Psychopathy and aggression: Examining the role of psychopathy factors in predicting laboratory aggression under hostile and instrumental conditions

Similar documents
Factor Structure of the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale: Two and Three factor solutions. Kevin Williams, Craig Nathanson, & Delroy Paulhus

Testing Factor Models of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth Version and Their Association With Instrumental Aggression

Usage of any items from the University of Cumbria s institutional repository Insight must conform to the following fair usage guidelines.

Survey the relationship between five factor model and psychopathic personality in a sample of male prisoners in Iran

Mapping the Darkness and Finding the Light: DSM-5 and Assessment of the Corporate Psychopath

psychopathy and women

Audio will stream through your computer speakers at 2:00 PM ET. Exploring the Impact of Suicide Prevention Research in the Criminal Justice System

Psychopathy. Phil408P

Examining the Correlates of the Coldheartedness Factor of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory Revised

Overstepping Our Bounds: A Critical Examination of Youth Psychopathy

Role of Callous-Unemotional Traits in prediction of Childhood behavior problems

The P-psychopathy continuum: Facets of Psychoticism and their associations with. psychopathic tendencies. Nadja Heym, Eamonn Ferguson, Claire Lawrence

Psychopathy: Literature Review. Psychopaths are the social predators who charm and ruthlessly manipulate in order to do

Florida State University Libraries

Report of the Committee on Serious Violent and Sexual Offenders

Personality and Individual Differences

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Pers Individ Dif. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2010 December 1.

Psychopathy and Criminal Behaviour: A Psychosocial Research Perspective. (Version accepted for publication) ABSTRACT

According to Kernberg

Reference Lists With Key Findings and Conclusions Program Evaluation and Research Youth Forensic Psychiatric Services

Violence by Youth in Norway. Recent Cases

Psychopathy Variants: Empirical Evidence Supporting a Subtyping Model in a Community Sample

Treatment of Psychopathic Offenders: Evidence, Issues, and Controversies

EXAMINING THE PREDICTIVE AND INCREMENTAL VALIDITY OF TWO GENERAL MODELS OF PERSONALITY: A STUDY OF PSYCHOPATHY USING THE HEXACO-PI-R AND NEO PI-R

EXPLORING PSYCHOPATHIC PERSONALITY TRAITS AND MORAL DEVELOPMENT IN A NON-CRIMINAL SAMPLE. Kyle Bewsey, M.A. Dissertation Prepared for Degree of

Cover Page. The handle holds various files of this Leiden University dissertation.

A comparison of factor models on the PCL-R with mentally disordered offenders - The development of a four-factor model

Brief Report. narcissistic personality disorder in a male offender sample. Miller 3. Word Count: Tables, 1 Supplemental Table

PATHWAYS. Age is one of the most consistent correlates. Is Desistance Just a Waiting Game? Research on Pathways to Desistance.

THE SRP-II AS A RICH SOURCE OF DATA ON THE PSYCHOPATHIC PERSONALITY WHITNEY S. LESTER A THESIS

The construct of psychopathy in a Chilean prison population

One Measure Does Not a Construct Make: Directions Toward Reinvigorating Psychopathy Research Reply to Hare and Neumann (2010)

CRIMINAL THINKING AND ANTISOCIAL LOGIC

Development and Construct Validation of MMPI-2-RF Indices of Global Psychopathy, Fearless-Dominance, and Impulsive-Antisociality

Long-term patterns in interpersonal behaviour amongst psychopathic patients in secure inpatient treatment: A follow-up study

The Two-Factor Model of Psychopathic Personality: Evidence From the Psychopathic Personality Inventory

Impaired decision-making on the basis of both reward and punishment information in individuals with psychopathy

A Differential Item Functioning (DIF) Analysis of the Self-Report Psychopathy Scale. Craig Nathanson and Delroy L. Paulhus

Can the Triarchic Model Differentiate between Psychopathy and Antisocial Personality Disorder?

