Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude

Similar documents
The presence of binaural interaction component (BIC) in the auditory brainstem response (ABR) of normal hearing adults

THRESHOLD PREDICTION USING THE ASSR AND THE TONE BURST CONFIGURATIONS

C ritical Review: Do we see auditory system acclimatization with hearing instrument use, using electrophysiological measures?

The Verification of ABR Response by Using the Chirp Stimulus in Moderate Sensorineural Hearing Loss

MEASUREMENTS AND EQUIPMENT FOR AUDIOLOGICAL EVALUATIONS

Comparison of Binaural Interaction Component (BIC) in Symmetrical and Asymmetrical Hearing Loss: Pilot Study

James W. Hall III, Ph.D.

Wheeler, K.S. M.Cl.Sc. (Aud) Candidate School of Communication Sciences and Disorders, U.W.O

Paediatric Whitepaper

Auditory P300: Selective Attention to 2 KHZ Tone-Bursts in Patients with Idiopathic Subjective Tinnitus

EPIC-PLUS System for auditory evoked potentials and neuro-otology. PEA - VEMPs ASSR - E-ABR FACIAL NERVE

But, what about ASSR in AN?? Is it a reliable tool to estimate the auditory thresholds in those category of patients??

PERIPHERAL AND CENTRAL AUDITORY ASSESSMENT

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

Binaural Hearing. Why two ears? Definitions

KANSAS GUIDELINES FOR INFANT AUDIOLOGIC ASSESSMENT

Prediction of Loudness Growth in Subjects with Sensorineural Hearing Loss Using Auditory Steady State Response. Afaf A. Emara

Audiological Diagnosis after Newborn Screening

functional, toxicological, and pharmacological similarities between inner ear cells and cells of the kidney tubules.

Evoked Potenital Reading Session: BAEPs

EPH - International Journal of Medical and Health Science

Final Summary Project Title: Cognitive Workload During Prosthetic Use: A quantitative EEG outcome measure

Evoked Potenital Reading Session: BAEPs

Lecture Outline. The GIN test and some clinical applications. Introduction. Temporal processing. Gap detection. Temporal resolution and discrimination

Provisional guidelines for using Auditory Steady State Responses (ASSR) in babies. July 2009

Auditory Assessment Based On EEG

Neuro-Audio Version 2010

III. Comprehensive. Efficient. Auditory EP Testing

Electrophysiological Substrates of Auditory Temporal Assimilation Between Two Neighboring Time Intervals

Supplementary Online Content

ABR assesses the integrity of the peripheral auditory system and auditory brainstem pathway.

No change in the acoustic reflex threshold and auditory brainstem response following short-

Infant Hearing Aid Evaluation Using Cortical Auditory Evoked Potentials

GRASON-STADLER HEARING ASSESSMENT SYSTEMS

What Is the Difference between db HL and db SPL?

Updates to NHSP guidance for post-screening diagnostic testing Update 1: August 2015 Author: BSA Electrophysiology Special Interest Group (BSA EP SIG)

Using Electrocochleography to Assess the Afferent Pathway in the Cochlea. Senior Thesis

MENTAL WORKLOAD AS A FUNCTION OF TRAFFIC DENSITY: COMPARISON OF PHYSIOLOGICAL, BEHAVIORAL, AND SUBJECTIVE INDICES

The Impact of Presentation Level on SCAN A Test Performance

Contents. Exercises and Questions. Answers

ORIGINAL ARTICLE. Auditory Sensitivity in Children Using the Auditory Steady-State Response

Technical Report: Distortion Product Otoacoustic Emissions That Are Not Outer Hair Cell Emissions DOI: /jaaa

Hello Old Friend the use of frequency specific speech phonemes in cortical and behavioural testing of infants

Audiology Curriculum Post-Foundation Course Topic Summaries

Twenty subjects (11 females) participated in this study. None of the subjects had

ABR PTA ASSR Multi-ASSR OAE TEOAE DPOAE SOAE VEMP ECochG MLR P300

Application of a Stimulus Spectral Calibration Routine to Click Evoked Otoacoustic Emissions

Protocol for Fast, Efficient Audiogram Prediction using Electrophysiology. Sponsored By

Quick Guide - eabr with Eclipse

REFERRAL AND DIAGNOSTIC EVALUATION OF HEARING ACUITY. Better Hearing Philippines Inc.

