Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed

Similar documents
Values and Environmental Behaviours. Judith de Groot

Study Guide #2: MULTIPLE REGRESSION in education

Title: The Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) and Texting While Driving Behavior in College Students MS # Manuscript ID GCPI

Media, Discussion and Attitudes Technical Appendix. 6 October 2015 BBC Media Action Andrea Scavo and Hana Rohan

Correlation and Regression

Study of Meditational Role of Self-Esteem in the Relationship Between Perfectionism and Competitive Anxiety Elite Athletes

Supporting Information

Assessing the Validity and Reliability of the Teacher Keys Effectiveness. System (TKES) and the Leader Keys Effectiveness System (LKES)

Basic concepts and principles of classical test theory

isc ove ring i Statistics sing SPSS

Seeing the world through a lens: How do people form opinions on climate change?

2008 Ohio State University. Campus Climate Study. Prepared by. Student Life Research and Assessment

Kuusisto, E., Gholami, K., Schutte, I.W., Wolfensberger, M.V.C., & Tirri, K. (2014).

Understanding Tourist Environmental Behavior An Application of the Theories on Reasoned Action Approach

Effect of job characteristics on satisfaction and performance: A test in Egyptian agricultural extension system

VALUES AND ENTREPRENEURIAL ATTITUDE AS PREDICTORS OF NASCENT ENTREPRENEUR INTENTIONS

Development of a measure of self-regulated practice behavior in skilled performers

CHAPTER VI RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

Daniel Boduszek University of Huddersfield

Overview of Lecture. Survey Methods & Design in Psychology. Correlational statistics vs tests of differences between groups

University of Groningen

ANSWERS TO EXERCISES AND REVIEW QUESTIONS

Analysis of Confidence Rating Pilot Data: Executive Summary for the UKCAT Board

Measurement Error 2: Scale Construction (Very Brief Overview) Page 1

UBC Social Ecological Economic Development Studies (SEEDS) Student Report

investigate. educate. inform.

Supplemental Materials: Facing One s Implicit Biases: From Awareness to Acknowledgment

DISCPP (DISC Personality Profile) Psychometric Report

Psychometrics in context: Test Construction with IRT. Professor John Rust University of Cambridge

PUBLIC KNOWLEDGE AND ATTITUDES SCALE CONSTRUCTION: DEVELOPMENT OF SHORT FORMS

CHAPTER III RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

What Causes Stress in Malaysian Students and it Effect on Academic Performance: A case Revisited

China January 2009 International Business Trip Analysis

Distinct profiles of reactive and proactive aggression in adolescents: associations with cognitive and affective empathy

Chapter 3 - Does Low Well-being Modify the Effects of

Adaptation of immigrant middle school students: The role of perceived discrimination against the self and against the group, and of acculturation

IDEA Technical Report No. 20. Updated Technical Manual for the IDEA Feedback System for Administrators. Stephen L. Benton Dan Li

Development of self efficacy and attitude toward analytic geometry scale (SAAG-S)

The relationship between organizational justice and organizational citizenship behavior

Online Appendix. According to a recent survey, most economists expect the economic downturn in the United

validscale: A Stata module to validate subjective measurement scales using Classical Test Theory

CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY. In this chapter, research design, data collection, sampling frame and analysis

RESPONSE SURFACE MODELING AND OPTIMIZATION TO ELUCIDATE THE DIFFERENTIAL EFFECTS OF DEMOGRAPHIC CHARACTERISTICS ON HIV PREVALENCE IN SOUTH AFRICA

International Conference on Humanities and Social Science (HSS 2016)

Self-Compassion, Perceived Academic Stress, Depression and Anxiety Symptomology Among Australian University Students

CHAPTER 3. Research Methodology

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model

Journal of Environmental Psychology

Teachers Sense of Efficacy Scale: The Study of Validity and Reliability

Student: Judy Middelburg ANR: Project theme: Strengths intervention. Project period: January 2014 summer 2014

Packianathan Chelladurai Troy University, Troy, Alabama, USA.