An examination of the treatment of callousunemotional

Levenson Psychopathy Inventory Grad 12 /Year 13 Fast Track Project Technical Report Anne-Marie Iselin and Richard L. Lamb 02/2010

Johanna Feilhauer. Same but different. Functional correlates of different psychopathy dimensions in (antisocial) youth

DELINEATING PSYCHOPATHY FROM COGNITIVE EMPATHY: THE CASE OF PSYCHOPATHIC PERSONALITY TRAITS SCALE

Validity of the PPI:SF and the TriPM using the CAPP as a Concept Map!

Psychological risk factors in Dutch violent female offenders

Psychopathy in Women. Dr Annette McKeown Highly Specialist Forensic Psychologist Tees Esk & Wear Valleys NHS Foundation Trust

Assessment Tools and Objective Measures of Alleged Sex Offenders

THE VULNERABILITY OF SELF-REPORT MEASURES OF PSYCHOPATHY TO POSITIVE IMPRESSION MANAGEMENT: A SIMULATION STUDY WITH INMATES. Katherine R. Kelsey, M.A.

THE TASTE OF AGGRESSION: A MODEL FOR PSYCHOPATHY AND REACTIVE AGGRESSION REBECCA M. KASTNER MARTIN SELLBOM, COMMITTEE CHAIR RANDALL SALEKIN

Factor structure of the Self-Report Psychopathy scale (SRP-II) in non-forensic samples

Psychopathic Traits, Skin Conductance, and Emotion in the Normal Population

PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS IN COLLEGE POPULATIONS 1

The Psychopath as Pimp

USING THE MMPI-A TO PREDICT RECIDIVISM IN ADJUDICATED MINORS

Countertransference with the psychopathic patient

A Rorschach Investigation of Psychopathy in a Sample of Incarcerated Females

Female Psychopathic Offenders: Personality Disorder and Offence Types

RUNNING HEAD: COLLEGE DRINKING: PSYCHOPATHY AND GENDER! 1!

Psychopathic Predators? Getting Specific About the Relation Between Psychopathy and Violence

The Dimensional Structure of the Self Report Psychopathy Scale

Personality Disorders. Mark Kimsey, M.D. March 8, 2014

Characteristics of Patients who Make Repeated Suicide Attempts

traits with aggression and delinquency in a nonreferred Boys and Girls

in London, UK Psychology of Crime: Psychopathy, Criminal Behavior and Violence PSY 4931, Summer 2014

Copyright: DOI link to article: Date deposited: This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License

Wesleyan University. From the SelectedWorks of Charles A. Sanislow, Ph.D.

Research Review: The importance of callousunemotional. aggressive and antisocial behavior

Violent risk assessment in women. Presentation outline. More media attention? Female violence

The Psychopathic Personality Inventory: Construct validity of the two-factor structure

CONDUCT DISORDER. 1. Introduction. 2. DSM-IV Criteria. 3. Treating conduct disorder

Personality and Individual Differences

Cognition and Psychopathology

TEST REVIEW: The Ontario Domestic Assault Risk Assessment Thomas A. Wilson, M.A., LCPC. Private Practice, Boise, ID

Psychopathy and Adolescent Females: Does Gender Alter the Relation Between Childhood Trauma and PCL:YV Scores?

by Natalia L. Nikolova M.A. (Psychology), Simon Fraser University, 2009

Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment (forthcoming)

The Relationship between Psychopathic Personality Traits and Lying

CRIMINOLOGY TODAY. Chapter 6 Psychological and Psychiatric Foundations of Criminal Behavior. By FRANK SCHMALLEGER. Pearson Education, Inc.