Simultaneous multiple stimulation of the auditory steadystate response (ASSR)

Thresholds of Tone Burst Auditory Brainstem Responses for Infants and Young Children with Normal Hearing in Taiwan

Otoacoustic Emissions as a Cross-Check in Pediatric Hearing Assessment : Case Report

THE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA AT CHAPEL HILL

VEMP: Vestibular Evoked Myogenic Potential

PC BASED AUDIOMETER GENERATING AUDIOGRAM TO ASSESS ACOUSTIC THRESHOLD

C HAPTER FOUR. Audiometric Configurations in Children. Andrea L. Pittman. Introduction. Methods

The CON-SOT-LOT Test for Nonorganic Hearing Loss

Bone Conduction Microphone: A Head Mapping Pilot Study

AccuScreen ABR Screener

The neural code for interaural time difference in human auditory cortex

A FRESH Approach to Pediatric Behavioral Testing

Normative Behavioral Thresholds for Short Tone-Bursts

A NORMATIVE STUDY ON AIR AND BONE CONDUCTION OCULAR VESTIBULAR EVOKED MYOGENIC POTENTIALS. Ho Sen Kee

PURPOSE OF THIS PRESENTATION GUIDELINES DEVELOPMENT CONFERENCE GUIDELINES DEVELOPMENT CONFERENCE: FACULTY GUIDELINES DEVELOPMENT CONFERENCE: OUTCOMES

DSL v5 in Connexx 7 Mikael Menard, Ph.D., Philippe Lantin Sivantos, 2015.

Background. Electrophysical tests. HearLab Instrument. - Developed by National Acoustic Laboratories (NAL) - NAL- (Australian population)

Auditory Steady-State Response (ASSR): A Beginner's Guide by Douglas L. Beck, AuD, David P. Speidel, MS, Michelle Petrak, PhD

Newborn Screening and Middle Ear Problems

Signals, systems, acoustics and the ear. Week 1. Laboratory session: Measuring thresholds

Proceedings of Meetings on Acoustics

PSYCHOMETRIC VALIDATION OF SPEECH PERCEPTION IN NOISE TEST MATERIAL IN ODIA (SPINTO)

Hearing Screening, Diagnostics and Intervention

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Ear Hear. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2008 June 27.

Normal Ipsilateral/Contralateral Asymmetries in Infant Multiple Auditory Steady-State Responses to Air- and Bone-Conduction Stimuli

AA-M1C1. Audiometer. Space saving is achieved with a width of about 350 mm. Audiogram can be printed by built-in printer

Clinical Forum. Detecting and Remediating External Meatal Collapse during Audiologic Assessment. Levi A. Reiter* Shlomo Silmant

CE-Chirp. Making ABR fast, accurate and clear! Stimulus Family

Infants, auditory steady-state responses (ASSRs), and clinical practice

Hearing Evaluation: Diagnostic Approach

Takwa Adly Gabr Assistant lecturer of Audiology

The Level Specific CE-Chirp

INTRODUCTION TO PURE (AUDIOMETER & TESTING ENVIRONMENT) TONE AUDIOMETERY. By Mrs. Wedad Alhudaib with many thanks to Mrs.

BIO-LOGIC PRO NAVIGATOR TRUSTED TECHNOLOGY. RELIABLE RESULTS.

EFFECTS OF NITROUS OXIDE ON AUDITORY CORTICAL EVOKED POTENTIALS AND SUBJECTIVE THRESHOLDS

Educational Module Tympanometry. Germany D Germering

Acoustic Immittance (ME measures) Tympanometery

Air conduction hearing thresholds of young and older Japanese adults for pure tones from 125 Hz to 16 khz

SPPA 332 Audiological Procedures, 3 Semester Credits Spring 2015 M-W-F 8:30 A.M. BH 181

Topic 4. Pitch & Frequency

***This is a self-archiving copy and does not fully replicate the published version*** Auditory Temporal Processes in the Elderly

Ear and Hearing (In press, 2005)

Reward prediction error signals associated with a modified time estimation task

Acoustic-Immittance Characteristics of Children with Middle-ear Effusion : Longitudinal Investigation

An Update on Auditory Neuropathy Spectrum Disorder in Children

Introduction 2/10/2012. C. G. Marx, Au. D., CCC A Edward Goshorn, Ph.D., CCC A/SLP Alaina Simmons, B.S.

