Span Theory: An overview

Similar documents
The Construct Validity of Memory Span as a Measure of Intelligence

Span theory: A theory of mental retardation and intelligence Bruce L. Bachelder Psychological & Educational Services Morganton, NC.

The Compound Stimuli Visual Information (CSVI) Task Revisited: Presentation Time, Probability Distributions, and Attentional Capacity Limits

COGNITIVE AND METACOCOGNITIVE PERFORMANCE ON MATHEMATICS

TEST # 1. Before starting the test, please print your name and ID number at the top left-hand corner of this page only.

From: What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015

Developmental Evidence for Working Memory as Activated Long-Term Memory

1.1 FEATURES OF THOUGHT

Chapter 11: Behaviorism: After the Founding

Theory, Models, Variables

AN EPIC COMPUTATIONAL MODEL OF VERBAL WORKING MEMORY D. E. Kieras, D. E. Meyer, S. T. Mueller, T. L. Seymour University of Michigan Sponsored by the

Chapter 2: Theories of Development

Development of Concept of Transitivity in Pre-Operational Stage Children

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening

Ilene Schwartz, Ph.D., BCBA-D University of Washington

HPSC30023 Science and Society

PRINCIPLES OF EMPIRICAL SCIENCE: A REMINDER

Bill Wilson. Categorizing Cognition: Toward Conceptual Coherence in the Foundations of Psychology

Behaviorism: An essential survival tool for practitioners in autism

DEVELOPMENTAL PROCESSES IN ADULTHOOD -

5.8 Departure from cognitivism: dynamical systems

PSYCHOLOGY IAS MAINS: QUESTIONS TREND ANALYSIS

INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY. Radwan Banimustafa

JENSEN'S THEORY OF INTELLIGENCE: A REPLY

The Microgenetic Method: Time for Change?

ʻThe concept of Deaf identity in Sloveniaʼ

AU TQF 2 Doctoral Degree. Course Description

Early Systematic Thinking Underlying Moral Values in Children Precognitive Stages: an Information Integration Theory Approach

Chapter 1 Introduction to Educational Research

Nathaniel L. Foster Department of Psychology St. Mary s College of Maryland St. Mary s City, MD (240)

LEARNING. Learning. Type of Learning Experiences Related Factors

Education Options for Children with Autism

Behaviorism: Laws of the Observable

B.A. IN PSYCHOLOGY. B.A. in Psychology 1. PSY 430 Advanced Psychopathology PSY 440 & PSY 441

Lecturer: Dr. Joana Salifu Yendork, Department of Psychology Contact Information:

Instructional Practices for Students with Autism A.. Kimberly Howard M.Ed.

A Critique of Thurber, Sheehan, & Valtinson s (2007) Appliance of a Piagetian Framework in Analyzing a Subject with Autism. Frederick Herrmann

9/5/ Research Hazards Awareness Training

BRIAR CLIFF UNIVERSITY Requirements for a Major in Psychology*

PSYCHOLOGY 5201 CLINICAL ASSESSMENT TECHNIQUES COURSE OUTLINE (FALL 2016 WINTER 2017)

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY (EPSY)

Psychology Courses PSYC 103 Introduction to Psychological Science (3) PSYC 211 Psychological Statistics (3) PSYC 213 Conditioning and Learning (3)

Catalogue Listing for School Psychology Core Courses

INTRODUCTION METHODS

Bachelor, Psychology, December 1996 Universidade Mackenzie, São Paulo, Brazil Majors: Clinical, Organizational and Educational Psychology

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Developing Core Competencies for the Counselling Psychologist Scope: Initial Consultation and Call for Nominations

The interplay of domain-specific and domain general processes, skills and abilities in the development of science knowledge

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Each copy of any part of a JSTOR transmission must contain the same copyright notice that appears on the screen or printed page of such transmission.

Psychology (PSYC) Psychology (PSYC) 1

Student Learning Overview and Goals

Protocol analysis and Verbal Reports on Thinking

PSYCHOLOGY (PSYC) Psychology (PSYC) 1. PSYC 2583 Developmental Psychology (S)

College of Arts and Sciences. Psychology

Interaction Effects: Centering, Variance Inflation Factor, and Interpretation Issues

ZIVA KUNDA. Updated July University of Waterloo Waterloo, Ontario Canada N2L 3G1 (519) ext

Development of Achievement Motivation

PSYCHOLOGY (PSY) 440, 452, hours: elective courses based on the student s focus (applied or pre-doctoral)

Effects of "Mere Exposure" on Learning and Affect

Item Analysis Explanation

Myths! Myths and Realities of Evaluation, Identification, and Diagnosis of ASD 10/11/10. Facts. The Ziggurat Group

Dynamics of Color Category Formation and Boundaries

June David S. Martin Professor/Dean Emeritus Gallaudet University

NEO-PIAGETIAN THEORIES OF COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT. Andreas Demetriou. University of Cyprus

INTRODUCTION TO PSYCHOLOGY

24. PSYCHOLOGY (Code No. 037)

Learning Support for Students with High Functioning Autism in. Post-secondary Learning Communities. Jeanne L. Wiatr, Ed.D.