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Keywords: consultation, drug-related problems, pharmacists, Theory of Planned Behavior

The relation between self-efficacy and feedback perception and between feedback and intrinsic motivation

Psychometric Instrument Development

Examining the efficacy of the Theory of Planned Behavior (TPB) to understand pre-service teachers intention to use technology*

CHAPTER 3 RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

CHAPTER 4: FINDINGS 4.1 Introduction This chapter includes five major sections. The first section reports descriptive statistics and discusses the

One-Way Independent ANOVA

PSY 216: Elementary Statistics Exam 4

CHAPTER NINE DATA ANALYSIS / EVALUATING QUALITY (VALIDITY) OF BETWEEN GROUP EXPERIMENTS

12/31/2016. PSY 512: Advanced Statistics for Psychological and Behavioral Research 2

Summary & Conclusion. Lecture 10 Survey Research & Design in Psychology James Neill, 2016 Creative Commons Attribution 4.0

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN LIFE SATISFACTION AND ATTACHMENT STYLES WITH PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING OF CHILDREN ON COVERED BY BEHZISTEY IN TEHRAN

2013 Supervisor Survey Reliability Analysis

Replications and Refinements. Effects of Own Versus Other s Fair Treatment on Positive Emotions: A Field Study

Repeated Measures ANOVA and Mixed Model ANOVA. Comparing more than two measurements of the same or matched participants

Journal of American Science 2010;6(10) Age and gender differences and construct of the children s emotional intelligence

RESULTS. Chapter INTRODUCTION

The Structure of Environmental Concern

The Psychometric Development Process of Recovery Measures and Markers: Classical Test Theory and Item Response Theory

SUMMATED RATING SCALES AND LEVELS OF MEASUREMENT

Introduction to Multilevel Models for Longitudinal and Repeated Measures Data

Chapter Eight: Multivariate Analysis

Factor Analysis of Gulf War Illness: What Does It Add to Our Understanding of Possible Health Effects of Deployment?

HPS301 Exam Notes- Contents

Factors Influencing Undergraduate Students Motivation to Study Science

Data and Statistics 101: Key Concepts in the Collection, Analysis, and Application of Child Welfare Data

Interaction Effects: Centering, Variance Inflation Factor, and Interpretation Issues

Measurement of Constructs in Psychosocial Models of Health Behavior. March 26, 2012 Neil Steers, Ph.D.

Improving the U.S. Public s Understanding of Climate Change

This self-archived version is provided for scholarly purposes only. The correct reference for this article is as follows:

Statistics as a Tool. A set of tools for collecting, organizing, presenting and analyzing numerical facts or observations.

The Influence of Health and Beauty Perception on Medical Tourism Intentions: A Learning Lesson from Korea for Hong Kong

Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences 114 ( 2014 ) th World Conference on Psychology, Counselling and Guidance

University of Warwick institutional repository:

Survey research (Lecture 1) Summary & Conclusion. Lecture 10 Survey Research & Design in Psychology James Neill, 2015 Creative Commons Attribution 4.

Survey research (Lecture 1)

Introduction to Multilevel Models for Longitudinal and Repeated Measures Data

Advanced ANOVA Procedures

Types of Tests. Measurement Reliability. Most self-report tests used in Psychology and Education are objective tests :

11/18/2013. Correlational Research. Correlational Designs. Why Use a Correlational Design? CORRELATIONAL RESEARCH STUDIES

BRIEF REPORT. Gerald J. Haeffel. Zachary R. Voelz and Thomas E. Joiner, Jr. University of Wisconsin Madison, Madison, WI, USA

Diana Hardmeier Moritz Jaeger Rebekka Schibli Adrian Schwaninger Center for Adaptive Security Research and Applications (CASRA) Zurich Switzerland

General Antecedents of Personal Norms, Policy Acceptability, and Intentions: The Role of Values, Worldviews, and Environmental Concern.