Early Maladaptive Schemas And Personality. Disorder Symptoms An Examination In A Nonclinical

Differences in Offending Patterns Between Adolescent Sex Offenders High or Low in Callous Unemotional Traits

PSYCHOPATHIC TRAITS, PROBLEM DRINKING, AND IMPLICIT COGNITIONS AS PREDICTORS OF ALCOHOL RELATED AGGRESSION AMONG COLLEGE STUDENTS JAIME LYNN RICHARDS

CC210: MID-TERM EXAMINATION #2 STUDY GUIDE

Different types of dangerousness autistic traits vs psychopathic traits

2012 Taylor and Francis

BETTER TOGETHER 2018 ATSA Conference Thursday October 18 3:30 PM 5:00 PM

Screening and Assessment

Psychopathy Factors and Risk for Aggressive Behavior: A Test of the Threatened Egotism Hypothesis

GENDER DIFFERENCES IN A PROTOTYPICAL ANALYSIS OF PSYCHOPATHY. Lisa L. Hazelwood, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

DEVELOPMENT OF A MULTIDIMENSIONAL APPROACH TO UNDERSTANDING YOUTHFUL OFFENDERS: THE INFLUENCE OF PSYCHOSOCIAL AND PERSONALITY RISK FACTORS

Testing Factorial Structure and Validity of the PCL:SV in Lithuanian Prison Population

ACDI. An Inventory of Scientific Findings. (ACDI, ACDI-Corrections Version and ACDI-Corrections Version II) Provided by:

Made available courtesy of the American Psychological Association:

Measures. What is child physical abuse? Theories of CPA. Social Cognition and CPA risk. Social Information Processing Model (Milner, 1994)

Suicidal Behavior in Inmates through the Pathway of Psychopathy and Depression

The role of affect and cognitive schemata in the assessment of psychopathy

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Assessment. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2011 August 9.

Callous Unemotional Traits, Impulsivity, and Emotional Processing in Adolescents With Antisocial Behavior Problems

Detailed Contents. 1 Science, Society, and Criminological Research 1. About the Authors xvii Preface xix

Transcription:

Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Research in Personality 41 (2007) 1244 1251 www.elsevier.com/locate/jrp Brief Report Psychopathy and aggression: Examining the role of psychopathy factors in predicting laboratory aggression under hostile and instrumental conditions Dennis E. Reidy, Amos Zeichner *, Joshua D. Miller, Marc A. Martinez Department of Psychology, University of Georgia, Athens, GA 30602-3013, USA Available online 18 March 2007 Abstract Psychopathy is a problematic configuration of traits and behaviors that is consistently correlated with aggressive, criminal behavior. Studies have suggested that psychopathy is composed of related but distinct factors that manifest divergent relations with a host of constructs including aggression. In the current study, we used a sample of 126 men to examine whether these psychopathy factors are differentially related to aggression manifested in two conditions (instrumental and hostile/reactive aggression) of a laboratory aggression paradigm. Traits related to an antagonistic interpersonal style and emotional detachment (i.e., Factor 1) were related to aggression in both conditions whereas traits related to negative emotionality, impulsivity, and an antagonistic style (i.e., Factor 2) were related to aggression only in the hostile/reactive condition. Potential explanations for these findings are put forth. Ó 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. Keywords: Psychopathy; Psychopathy factors; Instrumental and reactive aggression; Laboratory paradigm * Corresponding author. Fax: +1 706 542 8048. E-mail address: zeichner@uga.edu (A. Zeichner). 0092-6566/$ - see front matter Ó 2007 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2007.03.001