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE: AN OVERVIEW

DEVELOPMENT OF HUMAN POTENTIAL: INVESTMENT INTO OUR FUTURE

PSYCHOLOGY. Degree Programs Offered. Certificates Offered. Psychology 1

Developmental Disabilities: Diagnosis and Treatment. Sara Sanders, Psy.D. 03/05/15

PSYCHOLOGY (413) Chairperson: Sharon Claffey, Ph.D.

Word Association Type and the Temporal Stacking of Responses

Autism & intellectual disabilities. How to deal with confusing concepts

Learning Objectives: 1. To understand the core and other associated characteristics of autism spectrum disorders.

EDP 548 EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY. (3) An introduction to the application of principles of psychology to classroom learning and teaching problems.

Matrix of SM by Outcome and Age (years)

MASTER PROGRAMME IN LOGOPEDICS AUDITORY-VERBAL THERAPY AFTER COCHLEAR IMPLANTATION

Periodization as a philosophy of training program design

Metacognition, Self-Regulation, and Self-Regulated Learning: Research Recommendations

College of Arts and Sciences. Psychology

A Guide to Clinical Interpretation of the Test of Variables of Attention (T.O.V.A. TM )

Multiple Act criterion:

Chapter IV. Information Transfer Rate

Improving Reading Comprehension for Students with ADHD

Strategic Application of Human Development Applying Positive Psychology. The Principles of Developmental Psychology

Speaker Notes: Qualitative Comparative Analysis (QCA) in Implementation Studies

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

The Logic of Causal Inference

A CASE STUDY ON CLINICAL DOHSATHERAPY ON THE SELF-CONTROL OF CHILDREN WITH MENTAL RETARDATION. 1. Introduction

Gender Differences in Adolescent Ego. Development and Ego Functioning Level

CURRICULUM OF THE DOCTORAL SCHOOL OF PSYCHOLOGY from 2016 (full time) 1 type of

Measuring mathematics anxiety: Paper 2 - Constructing and validating the measure. Rob Cavanagh Len Sparrow Curtin University

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Neuro Q no.2 = Neuro Quotient

Depressed mood effects on processing of highand low content structure text in American and Australian college women

Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior 13 th ed. Introduction: The Psychology of Studying Reflective Learning.

Transcription:

Page 1 Span Theory: An overview Bruce L. Bachelder 1 2 Morganton, NC Span theory (Bachelder, 1970/1971; 1974; 1977a, b, c; 1978; 1980; 1981; 1999; 2001a,b; 2003; 2005a,b; 2007; Bachelder & Denny, 1976; 1977a, b; Denny, 1980, Chap. 14) is a theory of intelligence which got its start in the late 60s in a program (Denny, 1966) which aimed to bring the findings of basic and applied mental retardation research to bear on the challenge of teaching concepts to adolescents with moderate to severe mental retardation. The theory blends critical features of behaviorism, cognitivism, and psychometrics in an account of developmental and individual differences in diverse tasks. It rigorously avoids mentalism. Each of us has a span ability defined in terms of the behavioral concept of stimulus control and measured by the standard staircase span test. Task analysis of span load (TASL) is a type of task analysis which links span ability with the ability to perform in specific tasks. Span theory focuses on traditional tasks of cognitivism including the three span limits and individual and developmental differences in diverse cognitive tasks and mental abilities and intelligence. The original goal of span theory, to understand individual differences in intelligence and learning in a way which will lead to more effective teaching and training, is still a central concern. Eventually, however, it became clear that (a) the theory could be a full theory of intelligence, apply to delayed, normal, average, and bright individuals; and (b) span theory is a Kuhnian (Kuhn, 1970) paradigmatic alternative to both cognitivism and behaviorism as an approach to scientific psychology. Span theory is grossly similar to two theories from the cognitive and cognitive science traditions. These are, respectively, Pascual-Leone s neo-piagetian M-space theory (e.g. Case, 1972; 1974; Pascual-Leone, 1970; 1987) and working memory capacity theory ( e.g. Case, 1978; Engle & Kane, 2004). Span theory developed independantly of either of these theories and work from these alternative points of view has made little reference to span theory. All three approaches aim to account for developmental and individual differences in diverse tasks via a capacity notion which has to do with coping with task demands. All three use variations on task analysis to assess task demand for capacity. All emphasize the link between their capacity notions and cognition and intelligence. Span theory has had far ranging utility. It proposes an unitary answer to the spans question (Bachelder, 2005a), which asks, What is the nature of the curious span limits in human performance? It accounts for diverse data of children and adults, both normally developing and developmentally delayed (e.g. Bachelder, 1970/1971; 1974; 1977a, b; 1978; 1979; 1980; Bachelder & Denny, 1977a, b). It offers an account of intelligence, ----------------------------------- 1 306 West Union St, Morganton, NC Bruce@BruceBachelderPhD.com www.brucebachelderphd.com 2 I am gradually posting my work to my web page. Call up my page and on the opening page click on Credentials, Experience, Publications... This calls up my CV and the materials are entered as links in the publications, presentations, and miscellaneous papers sections.

