with the same dimensions as the one mentioned above. For this study the participants were asked to throw only fastballs. In order to assure precise measurement, every bullpen session was recorded using a camcorder set approximately five meters in front and to the left side of the target. The target was set up at the back edge of home plate. The target was the size of the strike zone, 43.18 cm wide and approximately 101.60 cm in height. The strike zone was divided into 11 sections

9 (approximately every 10.16 cm apart) that were given point totals from 5 to -5 (see Figure 1). Data Collection Chart Date: Practice #: Pitcher: 5 4 Top Letters 3 2 1 0 Belt Line -1-2 Mid-Thigh -3-4 Bottom Knees -5 Figure 1. Chart used for data collection

10 Procedures All participants performed a ten minute dynamic warm up led by an athletic trainer, this was followed by a five minute cord warm up for their arms. The cord warm up included internal/external rotation exercises, front shoulder raises, pull-downs, and flexion/extension exercises for the wrist and forearm. This experiment utilized a within participant design and the two focus conditions were counter balanced across days so each participant performed each condition in a random order. The two conditions were external focus, and internal focus. Participants were read the appropriate set of instructions before beginning each session. After every five trials participants were asked what they were supposed to be focusing on. If they didn t remember the researcher reminded them what they were supposed to focus on for that particular session. Each participant was told to throw the baseball to the center of the target with maximum effort, but no other specific instructions on how to throw the ball were provided. Participants completed 60 trials under each condition, 20 trials per day over a six-day period for a total of 120 trials. Participants were provided a different set of instructions each day. The days they threw with the internal condition, participants were told to focus on getting extension by taking your fingers towards the target. The days they threw with the external condition, participants were told to focus on creating maximum backspin on the ball. Participants were not informed of the purpose of the study, nor were they debriefed following their participation. Data Analyses The researcher recorded the trials using a camcorder positioned about five meters in front and to the left side of the target. Once all the trials were finished the researcher

11 reviewed the video to chart where each trial hit on the target. To record the trials a chart set up to look like the target was used (see Figure 1). Once the data were collected, the researcher counted the number of pitches thrown to each section of the target, and then totaled them up for that day. The totals were then reversed scored for data analysis. The Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 17 was used for all statistical calculations. A univariate analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted for absolute error and constant error. The reliability of the dependent variable was determined by calculating interclass correlation coefficient reliabilities (ICCRs). The criterion for significance was set using an alpha level of p = 0.05.

12 RESULTS Results of the ANOVA conducted on constant error indicated that there was no significant difference between the Internal (M constant error = -1.68, SD = 2.78) and External (M constant error = -1.64, SD = 2.88) focus of attention conditions, F(1, 1318) = 0.069, p = 0.793 (see Figure 2). 2 Average Scores Using Constant Error 1 Constant Error Scores 0-1 -2-3 INT EXT -4-5 Focus Condition Figure 2. Average scores using constant error. Error bars represent standard deviation. The results of the ANOVA for absolute error also indicated there was no significant difference between the Internal (M absolute error = 2.74, SD = 1.73) and External (M absolute error = 2.80, SD = 1.77) focus of attention conditions, F(1, 1318) = 3.063, p =.582, (see Figure 3).

13 Absolute Error Scores 4.5 5 3.5 4 2.5 3 1.5 2 0.5 1 0 Average Scores Using Absolute Error INT EXT Focus Condition Figure 3. Average scores using absolute error. Error bars represent standard deviation. The ICCRs determined that the dependent variable was reliable for absolute error (r = 0.95) and constant error (r = 0.84) measures.

14 DISCUSSION The purpose of this study was to examine whether using verbal instructions to induce different forms of attentional focus (i.e., internal or external) influenced throwing accuracy in highly skilled collegiate pitchers. In order to do this each participant completed 60 trials under each condition over a six-day period for a total of 120 trials. It was hypothesized that using an external focus would yield better accuracy than an internal focus. However, results of the statistical analysis revealed there were no accuracy differences between the external focus and internal focus of attention conditions. When coaches give instructions to their athletes, they typically give instructions that reference specific body parts or body movements. This in turn would likely induce an internal focus of attention (Wulf, 2007a). Porter et al. (2010) looked at the types of verbal instructions and feedback provided by experienced track & field coaches during practice, and how this information influenced elite athletes focus of attention during competition. What they found was 84.6% of participants reported that coaches provided instructions during practice that promoted an internal focus of attention and participants reported they utilize internal focus cues 69% of the time during competition (Porter et al., 2010). These results are inconsistent with motor learning research, which shows learning and performance are typically enhanced when using an external focus of attention during motor skill execution. Previous research suggests that internally focusing on one s own movements constrains the motor system and leads to movements that are less accurate (Wulf & Su, 2007). This can be explained by the constrained action hypothesis which states when performers utilize an internal focus of attention they may actually constrain or interfere