1. Introduction D.E. Reidy et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 41 (2007) 1244 1251 1245 Psychopathy is a personality disorder characterized by traits such as callousness, manipulativeness, egocentricity, impulsivity, and a need for stimulation (e.g., Cleckley, 1941; Hare, 1991), as well as a history of early, pervasive antisocial behavior. It is related to criminality and aggression (Gretton, Hare, & Catchpole, 2004; Skeem & Mulvey, 2001) and is a significant predictor of general and violent recidivism (Hemphill, Hare, & Wong, 1998). The Psychopathy Checklist (PCL) and its revision (PCL-R) have long been considered the gold standard in the assessment of psychopathy among incarcerated offenders. Until recently (see Cooke & Michie, 2001), psychopathy, as assessed by the PCL(R), was thought to be composed of two inter-related factors (r.50; Hare, 1991). Factor 1 (F1) is thought to be related to the interpersonal and affective components of psychopathy (e.g., grandiosity, lying, lack of remorse or guilt), whereas factor 2 (F2) is comprised of traits and behaviors thought to be indicative of social deviance (e.g., early behavior problems, juvenile delinquency, impulsivity; Hare, 1991). The divergent nature of these two factors is well-established. F1 is negatively associated with psychological distress (Harpur, Hare, & Hakstian, 1989; Verona, Patrick, & Joiner, 2001) and positively associated with narcissism (Harpur et al., 1989), as well as physiological signs of emotional detachment (Patrick, Bradley, & Lang, 1993), while F2 is positively associated with aggression and antisocial personality disorder (Hare, 1991; Skeem & Mulvey, 2001), recidivism (Hemphill et al., 1998), and distress (e.g., Verona et al., 2001). Another area of potential divergence is in relation to various forms of aggression, namely instrumental and reactive aggression. 1 Instrumental aggression (IA) is viewed as goal-driven behavior motivated by attainment of an external reward (Bandura, 1983). Hostile/reactive aggression (H/RA) has been described as an emotion-driven, impulsive, defensive response to a perceived threat (Berkowitz, 1989). Studies have documented a general relation between psychopathy and IA (Miller & Lynam, 2003; Williamson, Hare, & Wong, 1987). Despite this relation, few studies have examined whether these relations differ depending on which component of psychopathy is examined. Woodworth and Porter (2002) found significant positive relations between both PCL-R factors and IA. However, partial correlations suggested a significant unique relation only for the affective/interpersonal (F1) aspects of psychopathy. In a juvenile offender sample, Vitacco, Neumann, Caldwell, Leistico, and Van Rybroek (2006) found that a psychopathy factor comprising of the interpersonal items (e.g., pathological lying, conning) was significantly, positively correlated with IA, whereas a factor comprising the antisocial behaviors (e.g., poor anger control, serious criminal behavior) was significantly, negatively related. Finally, Cornell et al. (1996) found that criminal offenders with a history of IA were more psychopathic than offenders with only a history of H/RA. However, the IA group was higher in F2 scores but not F1. In a second study, Cornell et al. (1996) found that those with a lifetime history of IA were higher on both factors than a H/RA only group. 1 Bushman and Anderson (2001) have put forth a thoughtful critique of the hostile versus instrumental dichotomy arguing that the distinction is problematic because it fails to take into account aggressive acts in which mixed motives are present and makes assumptions, that often are not met, regarding the level of automaticity versus control that is present in the two forms of aggression.

1246 D.E. Reidy et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 41 (2007) 1244 1251 In the current study, we examined the relations between psychopathy factors, as assessed by Levenson s self-report psychopathy scale (LSRP; Levenson, Kiehl, & Fitzpatrick, 1995), and aggression using a laboratory aggression paradigm with two conditions designed to elicit either form of aggression. 2 Given research linking F1 and F2 to IA (Cornell et al., 1996; Woodworth & Porter, 2002), it was hypothesized that both LSRP factors would be correlated with IA manifested in a laboratory paradigm. However, following Woodworth and Porter (2002), we expected that the unique relations would be stronger for F1. In the H/RA condition, we again expected both factors to be significantly related but expected stronger unique relations for F2. 2. Method 2.1. Participants and experimental design Participants were 135 undergraduate males self-selected from a larger pool. Their mean age was 19.61 years (SD = 1.80) and 86% were Caucasian. Participants were randomly assigned to either an IA condition (n = 64) or a H/RA condition (n = 71). Participants were told that they would be competing with a male peer in a series of reaction time trials. Participants assigned to the IA condition were told that they would earn $1 for each trial they won and lose $1 for each trial lost, ostensibly giving them an opportunity to win a maximum of $24. The outcome of each trial was predetermined so that participants won and lost an equal amount. To reinforce the instrumental nature of the task, participants were told that they could punish their opponent via shocks after each trial, which could interfere with their opponent s performance, thus helping the participant win money. Participants in the H/RA condition believed they were competing in a reaction time task but knew that there was no performance incentive. 2.2. Materials Demographic form. Participants completed a brief form assessing age, race, and education level to confirm that groups were equivalent on these variables. LSRP (Levenson et al., 1995). The LSRP is a 26-item self-report measure of psychopathy designed for use in noninstitutionalized settings. Coefficient alphas for the LSRP total score (26 items), F1 (16 items) and F2 (9 items) were.80,.81, and.52, respectively. 3 Sample items for F1 include For me, what s right is whatever I can get away with and I enjoy manipulating other people s feelings. Sample items for F2 include When I get frustrated, I often let off steam by blowing my top and I don t plan anything very far in advance. 2 Levenson et al. (1995) titled the two LSRP factors primary and secondary psychopathy because they believed that this factor structure, derived from the PCL-R, fits Karpman s (1948) distinction between primary and secondary psychopaths. However, there is no clear consensus regarding the existence of these subtypes or their congruence with the various psychopathy factors identified through empirical study. As such, we have opted to avoid this debate, which we feel is outside the purview of the current study, by referring to the LSRP factors as factor 1 and factor 2. 3 Following Lynam, Whiteside, and Jones (1999), we scored the LSRP factor 2 with 9 items rather than 10 as item 26 demonstrated substantial dual loadings in their study.