Clinical application of tympanometry in the topic diagnosis of hearing loss A study from Bulgaria

Prescribe hearing aids to:

Variation in spectral-shape discrimination weighting functions at different stimulus levels and signal strengths

Evaluation of the hearing function in individuals wearing

Transcription:

University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School 2004 Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude Tim Skinner University of South Florida Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd Part of the American Studies Commons Scholar Commons Citation Skinner, Tim, "Effect of intensity increment on P300 amplitude" (2004). Graduate Theses and Dissertations. http://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/1251 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact scholarcommons@usf.edu.

THE UNIVERSITY OF SOUTH FLORIDA COLLEGE OF ARTS AND SCIENCES Effect of Intensity Increment on P300 Amplitude By Tim Skinner An Audiology Doctoral Project submitted to the Faculty of the Department of the Communication Sciences and Disorders University of South Florida In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Audiology Raymond M. Hurley, Ph.D., Chair Jennifer J. Lister, Ph.D. Robert F. Zelski, Au.D. Date Tampa, Florida Version 6.0

Effect of Intensity Increment on P300 Amplitude 2. Tim Skinner (Abstract) The purpose of this study was to determine the effects of task difficulty on the amplitude and latency of the P300 by altering the intensity of the oddball stimulus. A P300 was obtained on 22 adult subjects ranging in age from 21 to 34 years of age (mean = 24 years) with normal hearing. The frequent stimulus was a 1000 Hz or 4000 Hz tone burst, gated with a rise and fall time of 10 msec and 20 msec plateau, presented at 75 dbn HL The oddball stimulus was a tone burst of the same frequency (1000 Hz or 4000 Hz)presented at 77, 79, or 81 dbn HL. A four-channel recording was made with linked reference electrodes and the following montages:cz-a1+a2, Pz-A1+A2, and Fz-A1+A2. The fourth channel was used to monitor "eye blink" activity. The investigation tested the null hypothesis that changing the intensity of the oddball stimuli would not result in a significant change in either the amplitude or latency of the P300. Analyses of Variance (ANOVA) indicate that P300 latency and amplitude did not differ significantly by run, stimulus frequency, intensity of the oddball, or montage. Thus the null hypothesis was supported.

3. Introduction The P300 (P3) is an auditory evoked potential (AEP) referred to as a cognitive or event-related response occurring in the 300 msec latency region with a large positive voltage peak, hence P, after an acoustic stimulus. Like most long latency potentials (LLP), the P300 is an endogenous response, highly dependent upon subject attention to auditory stimuli. The P300 is typically recorded with the patient attending or listening for a rare, oddball, or target stimulus that is presented along with frequent stimuli. The stimulus that occurs the majority of the time is the frequent stimulus and the infrequent stimulus is known as the oddball. In the oddball test paradigm, two stimuli are presented with one occurring between 80% and 85% of the time and the other occurring between 15% and 20% of the time. The participant is asked to respond, usually by counting out loud or by pressing a button, when the oddball stimuli is perceived. The major peak is a large positive voltage (5 µv) occurring approximately 300 msec after the rare or "oddball" response (Polich, 1996). The P300 response is believed to reflect processing at the medial temporal lobe (hippocampus); however, the exact neural generators are not clear at this time (Molnar, 1994). It is known that the P300 response changes throughout the lifespan. Full maturity of the P300 does not occur until about 15 years of age. From birth through 15 years of age, latencies decrease, while amplitudes increase. The reverse is true from about 40 years of age through death; latencies slowly increase and amplitudes decrease. The P300 is classified as an endogenous potential, meaning that it originates from within the subject and is dependent on the subject attending to or processing the stimuli. (Halgren, Marinkovic, & Chauvel, 1998). Unlike the auditory brainstem response (ABR), the P300 is not directly impacted by the stimulus characteristics. Attention and state of arousal are the two most important factors in eliciting a P300 response. In order to adequately assess the P300 response,

