Introduction to the Special Issue on Multimodality of Early Sensory Processing: Early Visual Maps Flexibly Encode Multimodal Space

Similar documents
Plasticity of Cerebral Cortex in Development

Early Stages of Vision Might Explain Data to Information Transformation

Retinotopy & Phase Mapping

How do individuals with congenital blindness form a conscious representation of a world they have never seen? brain. deprived of sight?

Chapter 5. Summary and Conclusions! 131

Multimodal interactions: visual-auditory

Neural Correlates of Human Cognitive Function:

Visual cortical plasticity

MULTI-CHANNEL COMMUNICATION

THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES

Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience. Jan 27

Cognitive Modelling Themes in Neural Computation. Tom Hartley

Selective bias in temporal bisection task by number exposition

Physiology of Tactile Sensation

Cognitive Neuroscience Section 4

An Account of the Neural Basis and Functional Relevance of Early Cross-Modal Interactions

Livingston American School Quarterly Lesson Plan

5th Mini-Symposium on Cognition, Decision-making and Social Function: In Memory of Kang Cheng

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

Cortical Map Plasticity. Gerald Finnerty Dept Basic and Clinical Neuroscience

LEAH KRUBITZER RESEARCH GROUP LAB PUBLICATIONS WHAT WE DO LINKS CONTACTS

Lateral Geniculate Nucleus (LGN)

Theoretical Neuroscience: The Binding Problem Jan Scholz, , University of Osnabrück

Inner Vision: Seeing the Mind s Eye

Clusters, Symbols and Cortical Topography

VISUAL CORTICAL PLASTICITY

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

EDGE DETECTION. Edge Detectors. ICS 280: Visual Perception

The previous three chapters provide a description of the interaction between explicit and

Basics of Computational Neuroscience

The Visual System. Cortical Architecture Casagrande February 23, 2004

Vision Science (VIS SCI)

Ch 5. Perception and Encoding

Introduction and Historical Background. August 22, 2007

The Integration of Features in Visual Awareness : The Binding Problem. By Andrew Laguna, S.J.

Hebbian Plasticity for Improving Perceptual Decisions

Frank Tong. Department of Psychology Green Hall Princeton University Princeton, NJ 08544

CPC journal club archive

Cognitive Penetrability and the Content of Perception

The Perceptual Experience

COGNITIVE SCIENCE 107A. Hippocampus. Jaime A. Pineda, Ph.D.

Introduction to sensory pathways. Gatsby / SWC induction week 25 September 2017

Carnegie Mellon University Annual Progress Report: 2011 Formula Grant

Sensation vs. Perception

Neurophysiology and Information

Studying the time course of sensory substitution mechanisms (CSAIL, 2014)

The Central Nervous System

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Requirements for Modeling the Two Spatial Visual Systems

The Standard Theory of Conscious Perception

Models of Attention. Models of Attention

Chapter One- Introduction to Cognitive Psychology

Visual Object Recognition Computational Models and Neurophysiological Mechanisms Neurobiology 130/230. Harvard College/GSAS 78454

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017

PSYCHOLOGY. Part/Unit Topics Marks. A Theory What is Psychology 7. 2 Methods of Enquiry in Psychology The bases of Human Behaviour 8

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Cognitive Neuroscience History of Neural Networks in Artificial Intelligence The concept of neural network in artificial intelligence

Psychology Scientific Inquiry Domain Research Methods, Measurement, and Statistics

Prof. Greg Francis 7/31/15

Lateral view of human brain! Cortical processing of touch!

Wetware: The Biological Basis of Intellectual Giftedness

Sensory Physiology Bi353 Fall Term 2016

USING AUDITORY SALIENCY TO UNDERSTAND COMPLEX AUDITORY SCENES

Brook's Image Scanning Experiment & Neuropsychological Evidence for Spatial Rehearsal

Sensory coding and somatosensory system

lateral organization: maps

Computational Cognitive Neuroscience (CCN)

Myers Psychology for AP*

PS3021, PS3022, PS4040

PHGY 210,2,4 - Physiology SENSORY PHYSIOLOGY. Martin Paré

Erasmus & Visiting Students: Modules & Assessments

Complementarity and the Relation Between Psychological and Neurophysiological Phenomena

Early Interaction between Vision and Touch during Binocular Rivalry

LISC-322 Neuroscience Cortical Organization

9.14 Classes #21-23: Visual systems

Cortical Visual Symptoms

Mirror neurons. Romana Umrianova

TMS Disruption of Time Encoding in Human Primary Visual Cortex Molly Bryan Beauchamp Lab

Sensory substitution, and the third kind of qualia*

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Photoreceptors Rods. Cones

Selective Attention. Modes of Control. Domains of Selection

Spontaneous Cortical Activity Reveals Hallmarks of an Optimal Internal Model of the Environment. Berkes, Orban, Lengyel, Fiser.