Response choice aggression paradigm (RCAP; Zeichner, Frey, Parrott, & Butryn, 1999). Under the guise of a 24-trial competition, participants used an apparatus consisting of a metal box mounted with electrical switches and LEDs. Ten push-button switches labeled 1 through 10 are provided for the ostensible administration of shocks by the participant. A reaction time key is located on the console. Shocks are administered via electrodes attached to the participant s non-dominant hand. The RCAP results in seven inter-related indices of aggression (e.g., shock intensity, duration, and proportion of highest shock; see Zeichner et al., 1999). The median inter-correlation for these indices was.54; we report only a composite score (i.e., RCAP score) that was the sum of the standardized values for the seven indices. 2.3. Procedure Participants were told that they would be competing in a reaction time task against an opponent who was in the adjacent chamber, and that they would have the opportunity to punish him following each trial through the administration of an electric shock. Participants were told that their opponent could also punish them with shocks and that the shocks might interfere with performance on subsequent reaction time trials. Following each trial, the participants were informed whether they won or lost and were given an opportunity to administer a shock, regardless of the trial outcome. To do so, participants pressed 1 of 10 shock buttons that, ostensibly, increased the shock intensity. Participants were able to refrain entirely from administering shocks. LEDs provided feedback as to the level of shock participants received from the opponent. No shocks were administered above a participant s predetermined pain threshold. Each participant won 12 trials and, accompanied by shocks, lost 12 trials in a single randomized order (standardized across conditions). 3. Results D.E. Reidy et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 41 (2007) 1244 1251 1247 3.1. Bivariate relations between psychopathy and RCAP aggression 4 IA condition. The correlations among the LSRP scores and the aggression composite are presented in Table 1. Only the LSRP total and F1 scores were significantly correlated with the aggression composite. The LSRP factors were not significantly related. The correlations between the LSRP F1 and F2 and the aggression score were significantly different, t(55) = 2.17, p 6.05. H/RA condition. All three LSRP scores were significantly correlated with aggression. In addition, the LSRP factors were significantly related. Unlike the IA findings, the correlations between the LSRP F1 and F2 and aggression were not significantly different, t(65) =.72, ns. 4 Eight participants were excluded from data analyses as a result of their answers to the post-rcap manipulation check. No significant differences were found between the groups with regard to demographic variables or levels of psychopathy. One additional participant was excluded because his LSRP profile was deemed invalid.