Page 2 combining the behavioral repertoire notion with an empirically derived ability construct defined in terms of the behavioral concept of stimulus control. It provides the scientific base for the development and application of new psychometric assessment and prescription techniques (Bachelder, 1978, Pt. 3; 2003) which are useful with people who can t take standard IQ tests or who have moderate to severe mental retardation. It bridges and integrates cognitivism and behaviorism. This diverse applicability stems in large part from a departure from both cognitivism and behaviorism; span theory uses a different unit of theoretical analysis. Cognitivism uses the structure and processes of the mind; behaviorism uses the response, the S-R bond, or the operant; span theory uses the task. The generic nature of the task means the theory lends itself readily to analysis of the tasks of both behaviorism and cognitivism but without mentalism. Since IQ scores are derived from performances on subtests and since subtests are complex tasks, the theory is also a theory of IQ scores and, therefore, of intelligence. Span theory can be distilled to a single statement: P task = f(span ability, 1/span load, acquired repertoire, other variables). As is typical of well developed scientific theories, a statement like this is incomprehensible without a good bit of background in the meanings of terms and experience applying it in diverse situations (Kuhn, 1970, pp. 46-47). P task refers to performance in a specified task or task family, assessed as the probability of a correct response and ranging from 0.0 to 1.0. Span load is a characteristic of a task or task family. It is a count of the number of stimuli conjunctively relevant for a target response. Loosely, in mentalistic terms, it is the number of stimuli which must be kept in mind as a basis for correct responding. Span ability is a developmental and individual differences variable, defined as the ability to cope with span load and measured with a psychometric span test. Tasks. Both cognitivism and behaviorism view tasks as tools to explore theoretically important hypothetical processes. For example, cognitivism has tasks to measure memory and attention; behaviorism has tasks to measure learning, conditioning, and stimulus discrimination. In span theory the task is not a tool to measure hypothetical processes, it is a molar unit of behavior, the central unit of theoretical analysis, the object of a behavioral taxonomy. A task has seven defining characteristics: (a) the stimulus pool, (b) the response pool, (c) the relevant stimulus set, (d) the procedure, (e) the S-R rule, (f) the counting rule, and (g) the task equation. This is not so esoteric as it first appears. All characteristics except the counting rule and the task equation are standard parts of the methods sections of both cognitivism and behaviorism. The concept of the task is critical in both traditions, but it is not usually thought of that way. Stimulus pool, response pool, relevant stimulus set, and procedure need little explanation. The S-R rule is an explicit statement of the relation between stimuli and

Page 3 responses and is often implicit in methods sections. In many experiments the S-R rule is the basis for scoring a response right or wrong. In other experiments, such as those in operant conditioning chambers, it is the rule specifying the occurrence of a target response and is built into the equipment. In a bar-pressing situation the bar provides much of the stimulus function as participants orient to, approach, and press the bar. Subjects can t behave coherently without stimuli to specify correct responses. The S-R rule is essentially a statement of stimulus relevance. For the subject stimuli are necessary as a basis for correct or coherent responding; for the observer they are necessary for prediction responses. The counting rule is the method of counting conjunctively relevant stimuli to assess span load. Span load is the number of stimuli conjunctively relevant for a target response. Stimulus relevance refers to situations in which a particular stimulus is necessary or sufficient for the prediction of a target response. Conjunct or joint relevance refers to situations in which two or more stimuli are conjunctively or jointly relevant. That is, situations in which two or more stimuli are singly necessary and collectively sufficient to specify a correct target response. Conjunct relevance translates roughly to the cognitive notion of the number of stimuli which must be kept in mind as a basis for correct responding. Conjunct relevance stands in contrast to redundancy in which any single stimulus of a group is sufficiently relevant for the target response. In cognitive terms, redundancy refers to situations in which attention to any single stimulus of two or more stimuli enables reliable correct responding. The task equation is a mathematical expression of the form, Performance=f(span load, span ability). The goals of span theory research include: (1) Exploration of the role of individual and developmental differences in psychometric span ability in the performance of diverse tasks (2) Development of methods of task analysis (collectively, Task Analysis of Span Load or TASL) which will lead to testable hypotheses concerning the relation between performance and span load and span ability. (3) Development and validation of applications of span theory, and (4) Development of a taxonomy of tasks.