Parallel processing strategies of the primate visual system

Neuroscience Tutorial

PSYC 223 BIOLOGICAL PSYCHOLOGY

Thalamocortical Feedback and Coupled Oscillators

23 Problems in Systems Neuroscience

Retinal Waves and Ocular Dominance Columns

Activity-Dependent Development II April 25, 2007 Mu-ming Poo

The How of Tactile Sensation

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

Oscillatory Neural Network for Image Segmentation with Biased Competition for Attention

Memory, Attention, and Decision-Making

Reading Assignments: Lecture 5: Introduction to Vision. None. Brain Theory and Artificial Intelligence

Consciousness The final frontier!

P. Hitchcock, Ph.D. Department of Cell and Developmental Biology Kellogg Eye Center. Wednesday, 16 March 2009, 1:00p.m. 2:00p.m.

ASSAF HAREL, PHD CURRICULUM VITAE Department of Psychology Wright State University

Goals. Visual behavior jobs of vision. The visual system: overview of a large-scale neural architecture. kersten.org

35 Gedney Rd

Transcription:

BRILL Multisensory Research 28 (2015) 249 252 brill.com/msr Introduction to the Special Issue on Multimodality of Early Sensory Processing: Early Visual Maps Flexibly Encode Multimodal Space Roberto Arrighi1, Paola Binda2 * and Guido Marco Cicchini 1University of Florence, Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Pharmacology and Child Health, Florence, Italy 2 University of Pisa, Department of Translational Research on New Technologies in Medicine and Surgery, Pisa, Italy 3 CNR, Institute of Neuroscience, Pisa, Italy As living organisms, we have the capability to explore our environments through different senses, each making use of specialized organs and returning unique information. This is relayed to a set of cortical areas, each of which appears to be specialized for processing information from a single sense hence the definition of unisensory areas. Many models assume that primary unisensory cortices passively reproduce information from each sensory organ; these then project to associative areas, which actively combine multisensory signals with each other and with cognitive stances. By the same token, the textbook view holds that sensory cortices undergo plastic changes only within a limited critical period ; their function and architecture should remain stable and unchangeable thereafter. This model has led to many fundamental discoveries on the architecture of the sensory systems (e.g., oriented receptive fields, binocularity, topographic maps, to name just the best known). However, a growing body of evidence calls for a review of this conceptual scheme. Based on single-cell recordings from non-human primates, fmri in humans, psychophysics, and sensory deprivation studies, early sensory areas are losing their status of fixed readouts of receptor activity; they are turning into functional nodes in a network of brain areas that flexibly adapts to the statistics of the input and the behavioral goals. This special issue in Multisensory Research To whom correspondence should be addressed. E-mail: p.bindal @in.cnr.it Koninklijke Brill NV, Leiden, 2015 DOI: 10.1163/22134808-00002500

250 R. Arrighi et al. / Multisensory Research 28 (2015) 249-252 aims to cover three such lines of evidence: suggesting that (1) the flexibility of spatial representations, (2) adult plasticity and (3) multimodality, are not properties of associative areas alone, but may depend on the primary visual cortex V 1. 1. Flexible Spatial Vision Size constancy, or knowing that an elephant is always larger than a mouse even if their retinal images may be matched in size, is a fundamental visual ability. The key to perceptual constancy is the ability to dissociate the representations sub-serving perception from the proximal stimulus, e.g., the retinal image; this implies integrating retinal information with extra-retinal (contextual, motor, cognitive) cues. Sperandio and Chouinard (in this special issue) review strong evidence for this multimodal integration to occur as early as in V 1, affecting the basic receptive field properties of a large fraction of neurons. Neuroimaging studies have repeatedly shown that VI, far from being a mirror of the retinal image, provides a representation of space that is close to what we perceive more often than not, the location and size of fmri activations in V 1 match the perceived location and size of objects, not the proximal stimulus. Is this flexibility of spatial maps an intrinsic process of V 1? Or does it depend on feedback from higher levels? This remains an open question, which is directly addressed in Schwarzkopf s review in this issue. Starting from the intriguing finding of a correlation between susceptibility to size illusions and individual s V 1 surface area, his review offers suggestions for exploiting a range of experimental approaches to disentangle the contributions of V1 and other cortical areas in mediating spatial perception. 2. Adult Plasticity If the layout of V 1 activity can change depending on perceptual demands, then retinotopic maps cannot be rigidly determined by the anatomy of retinothalamic pathways; they must retain a level of flexibility. How does this adult flexibility compare to the plasticity that the cortex displays during the critical period? To address this issue, Haak, Morland and Engel (this issue) juxtapose the effects of sensory deprivation in the adult vs. developing brain, highlighting the relative stability of the adult brain when deprived of oriented input (long-term orientation adaptation) or suffering a local loss of vision (retinal scotoma). The claim is that plastic changes can occur, possibly even in early sensory areas like VI, but they are generally not maintained as a stable feature of the system. This may reflect the cost-benefit trade off between reallocating resources to optimize sensory processing and the coding catastrophe that may result when information is passed along to higher areas. Biasing this trade