THE STATSWHISPERER. Introduction to this Issue. Doing Your Data Analysis INSIDE THIS ISSUE

Is Happiness Different From Flourishing? Cross-Country Evidence from the ESS

Predictors of Avoidance of Help-Seeking: Social Achievement Goal Orientation, Perceived Social Competence and Autonomy

Daniel Boduszek University of Huddersfield

CHAPTER - 6 STATISTICAL ANALYSIS. This chapter discusses inferential statistics, which use sample data to

Transcription:

1. Method and measures 1.1 Sample Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed In the cross-country study, the age of the participants ranged between 20 and 79 years, with approximately 20% of the participants (n = 80) belonging to each age group among the surveyed countries. Participants educational level was generally higher than the average, with approximately 30% of respondents having received university degrees in Canada, Germany, Switzerland, and the United Kingdom. Participants from China and the United States possessing university degrees accounted for 57% and 37% of the samples, respectively (Table S1). In the additional Swiss study, the sample was similar with the cross-country study. About 20% of the participants were in each age group. 8% (n = 26) of the respondents fell into the low education category and 22% (n = 74) people had a university degree ( high education ). Table S1. Demographic characteristics of participants in the six different countries: Frequencies and percentages are shown (gender and age were quota variables). Canada (N = 426) China (N = 420) Germany (N = 421) Switzerland (N = 404) UK (N = 401) US (N = 423) n % n % n % n % n % n % Gender male 205 48% 211 50% 208 49% 194 48% 197 49% 201 48% female 221 52% 209 50% 213 51% 210 52% 204 51% 222 53% Age 20-29 68 16% 86 21% 83 20% 77 19% 69 17% 63 15% 30-39 77 18% 82 20% 77 18% 75 19% 74 19% 75 18% 40-49 95 22% 79 19% 86 20% 78 19% 79 20% 93 22% 50-59 89 21% 91 22% 85 20% 87 22% 85 21% 91 22% 60+ 97 23% 82 20% 90 21% 87 22% 94 23% 101 24% Education # low 17 4% 24 6% 134 32% 34 8% 15 4% 19 5% middle 294 69% 127 30% 153 36% 253 63% 253 63% 248 59% high 115 27% 238 57% 123 29% 106 26% 111 28% 156 37% Notes. Totals of percentages are not 100 for every demographic characteristic and country because of rounding and missing responses. # Low education indicates the highest degree participants received is below high school (including no education, primary school, secondary school, middle school, and some high school in different countries). Middle education indicates the participants have received the degree of high school or higher but not yet a university degree (including high school, vocational school, college, and some university). High education indicates the participants have received a university degree or higher (including Bachelor, Master, and PhD). NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange 1

1.2 Knowledge scale Our objective knowledge scale consisted of 12 items used in previous studies 1,2 and had three subscales representing three different forms of knowledge: knowledge about physical characteristics assessed fundamental knowledge about the physics underlying climate change, knowledge about the causes of climate change measured the reasons that climate change is happening, and knowledge about the consequences of climate change included items about the different natural hazards and environmental effects of climate change. Other authors discussed the relevance of procedural or action-related knowledge to explain ecological behaviors 3. Because procedural knowledge was not expected to explain people s perception of environmental issues 1,2, this was not investigated in the current research. Respondents answered each item with true, false or don t know. The latter response option was provided to avoid guesses from the participants. The wordings of the knowledge items were carefully selected in order to avoid indicating the correctness of the statements. Respondents answers on the knowledge scale were first reversed for knowledge items with incorrect statements. Hence, the results indicated whether the answers were correct, wrong, or whether the respondent did not know the answer. Don t know answers were recoded as wrong answers thereby allowing for an analysis of binary data (1 = correct ; 0 = wrong and don t know ). In the additional survey in Switzerland, we also included a self-assessed knowledge measure. Participants were asked to rate their level of knowledge about climate change on a 6-point scale ranging between very low knowledge and very high knowledge. 1.3 Value orientations and cultural worldviews We adopted three general value orientations in the present paper from De Groot and Steg 4 : 1) egoistic values reveal people s desire for personal interests and advancements; 2) altruistic values explore to what extent do participants care about the welfare of others; and 3) biospheric values indicate whether people concern about the environment and about supporting living organisms. The value items were assessed on a nine-point response scales, 2 NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange

ranging from -1 ( opposed to my values ), 0 ( not important ) to 7 ( extremely important ). The reliability coefficients suggested three reliable scales (Cronbach s αs 0.84, Table S2). Table S2. Value orientations: The items, their means, standard deviations, corrected itemtotal correlations, as well as internal reliabilities, means and standard deviations of the scales of the cross-country study. Items per scale M SD Corrected itemtotal correlation Egoism (Cronbach s α = 0.84, M = 2.71, SD = 1.81) Social power: control over others, dominance. 1.73 2.40 0.66 Wealth: material possessions, money. 3.06 2.07 0.63 Authority: the right to lead or command. 2.68 2.17 0.78 Influence: having an impact on people and events. 3.38 2.13 0.65 Altruism (Cronbach s α = 0.90, M = 5.63, SD = 1.41) Equality: equal opportunity for all. 5.52 1.67 0.75 A world at peace: free of war and conflict. 5.87 1.56 0.76 Social justice: correcting injustice, care for the 5.63 1.62 0.84 weak. Helpful: working for the welfare of others. 5.49 1.59 0.76 Biospherism (Cronbach s α = 0.95, M = 5.62, SD = 1.48) Preventing pollution: protecting natural resources. 5.57 1.59 0.86 Respecting the earth: harmony with other species. 5.67 1.58 0.91 Unity with nature: fitting into nature. 5.50 1.65 0.88 Protecting the environment: preserving nature. 5.75 1.53 0.89 In the additional study in Switzerland, we also measured participants cultural worldviews by adopting the 12-item scale from the study of Kahan et al. 5 (egoismcommunitarianism, hierarchy-egalitarianism) and slightly changed the items to better fit the Swiss society 2. All items were assessed on six-point Likert scales ranging from 1 ( strongly disagree ) to 6 ( strongly agree ). The internal consistency of these two subscales was fairly good, with Cronbach s α = 0.77, N = 6 for the individualism dimension and Cronbach s α = 0.68, N = 6 for the hierarchy dimension. NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange 3

1.4 Concern about climate change We studied people s concern about climate change using a scale consisting of four items taken from previous research in the cross-country study 1,2 (see Table S3). Participants rated their concern about climate change on these items on 6-point Likert scales, with higher scores indicating more concern about climate change. The internal reliability of the scale was high, Cronbach s α = 0.94. Table S3. Climate change concern: The items, their means, standard deviations, and corrected item-total correlations in the cross-country study. Items M SD Corrected item-total correlation I worry that the state of climate is changing. 4.51 1.44 0.81 Climate change has severe consequences for humans and nature. 4.94 1.28 0.85 Climate protection is important for our future. 5.07 1.28 0.88 We must protect the climate s equilibrium. 5.01 1.29 0.88 Scale s Cronbach s α = 0.94, M = 4.88, SD = 1.21 Likewise, we assessed people s concern about climate change using the same approach in the additional Swiss study (see Table S4). The internal reliability suggested a sufficiently good scalability factor with Cronbach s α = 0.90. 4 NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange

Table S4. Climate change concern: The items, their means, standard deviations, and corrected item-total correlations in the additional Swiss study. Items M SD Corrected item-total correlation I worry that the state of climate is changing. 4.42 1.31 0.75 Climate change has severe consequences for humans and nature. 4.89 1.17 0.77 Climate protection is important for our future. 5.12 1.15 0.79 We must protect the climate s equilibrium. 5.10 1.12 0.82 Scale s Cronbach s α = 0.90, M = 4.88, SD = 1.04 2. Data analysis Mokken scale analysis was conducted to test the quality of our knowledge scales. This method is subject to stricter assumptions than reliability analyses based on Cronbach s alpha 6 ; instead of assuming that all items have the same frequency distribution, a Mokken scale analysis explicitly takes into account (1) the latent knowledge of respondents about the subject of interest (i.e., climate change) and (2) key characteristics of the survey items (question difficulty on our case). Not only can a Mokken scale analysis rank participants according to their probability of positive responses (i.e., based on latent traits such as knowledge), it can also scale items based on their probability of being answered correctly. One of the important assumptions of a Mokken scale analysis is double monotonicity. Monotonicity firstly means that the expected order of respondents should be monotonically non-decreasing for each item. That is, the items that are responded to correctly by persons with little knowledge should also be answered correctly by persons with more knowledge. Secondly, the items should be monotonically ordered for each person: people who are able to correctly answer the difficult questions should also respond correctly to the less difficult and easier items. NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange 5

In a Mokken scale analysis, scalability is measured by Loevinger's coefficient H for the entire scale and H i for each item i. The Loevinger scalability coefficient H indicates the extent to which respondents can be accurately ordered by the suggested set of items, while scalability coefficient H i indicates the degree to which each item could be accurately ordered by the participants. H i and H will take values between 0 and 1, and the larger the H and H i values, the higher is the confidence in this ordering. A scale with a set of items with H values between 0.3 and 0.4 is considered to be an acceptable scale. H = 0.4 0.5 indicates medium scalability, and H = 0.5 1 suggests a strong scale. Concerning the individual items, the scalability coefficients H i for each individual item should be H i 0.3. The items kept in the knowledge scale were proved to be of good quality through the Mokken scale analysis both in the cross-country study (see Fig. 1) and the additional study. After conducting the Mokken scale analysis, we summed the number of correct responses for each respondent, for each of the three knowledge scales, and calculated the proportion of correct responses for each type of knowledge to examine what kind of knowledge was more present and which was less present in our participants. The three sum scores of the knowledge scales were included in the regression analyses described below. Further, in order to investigate the additional effects of knowledge on public concern about climate change after controlling for demographics and value orientations, we conducted hierarchical linear regression analyses with public concern as dependent variable in the crosscountry study. In Model 1, demographics, and value orientations were included as independent variables. In Model 2, the three types of knowledge were added as independent variables. The analysis was done for each country separately. In the main paper, we only reported the total effects of these predictors (Model 2 only) on concern about climate change for each different country (Table I). Here, we also present the results of the hierarchical regression analyses (Table S7). In order to examine the country differences regarding the three types of knowledge about climate change, a multivariate ANOVA was conducted 7. We included education level as covariate, country as independent variable (IV), and knowledge about physical 6 NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange

characteristics, knowledge about causes and knowledge about consequences as dependent variables (DVs). Likewise, using MANCOVA, we examined countries that differed in terms of the value orientations with egoistic value, altruistic value, and biospheric value as dependent variables and country as independent variable, and education level as covariate. In addition, a one-way ANCOVA was conducted to illustrate the country differences on the concern about climate change, with education level as covariate. In the additional study in Switzerland, we conducted hierarchical linear regression analysis to examine the impacts of cultural worldviews [Model 3(a)] and self-assessed knowledge [Model 3(b)]. With the concern about climate change as dependent variable, we included cultural worldviews in Model 3(a) and self-assessed knowledge in Model 3(b) in addition to Model 2 with demographics, value orientations, and objectively measured knowledge. 3. Additional results 3.1 Different types of knowledge According to the results in Fig. 1, our respondents seemed to be reasonably wellinformed about climate change related questions. Specifically, in the cross-country study, within the full sample of 2,495 people, average correct response constituted 49.2% for knowledge about the physical characteristics, 59.7% for knowledge about the causes and 70.4% for knowledge about the consequences of climate change. The scale with four items about the physical aspects of climate change was acceptable, H = 0.41, H i 0.27 and ρ = 0.57 (Fig. 1). Respondents appeared least knowledgeable on this dimension: more than half of the participants managed to respond correctly about how CO 2 is produced (item 1) and that CO 2 is not harmful for plants (item 2), which accounted for 80% and 56% correct responses, respectively. By contrast, few people knew about the CO 2 emissions during the operation of nuclear power plants (item 3, 31% correct) and about the comparison of environmental impact between CO 2 and methane (item 4, 29% correct). In addition, the four items that intended to assess respondents knowledge regarding NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange 7