4. the subject must actively attend to the oddball stimulus and be able to discriminate it from the frequent stimulus. If the subject is unable to discriminate the oddball from the frequent stimulus, then the P300 will not be present (Hall, 1992). As the stimulus, both frequent and oddball, are reduced in intensity towards threshold, the amplitude of the P300 decreases (Papanicolaou et al., 1985; Covington & Polich, 1996; Sugg & Polich, 1995). Therefore, most P300 protocols use a 75 to 80 db nhl stimulus intensity to decrease amplitude variability (Polich, 1998). P300 amplitudes and latencies are used clinically to assess patients with Alzheimer s Disease, Parkinson s Disease, and dementia. Patients with these neurodegenerative disorders tend to have prolonged P300 latencies, believed to be related to changes in neurotransmitters (Kugler, Taghavy, & Platt, 1993). The P300 can be extremely valuable tool when evaluating general cognitive function (Polich & Herbst, 2000). P300 latencies have been shown to increase, while amplitudes decrease, with decreases in cognitive function (Polich, Howard, & Starr, 1983). It is important, from both clinical and research perspectives, to design paradigms that will improve the utility of the P300. In 1995, Sugg and Polich found that the discrimination task needs to be difficult enough to elicit a P300 response. They found that if the discrimination task is too simple (> 7 db or >50 Hz), the amplitude decreases while the latency increases. The P300 also becomes less reliable outside of those parameters. Since the characteristics of the P300 are affected by cognitive processing, theoretically, the more difficult the oddball task, the greater the amplitude of the P300 response. The proposed study was designed to determine if a simple psychoacoustic ability can be measured electrophysiologically. The psychoacoustic ability of interest for this study was intensity discrimination. Intensity discrimination thresholds are dependent on the frequency and sensation levels of the stimuli (Turner, Zwislocki, & Filion, 1989). Intensity difference limens decrease as the sensation levels are increased (Yost, 1994). The intensity discrimination for a normal hearing

5. adult for a stimulus with a sensation level above 60 db HL is less than 1 db (Gelfand, 1998). The purpose of this study is to determine the effects of changing the difficulty of the discrimination task on the amplitude and latency of the P300 by altering the intensity of the oddball stimulus. Methods Participants Twenty-two participants (5 males and 17 females) between the ages of 21 and 34 years (mean = 24 years of age) participated in this study. All participants in this study were required to have normal hearing and normal middle ear function with no known neurological or cognitive disorders. Normal hearing was defined as air conduction thresholds at or below 20 db HL for the frequencies 500 Hz, 1000 Hz, 2000 Hz, and 4000 Hz in each ear (American National Standard Institute, 1996; American Speech-Language and Hearing Association, 1985). Normal middle ear function was defined as an ear canal volume between 0.5 ml and 2.0 ml, a peak pressure between 150 dapa and +100 dapa, and a compliance value between 0.2 ml and 1.8 ml. Contralateral acoustic reflexes were measured and were required to fall within normal limits determined by the published acoustic reflex threshold 90% cutoff values (Silman & Gelfand, 1981). These criteria for inclusion were used in order to make the group as homogenous as possible. The participants were not financially compensated. Instrumentation Air conduction thresholds were measured using an Interacoustics (Model AC40) audiometer and middle ear function was determined using a GSI Tympstar. For P300 measures, stimulus presentation and signal averaging were controlled by a Nicolet Spirit evoked potential system. All equipment used was calibrated according to ANSI specifications.