R. Arrighi et al. / Multisensory Research 28 (2015) 249-252 251 off in favor of plasticity is the aim of many rehabilitative approaches for ameliorating visual deficits. One possibility is that a residual plasticity potential is more prominent in areas where the sensory deprivation (and the necessary neural reorganization) is less dramatic for example, in the transition areas bordering the retinal scotomata. This hypothesis has guided the work on a large cohort of patients with heterogeneous deficits, led by the group of Bernhard Sahel, reviewed in this issue by Turco, Albamonte, Ricci, Fortini and Mariamore. Keeping in mind the varied outcome of these rehabilitative attempts, and more generally the potential for plastic reorganization of the adult cortex, is important, especially as sight-restoration technology marches on at a rapid pace. 3. Multimodality Of course, while the adult plasticity potential is somewhat debated and generally small, dramatic reorganization of sensory areas results from early and congenital deprivations. Part of this reorganization involves the rerouting of preserved sensory inputs to deprived primary cortical areas, e.g., auditory input to VI or visual input to A l. Considering these results, one may come to a profoundly new characterization of primary cortices. Rather than being identified by their dominant input, they may instead be better defined by their function: VI as a spatial map, Al as a temporal map. These maps are usually populated with visual/auditory signals, but their potential may also be fulfilled with information from other modalities. To what extent is this strategy part of the regular functioning of adult brains with intact sensory experience? Azevedo, Ortiz-Rios, Li, Logothetis and Keliris address this point in this issue, starting from a description of the interactions between visual and auditory signals in the spatial domain and noting how these may imply early and immediate integration of audio-visual information, which may be instantiated in VI. This leads to the more general issue of whether the integration of multisensory cues occurs at an early level of processing or at a later, cognitive stage. Besides investigating this question at a neurophysiological level, two behavioral approaches have established their utility over the past decades. Each is instantiated in the two original research papers in this special issue. Ho, Orchard-Mills and Alais adopt one such classic approach, which entails testing for spatial specificity of multisensory interactions specifically temporal recalibrations. The rationale is that, if the integration is due to centralized cognitive processes, then it should generalize across spatial positions; spatial specificity, on the other hand, speaks against a cognitive-level hypothesis. The other traditional approach focuses on mandatory interferences across sensory modalities whereby strict modularity predicts little cross-sensory interference. This approach, forms the basis for Wahn and Konig s work. Both these ex-

252 R. Arrighi et al. / Multisensory Research 28 (2015) 249-252 perimental efforts lead to the suggestion that the ability to combine cues from different sources pervades all levels of sensory processing, both early and late. 4. Conclusions Starting from the consideration of three very different problems, flexible spatial vision, adult plasticity and multisensory processing, the papers in this special issue converge on a similar solution: that multimodal, flexible integration of sensory information may not be found only at high-level, associative stages of sensory processing. At the same time, the special issue highlights the importance of acknowledging the limits of low-level flexibility, as cortical reorganization and sensory rehabilitation may in some cases have only partial or no success. By combining the evaluation of bold new ideas with critical reviews of the literature, the papers in this special issue provide a strong impulse towards establishing a new theoretical framework for multisensory processing and its plasticity. Acknowledgements The editing of this special issue was funded by the project Futuro in Ricerca from the Italian Ministry of University and Research, under grant agreement n. RBFR1332DJ, and by the European Research Council (FPT/2007-2013), under grant agreement n. 338866.

Copyright of Multisensory Research is the property of Brill Academic Publishers and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.