1248 D.E. Reidy et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 41 (2007) 1244 1251 Table 1 Bivariate relations between LSRP scores and aggression in IA and H/RA conditions Aggression (RCAP) LSRP total score LSRP F1 LSRP F2 IA condition Aggression (RCAP) LSRP total score.36 ** LSRP F1.38 **.91 ** LSRP F2.02.52 **.14 H/RA condition Aggression (RCAP) LSRP total score.51 ** LSRP F1.48 **.95 ** LSRP F2.39 **.74 **.49 ** ** p 6.01. Table 2 Simultaneous regression analyses of LSRP factors across conditions Instrumental condition Hostile/reactive condition B RCAP total B RCAP total SE b SE b LSRP 1.31.10.39 **.30.10.42 ** LSRP 2.05.21.03.35.21.10 R 2.15 *.26 ** * p 6.05. p 6.01. Differences across conditions. We tested whether the correlations between the LSRP scores and aggression were significantly different across the conditions. There was only a significant difference in the size of the correlations for F2 (z = 2.14, p 6.05). 3.2. Psychopathy factors and aggression: simultaneous regression analyses IA condition. The results of simultaneous regression analyses, regressing RCAP scores on the LSRP factors, are presented in Table 2. Only F1 manifested a significant unique relation with aggression. Overall, the factors accounted for 15% of the variance in the aggression score. H/RA condition. Again, RCAP scores were regressed on LSRP F1 and F2; only F1 manifested a significant unique relation with the RCAP score. Overall, the LSRP factors accounted for 26% of the variance in the RCAP score. 4. Discussion This study investigated the relations between two psychopathy factors and aggression manifested in either an IA or H/RA laboratory paradigm. As noted, psychopathy manifests a consistent relation with aggression (e.g., Hemphill et al., 1998; Skeem & Mulvey, 2001). However, the nature of this relation, as it pertains to forms of aggression that might

D.E. Reidy et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 41 (2007) 1244 1251 1249 be differentiated by the motivating goal, level of negative affectivity present, and degree of impulsivity involved, is less clear. While the distinction between these forms may be tenuous at times (e.g., Bushman & Anderson, 2001), studies have demonstrated that psychopathy is linked to IA (e.g., Miller & Lynam, 2003; Williamson et al., 1987). However, few have examined whether this relation differs depending on which psychopathy component is used. In addition, previous research has relied on measures of aggression (IA vs. H/RA) that are based either on file reviews or self-reports. In the current study, we examined the relations between psychopathy factors and IA and H/RA in two controlled laboratory conditions. While psychopathy has previously proven itself a predictor of laboratory measures of aggression (e.g., Miller & Lynam, 2003; Parrott & Zeichner, 2006), it has not been examined with relation to this distinction. Consistent with previous research, psychopathy was significantly related to aggression manifested in both conditions; together, psychopathy factors accounted for 15 and 26% of the variability in laboratory aggression. We expected that both LSRP factors would be correlated with aggression in both conditions but that there would be a stronger unique relation between F1 and IA, as well as between F2 and H/RA. Contrary to previous research (e.g., Cornell et al., 1996; Woodworth & Porter, 2002), only LSRP F1 was significantly related to aggression in the IA condition. Consistent with predictions, both LSRP factors were significantly related to aggression in the H/RA condition. Unexpectedly, LSRP F1 was the only unique predictor of H/RA. We had hypothesized that the role of impulsivity and negative affectivity involved in H/RA would be more strongly linked to the LSRP F2. In partial support of these hypotheses, F2 was more strongly linked to aggression in the H/RA condition; the reverse was not true in that the relation between LSRP F1 and aggression did not differ across conditions. These results indicate that regardless of whether there is external motivation, the traits associated with F1 put individuals at higher risk for aggression. Alternatively, the traits associated with F2 put individuals at risk for aggression only in situations in which those traits are activated. One way to understand these findings is to review what is known about the two LSRP factors. Lynam (2002) and Ross, Lutz, and Bailley (2004) suggest that the LSRP factors demonstrate divergent relations with general personality traits. While both LSRP factors are significantly related to Antagonism, LSRP F2 is more strongly related to Neuroticism (positively) and Conscientiousness (negatively). This supports the contention that F1 comprises of interpersonal traits such as grandiosity, manipulativeness and decreased concern for others, while F2 comprises a variety of traits including impulsivity, negative affectivity, as well as antagonistic traits (albeit at a lower level). It may be that the prominent role of Antagonism in LSRP F1 is responsible for its relation to aggression across conditions. Miller and Lynam (2006) found that both IA and H/RA are characterized by strong relations with Antagonism. In addition, several studies have found that this personality dimension is among the strongest correlates of antisocial and aggressive behavior (e.g., Skeem & Mulvey, 2001). The role of Neuroticism may explain why LSRP F2 is only a significant predictor of aggression in the H/RA situation. The current findings mirror the conclusions drawn from a meta-analysis examining the relations between personality and aggression under neutral and provoking conditions (Bettencourt, Talley, Benjamin, & Valentine, 2006). These authors conclude that Neuroticism may be more likely to be positively associated with aggressive behavior only in response to provocation and that Antagonism may be more likely to be positively associated with a proneness to engage in aggressive behavior across a variety of situations