Materials and Procedures 6. The hearing test and middle ear measures were conducted at the Communication Sciences and Disorders Audiology Clinic at the University of South Florida. The P300 testing was conducted in a research laboratory in the Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders using a Nicolet Spirit evoked potential system. The frequent stimulus used was a 1000 Hz or 4000 Hz tone burst, gated with a rise and fall time of 10 msec and a 20 msec plateau, presented at 75 dbn HL. The oddball stimulus was a tone burst of the same frequency (1000 or 4000 Hz) presented at 77, 79, or 81 dbn HL. The frequent tone bursts were presented in a stimulus train with a probability of 80% while the oddball tone bursts occurred with a probability of 20% within the stimulus train. Rarefaction was used as the polarity and the time window was 600 msec. The responses were band pass filtered between 1 and 30 Hz. Thirty responses for each oddball were averaged and constituted one run. The tone bursts were presented binaurally through ER-3 insert earphones at a rate of 0.77 per second. A four-channel recording was made with linked reference electrodes and the following montage: Cz-A1+A2, Pz- A1+A2, and Fz-A1+A2. The fourth channel was used to monitor "eye blink" activity. If the amplitude of the fourth channel exceeded 5 µv in the region of the P300, the run was discarded. The forehead (Fpz) was used as the ground. Impedance was maintained below 2000 ohms. The P300 measures took place in a double walled IAC sound treated room. The starting oddball intensity was counterbalanced. Participants were instructed to respond to the oddball stimulus by pressing a button that they were given. The patient response button was unattached, but the participant did not have knowledge of this. P300 amplitude and latency for each oddball were recorded for each subject. All subjects were involved in two runs for each stimulus frequency and intensity combination for a total of 12 runs. All of the runs and the initial hearing test were completed in one session.

7. Results Figure 1 displays the mean audiometric thresholds for all participants. Figures 2a and 2b display the mean (±1 SE) amplitude for each intensity level by electrode montage. In order to determine 0 5 Frequency (Hz) 1000 10000 Left Ear Right Ear db HTL 10 15 20 25 Figure 1. Mean (±1 SD) hearing thresholds levels (HTLs) for all participants if these mean data were significantly different, the data were analyzed using a 4-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) with four within subjects variables (run, frequency, intensity of the oddball, and montage). For amplitude, only one interaction reached statistical significance, that between run and frequency [F (1,21)= 4.33, p = 0.05]. A Tukey post-hoc analysis of the significant interaction between run and frequency revealed that none of the possible combinations of run and frequency differed significantly from each other (p>0.05).

8. Amplitude (µv) 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 77 db 1 khz 79 db 1 khz 81 db 1 khz Cz-A1+A2 Fz-A1+A2 Pz-A1+A2 Montage Amplitude (µv) 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 0 77 db 1 khz 79 db 1 khz 81 db 1 khz Cz-A1+A2 Fz-A1+A2 Pz-A1+A2 Montage Figures 2a and 2b. Mean (±1 SE) P300 amplitude at the three electrode montages for the three intensity levels at 1000 and 4000 Hz.

9. Figures 3a and 3b illustrate the mean latency for each intensity level by electrode montage. In order to determine if these mean data were significantly different, the data were analyzed using a 4-way ANOVA with four within subjects variables (run, frequency, intensity of the oddball, and montage). None of the main effects (run, frequency, intensity of the oddball, and montage) or interactions were significant (p > 0.05). 400 350 77 db 1 khz 79 db 1 khz 81 db 1kHz 300 Latency (msec) 250 200 150 100 50 0 Cz-A1+A2 Fz-A1+A2 Pz-A1+A2 Montage Figures 3a. Mean (±1 SE) P300 latency at the three electrode montages for the three intensity levels at 1000 Hz. The SE values are so small that the error bars are not visible on this figure.

10. 400 350 77 db 4 khz 79 db 4 khz 81 db 4 khz 300 Latency (msec) 250 200 150 100 50 0 Cz-A1+A2 Fz-A1+A2 Pz-A1+A2 Montage Figure 3b. Mean (±1 SE) P300 latency at the three electrode montages for the three intensity levels at 4000 Hz. The SE values are so small that the error bars are not visible on this figure. Figures 4 and 5 display the mean (±1 SE) test retest raw data for run 1 versus run 2 for amplitude and latency respectively. There were no significant differences between run 1 and run 2 for either amplitude or latency as analyzed using the two 4-way ANOVAs described above.

Amplitude (µv) 10 7.5 5 2.5 Test 1 khz Test 4 khz Re-Test 1 khz Re-Test 4 khz 11. 0 77 79 81 Intensity (db) Figure 4. Mean (±1 SE) P300 test-retest amplitude for the three intensity levels at 1000 Hz and 4000 Hz. P300 Latency (msec) 400 350 300 250 200 150 100 50 0 Test 1 khz Re-Test 1 khz Test 4 khz 77 79 81 Intensity (db) Re-Test 4 khz Figure 5. Mean (±1 SE) P300 test-retest latency for the three intensity levels at 1000 Hz and 4000 Hz. The SE values are so small that the error bars are not visible on this figure.