Page 4 References Bachelder, B. L. (1970/1971). The memory span paradigm: Its use for the analysis of mental retardation. (Doctoral Dissertation, Michigan State University, 1970). Dissertation Abstracts International, 32(1), 576B-577B. Bachelder, B. L. (1974). Span ability, word frequency, and free learning among retarded adolescents. Western Carolina Center Papers and Reports, IV(16). Bachelder, B. L. (1977a, March). A research strategy for studying cognition and intelligence through study of individual differences in span ability. Paper presented at the Tenth Annual Gatlinburg Conference on Research in Mental Retardation, Gatlinburg, TN. Bachelder, B. L. (1977b). An experimental-psychometric analysis of span ability. Bachelder, B. L. (1977c). Memory span tests and the ability to imitate word sequences and sentences. Ohio Journal of Speech and Hearing, 13(1), 5-15. Bachelder, B. L. (1978, March). Span Theory: Laboratory and classroom applications. Three-Part Paper Presented at the Eleventh Annual Gatlinburg Conference on Research in Mental Retardation. Gatlinburg, TN. Bachelder, B. L. (1979, November). Span ability and retardate performance and learning in multi-trial free recall. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the Cognition Group of North Carolina, Durham, NC. Bachelder, B. L. (1980, March). Immediate memory span and multidimensional absolute judgment. Paper presented at the 13th Annual Gatlinburg Conference on Research in Mental Retardation/Developmental Disabilities, Gatlinburg, TN. Bachelder, B. L. (1981). Span theory and task complexity analysis: An introduction. Bachelder, B. L. (1999). SPAN theory is not Span Theory: A brief note. Unpublished paper. Bachelder, B. L. (2001a). The magical number 4 = 7: Span theory on capacity limitations. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(1), 116-117. Bachelder, B. L. (2001b). The magical number 7: What did Miller really prove? Bachelder, B. L. (2003). SAAS-123: A system of assessment and prescription for children and adults with moderate to profound developmental delays. Morganton, NC: Bruce L. Bachelder, PhD. Bachelder, B. L. (2005a, February). The spans question: Challenges to the memory interpretation of the so-called "memory" span test. Paper presented at the Annual Conference of the North Carolina Cognition Group, Winston-Salem, NC.

Page 5 Bachelder, B. L. (2005b, August). Crucial errors in Miller's application of information metrics in a comparison of the spans of absolute judgment and immediate memory. Paper presented at the 38th Annual Conference of the Society for Mathematical Psychology, Memphis, TN Bachelder, B. L. (2007, March). The construct validity of memory span as a measure of intelligence. Paper presented at the Annual North Carolina Cognition Conference, Chapel Hill, NC. Bachelder, B. L., & Denny, M. R. (1976, March). Span theory: A new theory of intelligence. Paper presented at the Ninth Annual Gatlinburg Conference on Research in Mental Retardation, Gatlinburg, TN. Bachelder, B. L., & Denny, M. R. (1977a). A theory of intelligence: I. Span and the complexity of stimulus control. Intelligence, 1, 127-150. Bachelder, B. L., & Denny, M. R. (1977b). A theory of intelligence: II. The role of span in a variety of intellectual tasks. Intelligence, 1, 237-256. Case, R. (1972). Validation of a new-piagetian mental capacity construct. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 14, 287-302. Case, R. (1974). Structures and strictures: Some functional limitations on the course of cognitive development. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 544-573. Case, R. (1978). Developmentally based theory and technology of instruction. Review of Educational Research, 48(3), 439-463. Case, R. (1992). The mind's staircase: Exploring the conceptual underpinnings of children's thought and knowledge. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Denny, M. R. (1966). A theoretical analysis and its application to training the mentally retarded. In N. R. Ellis (Ed.), International review of research in mental retardation (Vol. 2). (pp. 1-27). New York: Academic Press. Denny, M. R. (1980). Comparative psychology: An evolutionary analysis of animal behavior. New York: John Wiley. Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 44, 145-199. Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions (2 ed.). Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Pascual-Leone, J. (1970). A mathematical model for the transition rule in Piaget's developmental states. Acta Psychologica, 32, 301-345. Pascual-Leone, J. (1987). Organismic processes for neo-piagetian theories: A dialectical causal account of cognitive development. International Journal of Psychology, 22, 531-570.