1250 D.E. Reidy et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 41 (2007) 1244 1251 (p. 770). As such, LSRP F1, which is composed primarily of Antagonism (e.g., Lynam, 2002; Ross et al., 2004), should be related to aggression regardless of experimental situation; such was the case here. Alternatively, LSRP F2, which is composed, in part, of high Neuroticism, should primarily be related to aggression only in response to provocation. Again, this is consistent with the current findings. Individuals in the H/RA condition may have viewed the shocks they received as being provocative as there was no external incentive to use this option. Another potential explanation for the current findings may be that the LSRP factors are not ideal indicators of the typical psychopathy factor structure. Lilienfeld and Fowler (2005) suggest that there are problems with the LSRP F1 as it is more highly related to measures of secondary psychopathy and antisocial behaviors than to measures of the core affective and interpersonal features of psychopathy (p. 118). While it is not unusual to see poor congruence between the factor scores of self-report psychopathy measures and the PCL-R (e.g., Berardino, Meloy, Sherman, & Jacobs, 2005), it may be that the LSRP F1 was the primary correlate because it assesses either a combination of both factors or, as suggested by Lilienfeld and Fowler, primarily the social deviance factor. This potential limitation of the LSRP might explain why the current findings diverge from much of the extant research that suggests that the social deviance component (F2) is the stronger correlate of aggression (e.g., Gretton et al., 2004; Skeem & Mulvey, 2001). A third explanation is also methodological in nature. The reliability of the LSRP F2 was poor, which would have made it difficult to detect significant effects and the differential reliabilities of F1 and F2 would have made it difficult to find a unique contribution for F2. In addition, the LSRP factors were not significantly related in the IA condition, which is unusual. There may have been a methodological confound in the IA condition that resulted in the lack of correlation between F1 and F2 and between F2 and aggression. The current findings represent a new avenue of research into the manner in which psychopathic traits lead to aggression. Future research would benefit from using alternative measures of psychopathy and a general measure of personality that would allow a more refined analysis of these issues. Indeed, it is important that we identify the interactive effects among personality profiles (e.g., psychopathy), traits (e.g., Neuroticism, Antagonism) and ecological determinants as risk factors for aggression and violence. References Bandura, A. (1983). Psychological mechanisms of aggression. In R. G. Green & E. I. Donnerstein (Eds.). Aggression: Theoretical and empirical views (Vol. 1, pp. 1 40). New York: Academic Press. Berardino, S. D., Meloy, J. R., Sherman, M., & Jacobs, D. (2005). Validation of the Psychopathic Personality Inventory on a female inmate sample. Behavioral Sciences and the Law, 23, 819 836. Berkowitz, L. (1989). Frustration-aggression hypothesis: Examination and reformulation. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 59 73. Bettencourt, B. A., Talley, A., Benjamin, A. J., & Valentine, J. (2006). Personality and aggressive behavior under provoking and neutral conditions: A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 751 777. Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. A. (2001). Is it time to pull the plug on the hostile versus instrumental aggression dichotomy? Psychological Review, 108, 273 279. Cleckley, H. (1941). The mask of sanity. St. Louis, MO: Mosby. Cooke, D. J., & Michie, C. (2001). Refining the construct of psychopathy: Towards a hierarchical model. Psychological Assessment, 13, 171 188. Cornell, D. G., Warren, J., Hawk, G., Stafford, E., Oram, G., & Pine, D. (1996). Psychopathy in instrumental and reactive violent offenders. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64, 783 790.