15 with automatic control processes that would normally regulate the movement, whereas an external focus of attention allows the motor system to more naturally self-organize (Wulf, et al., 2001). The results of the present study do not suggest advantages in accuracy using either external or internal focus instructions. These findings are not consistent with the predictions of the constrained action hypothesis (Wulf, et al., 2001), which proposes that adopting an external focus elicits superior results compared to an internal focus. While this finding appears to be counter to several studies exploring the benefits of an external focus when compared to an internal focus, one explanation may be the participants chose what they wanted to focus on instead of what they were instructed to focus on. Meaning, even though they were instructed to focus either internally or externally they may have ignored the prescribed instructions and sought out the most efficient source of information to complete the task. In previous studies, advantages in learning and performance when using external focus instructions compared to internal focus or no focus instructions have been reported (McNevin & Wulf, 2002; Wulf & Su, 2007; Wulf et al., 2007). However, in the current study there were no benefits shown for either internal or external focus instructions. In a study done by Porter, Nolan, Ostrowski, & Wulf (2010), participants performed an agility task under an external focus, internal focus, and a control condition. They looked at the generalizability of the benefits of using an external focus of attention. They also wanted to see the accuracy in which participants followed prescribed instructions by using a manipulation check. This manipulation check also helped determine what participants focused on when they were given a neutral set of instructions (i.e., control). What they found was an external focus of attention facilitates performance compared to an internal

16 focus or control condition, which is consistent with previous research (McNevin, & Wulf, 2002; Wulf & Su, 2007; Wulf et al., 2007). Also, results indicated when participants were given a neutral set of instructions (i.e., control condition) their performances didn t differ from an internal focus condition. What was interesting though is in their study participants only focused internally 10% of the time (Porter et al., 2010) when they were in the control condition. Results of the attentional switching calculations suggested when participants in the control condition were provided a neutral set of instructions they chose to frequently switch their attention. Consequently, this strategy likely constrained the movements and interfered with the development of automatic processing, resulting in performance outcomes similar to participants who were directed to focus internally (Porter et al., 2010). In the present study, participants may have focused on something other than the prompted instructions they were given in order to find the most efficient method to improve performance. By not having a control condition or manipulation check to see what participants focused on in the present study, there was no way to measure the how accurately participants followed the prescribed instructions. This limitation should be considered when designing future experiments. Nevertheless, the present results suggest there may be a limit to the performance-enhancing effects of external focus instructions when used under real-world conditions. In a study by Wulf (2008), world-class balance performers were examined on the effects of internal and external attentional focus instructions relative to no instructions (i.e. control condition). Participants were required to balance on a semi-inflated rubber disk. They were instructed to focus on reducing movements of either their feet (internal focus) or the disk (external focus), or they were not given attentional focus instructions

17 (control). The results showed that regardless of type of instruction, the balance experts produced similar postural sway (Wulf, 2008). While there were no differences between conditions in the amount of postural sway, the frequency of movement adjustments was higher in the control condition, relative to both external and internal focus conditions. This suggests that movement automaticity and postural stability were greatest when the balance experts were free to adopt their normal focus of attention (Wulf, 2008). In other words, the instructions given in the present study may not have produced the optimal focus of attention. With increasing proficiency, individuals tend to control actions at higher levels (Vallacher, 1987). Meaning, the skilled pitchers in the present study were able to control their actions at a high level and both focus of attention instructions may have elicited a low-level effect. This would in turn disrupt automatic control processes used to throw a pitch. Future research should more thoroughly investigate how the skill level of the learner interacts with the optimal attentional focus needed to successfully achieve a desired action goal. Although the benefits of using an external focus of attention has been effective for a wide range of skill levels and motor learning tasks, the participants in the present study did not demonstrate these same benefits. While the current study provides an initial view into how pitchers perform when using verbal instructions, there are some limitations to the findings reported here, which raise questions to be addressed in future studies. The lack of a control condition was a big limitation on the study by preventing us from determining if the participants had better accuracy when they were free to adopt their normal focus of attention. Future research should use the same methods but implement a control condition in order to see whether participants accuracy was different than when

18 using an external or internal focus of attention. Future researchers can also use a manipulation check in order to see if participants switched their focus to something other than their prescribed instructions. This would also be valuable in understanding how participants focus their attention when they are allowed to choose what to consciously attend to. In addition, future studies should use a between-participant design and implement a retention test following practice in order to see if the verbal instructions given result in enhanced motor skill learning. Bullpens are commonly used by pitching coaches to evaluate their pitchers and have them work on their accuracy. Because of this, it is imperative that coaches provide their athletes with the most effective instructions to enhance the accuracy of their athletes. It is important for coaches to understand that what they say may impact the performance of their players. Although players may listen to what their coaches instruct them to do, they will actively seek out the most efficient sources of information and not adhere to specific instructions despite repeated reminders and encouragement, which may ultimately interfere with any instructions (Poolton, Maxwell, Masters, & Raab, 2006). Pitching coaches who utilize bullpens in practice must ensure that focus instructions are consistent among their athletes. Providing any inconsistency in instructions may lead to unreliable performances measures due to various types of attentional focus they may induce.