12. There were no statistically significant effects seen in any of the analyses of the data associated with this experiment. We originally hypothesized that changing the intensity of the oddball stimuli would not result in a change in either the amplitude of the latency of the P300. According to the data and analysis above, our hypothesis is supported. Discussion Though it has been proposed that the amplitude and latency of the P300 is affected by the difficulty of a discrimination task, this study has found that this is not the case (Sugg & Polich, 1995). Previous studies have demonstrated that the P300 is influenced by cognitive factors (Polich & Herbst, 2000; Kugler, Taghavy, & Platt, 1993), suggesting a potential clinical application. This study explored the clinical utility of the P300. This study investigated the effect of the magnitude of the intensity difference between the frequent and oddball stimulus on the amplitude and latency of the P300. The results indicated that the intensity difference between the frequent and oddball stimulus had no effect on either the latency or the amplitude of the P300. Further, the P300 did not vary as a function of the electrode montage. There several possible reasons that the manipulation of the intensity difference between the frequent stimulus and the oddball stimulus did not affect the amplitude or the latency of the P300. First, the P300 is believed to reflect processing that occurs primarily at the level of the medial temporal lobe (Molnar, 1994). The intensity discrimination task used in this study may be processed at a lower level of the auditory pathway. If the stimulus is processed at a lower level, then the P300 may be unaffected. Second, the intensity discrimination task may not have been difficult enough to make it cognitively challenging enough to affect the P300. Different psychoacoustic tasks should be used in future testing to assess the hypothesis of this study.

Summary 13. The present study suggests that the P300 is unaffected by changes in the intensity difference between the frequent and oddball stimulus. More research is needed to completely understand the neural generators that are involved in the formation of the P300. Different paradigms also need to be designed to help increase the clinical utility of the P300. Although the intensity discrimination task did not result in any changes to the P300, other tasks such as frequency discrimination or gap detection may prove to be useful.

14. References American National Standards Institute. (1996). American national standards specifications for audiometers (ANSI S3.6-1996). New York: Author. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association. (1985). Guidelines for identification audiometry. ASHA 27(5), 49-52. Covington, J., & Polich, J. (1996). P300, stimulus intensity, and modality. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 100, 579-584. Gelfand, S. (1998). Hearing: An introduction to psychological and physiological acoustics. New York: Marcel Dekker. Halgren, E., Marinkovic, K., & Chauvel, P. (1998). Generators of the late cognitive potentials in auditory and visual oddball tasks. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 106, 156-164. Hall, J. (1992). Handbook of auditory evoked responses. Needham Heights, Massachusetts: Allyn and Bacon. Katzman, R. (1987). Aging and dementia symposium abstract. Neuroscience, 22: S1. Kugler, C.F.A., Taghavy, A., & Platt, D. (1993). The event-related P300 potential analysis of cognitive human brain aging: A review. Gerontology, 39, 280-303. Molnar, M. (1994). On the origin of the P3 event-related potential component. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 17, 129-134. Papanicolaou, A., Loring, D.,Raz, N., & Eisenberg, H. (1985). Relationship between stimulus intensity and the P300. Psychophysiology, 27, 326-329. Polich, J. (1996). Meta-analysis of P300 normative aging studies. Psychophysiology, 33, 334-353.

Polich, J. (1998). P300 clinical utility and control of variability. Journal of Clinical 15. Neurophysiology, 15, 14-33. Polich, J., & Herbst, K. (2000). P300 as a clinical assay: Rationale, evaluation, and findings. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 38, 3-19. Polich, J., Howard, L., & Starr, A. (1983). P300 latency correlates with digit span. Psychophysiology, 20, 665-669. Silman, S., & Gelfand, S.A. (1981). The relationship between magnitude of hearing loss and acoustic reflex threshold levels. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 46, 312-316. Sugg, M., & Polich, J. (1995). P300 from auditory stimuli: Intensity and frequency effects. Biological Psychology, 41, 255-269. Turner, C., Zwislocki, J., & Filion, P. (1989). Intensity discrimination determined with two paradigms in normal and hearing impaired subjects. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 86, 109-115. Yost, W. (1994). Fundamentals of hearing: An introduction. London, England: Academic Press.