D.E. Reidy et al. / Journal of Research in Personality 41 (2007) 1244 1251 1251 Gretton, H. M., Hare, R. D., & Catchpole, R. E. (2004). Psychopathy and offending from adolescence to adulthood: A ten year follow-up. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology, 72, 636 645. Hare, R. D. (1991). Manual for the psychopathy checklist-revised. Toronto, Ontario, Canada: Multi-Health Systems. Harpur, T. J., Hare, R. D., & Hakstian, A. R. (1989). Two-factor conceptualization of psychopathy: Construct validity and assessment implications. Psychological assessment. A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1, 6 17. Hemphill, J. F., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1998). Psychopathy and recidivism: A review. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 3, 141 172. Karpman, B. (1948). The myth of the psychopathic personality. American Journal of Psychiatry, 104, 523 534. Levenson, M. R., Kiehl, K. A., & Fitzpatrick, C. M. (1995). Assessing psychopathic attributes in a noninstitutionalized population. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68, 151 158. Lilienfeld, S. O., & Fowler, K. A. (2005). The self-report assessment of psychopathy: Problems, pitfalls, and promises. In C. J. Patrick (Ed.), Handbook of psychopathy (pp. 107 132). New York: Guilford Press. Lynam D. R. (2002). Psychopathy from the perspective of the five factor model. In P. T. Costa, T. A. Widiger (Eds.), Personality disorders and the five-factor model of personality (2nd ed., pp. 325 350). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Lynam, D. R., Whiteside, S., & Jones, S. (1999). Self-reported psychopathy: A validation study. Journal of Personality Assessment, 73, 110 132. Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2003). Psychopathy and the five-factor model of personality: A replication and extension. Journal of Personality Assessment, 81, 168 178. Miller, J. D., & Lynam, D. R. (2006). Reactive and proactive aggression: Similarities and differences. Personality and Individual Differences, 41, 1469 1480. Parrott, D. J., & Zeichner, A. (2006). Effect of psychopathy on physical aggression toward gay and heterosexual men. Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 21, 1 21. Patrick, C. J., Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (1993). Emotion in the criminal psychopath: Startle reflex modulation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 82 92. Ross, S. R., Lutz, C. J., & Bailley, S. E. (2004). Psychopathy and the five factor model in a noninstitutionalized sample: A domain and facet level analysis. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26, 213 223. Skeem, J. L., & Mulvey, E. P. (2001). Psychopathy and community violence among civil psychiatric patients: Results from the MacArthur Violence Risk Assessment Study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 358 374. Verona, E., Patrick, C. J., & Joiner, T. T. (2001). Psychopathy, antisocial personality, and suicide risk. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 104, 462 470. Vitacco, M. J., Neumann, C. S., Caldwell, M. F., Leistico, A., & Van Rybroek, G. J. (2006). Testing factor models of the Psychopathy Checklist: Youth version and their association with instrumental aggression. Journal of Personality Assessment, 87, 74 83. Williamson, S., Hare, R. D., & Wong, S. (1987). Violence: Criminal psychopaths and their victims. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 19, 454 462. Woodworth, M., & Porter, S. (2002). In cold blood: Characteristics of criminal homicides as a function of psychopathy. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111, 436 445. Zeichner, A., Frey, F. C., Parrott, D. J., & Butryn, M. (1999). Measurement of laboratory aggression: A new response choice paradigm. Psychological Reports, 82, 1229 1237.