19 REFERENCES Bell, J.J., & Hardy, J. (2009). Effects of attentional focus on skilled performance in golf. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 21, 163-178. Bernstein, N.A. (1996). Dexterity and its development. In On Dexterity and its Development (edited and translated by M.L. Latash and M.T. Turvey), pp. 171-204. Mahwah: NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Freudenheim, A., Wulf, G., Madureira, F., Pasetto, S., & Correa, U. (2010). An external focus of attention results in greater swimming speed. International Journal of Sports Science and Coaching, 5, 533-542. House, T. (2000). The Pitching Edge. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics. Marchant, D.C., Clough, P.J., & Crawshaw, M. (2007). The effects of attentional focusing strategies on novice dart throwing performance and their task experiences. International Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 5. 291-303. McNevin, N. H., & Wulf, G. (2002). Attentional focus on supra-postural tasks affects postural control. Human Movement Science, 21, 187 202. NCAA. (2010). 2011-2012 NCAA Baseball Rules. Retrieved from http://www.ncaapublications.com/productdownloads/ba12.pdf Perkins-Ceccato, N., Passmore, S.R., & Lee, T.D. (2003). Effects of focus of attention depend on golfers skill. Journal of Sport Sciences, 21, 593-600. Poolton, J., Maxwell, J., Masters, R., & Raab, M. (2006). Benefits of an external focus of attention: Common coding or conscious processing? Journal of Sport Sciences, 24, 89-99.

20 Porter, J., Nolan, R., Ostrowski, E., & Wulf, G. (2010). Directing attention externally enhances agility performance: a qualitative and quantitative analysis of the efficacy using verbal instructions to focus attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 1, 1-7. Porter, J. M., Ostrowski, E.J., Nolan, R.P., Wu, W.F.W. (2010). Standing long-jump performance is enhanced when using an external focus of attention. Journal of Strength and Conditioning Research, 24, 1746-1750. Porter, J., Wu, W., Partridge, J. (2010). Focus of attention and verbal instructions: strategies of elite track and field coaches and athletes. Sports Science Review, 19, 199-211. Shea, C., & Wulf, G. (1999). Enhancing motor learning through external-focus instructions and feedback. Human Movement Science, 18, 553-571. Singer, R., Lidor, R., & Cauraugh, J. (1994). Focus of attention during motor performance. Journal of Sport Sciences, 12, 335-340. Vallacher, R. (1987). What do people think they re doing? Action identification and human behavior. Psychological Review, 94, 3-15. Vance, J., Wulf, G., Tollner, T., McNevin, N.H., & Mercer, J. (2004). EMG activity as a function of the performer s focus of attention. Journal of Motor Behavior, 36, 450-469. Weiss, S.M., Reber, A.S., & Owen, D. R. (2008). The locus of focus: the effect of switching from a preferred to a non-preferred focus of attention. Journal of Sport Sciences, 26, 1049-1058. Wulf, G. (2007). Attention and motor skill learning. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

21 Wulf, G. (2008). Attentional focus effects in balance acrobats. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 79, 319-326. Wulf, G., & Dufek, J. (2009). Increased jump height with an external focus due to enhanced lower extremity joint kinetics. Journal of Motor Behavior, 41, 401-409. Wulf, G., & Su, J. (2007). An external focus of attention enhances golf shot accuracy for beginners and experts. Research Quarterly for Exercise & Sport, 78, 384-389. Wulf, G., Hoess, M., & Prinz, W. (1998). Instructions for motor learning: Differential effects of internal versus external focus of attention. Journal of Motor Behavior, 30, 169-179. Wulf, G., Lauterbach, B., & Toole, T. (1999). Learning advantages of an external focus of attention in golf. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 70, 120-126. Wulf, G., McNevin, N.H., & Shea, C.H. (2001). The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54A, 1143-1154. Wulf, G., Shea, C., & Park, J. (2001). Attention and motor performance: preferences for and advantages of an external focus. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 72, 335-344. Wulf, G., Zachry, T., Granados, C., & Dufek, J. (2007). Increases in jump-and-reach height through an external focus of attention. International Journal of Sports Science & Coaching, 2, 275-284. Zentgraf, K., & Munsert, J. (2009). Effects of attenional-focus instructions on movement kinematics. Psychology of Sport & Exercise, 10, 520-527.

22 VITA Graduate School Southern Illinois University Brian O. Solemsaas Date of Birth: July 20, 1986 500 Saluki Blvd. Apt 813, Carbondale, Illinois 62901 1933 Billings Dr., Bismarck, North Dakota 58504 Email: bsolemsaas@gmail.com Upper Iowa University Bachelor of Science, Wellness/Fitness & Recreation, May 2009 Research Paper Title: Focus of Attention Instructions Impact on Pitching Accuracy Among College Baseball Pitchers Major Professor: Jared Porter, Ph.D