climate change causes formed a satisfactory one-dimensional scale, H = 0.42, H i 0.38, and ρ = 0.56 (Fig. 1). Most participants knew about the CO 2 increase in the atmosphere (item 5, 83% correct responses). Also, the items about the facts that human activities are responsible for climate change and temperature increases in the past centuries were well known (item 6: 75% correct on average and item 7: 64% correct responses). However, only 18% of the respondents knew about the changes in CO 2 concentrations during the past centuries (item 8). The four items about the consequences of climate change formed a reliable onedimensional scale with a Loevinger scalability coefficient of H = 0.41, the individual scalability coefficients where Hi 0.37, and a reliability coefficient of ρ = 0.61 (Fig. 1). Respondents were best informed about the consequences of climate change. Most participants knew about the rising see level (Fig. 1, item 9) and the increase of extreme weather events (item 10), which accounted for 90% and 89% correct responses, respectively. By contrast, participants seemed to be less knowledgeable about the expected pattern in climate change (item 11) and precipitation change (item 12), which made up 52% and 51% correct responses respectively. In the additional Swiss study, the Loevinger scalability coefficients (H) and reliabilities (ρ) for the three scales of knowledge about climate change were H = 0.43, ρ = 0.57 for knowledge about physical characteristics; H = 0.48, ρ = 0.62 for knowledge about causes; and H = 0.50, ρ = 0.62 for knowledge about consequences. The individual scalability coefficients (H i ) for each item were above 0.40 in each knowledge scale. Consistent with the results of the cross-country study, the proportions of correct responses for knowledge about the physical characteristics, about the causes, and about the consequences of climate change were 50%, 59% and 75%, respectively. Participants tended to report their own knowledge level about climate change as moderate on a scale from 1 to 6 (M = 3.53, SD = 1.11). 3.2 Differences among countries regarding different types of knowledge We examined the differences between the six countries for the three types of knowledge (Table S5). The MANOVA results indicated significant differences among countries on 8 NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange

knowledge about physical characteristics, causes, and consequences of climate change (see Table S5). To be more specific, respondents from Germany and Switzerland had significantly higher scores on knowledge about physical aspects of climate change than participants from Canada and the US. Chinese respondents knew significantly more about the causes of climate change than the respondents from the other countries. German and Swiss respondents were most knowledgeable about the consequences of climate change. In contrast, participants from the US had the lowest level of knowledge about climate change among the six countries we surveyed, independent of the type of knowledge. The effect size (η 2 ) suggested that the sample size used in this study was sufficient to detect the significance (at p < 0.05) of country differences about climate change knowledge (Table S5) 8. Table S5. Knowledge about physical characteristics, causes, and consequences of climate change: Means and 95% confidence intervals per country. Physical knowledge Causes knowledge Consequences knowledge M 95%CI M 95%CI M 95%CI Canada 0.45 b [0.42, 0.48] 0.60 b [0.57, 0.62] 0.67 b [0.64, 0.69] China 0.50 a,b [0.47, 0.53] 0.68 a [0.65, 0.71] 0.69 b [0.66, 0.71] Germany 0.55 a [0.52, 0.57] 0.62 b [0.59, 0.64] 0.79 a [0.76, 0.81] Switzerland 0.53 a [0.50, 0.56] 0.61 b [0.58, 0.63] 0.77 a [0.75, 0.80] UK 0.49 a,b [0.46, 0.52] 0.58 b [0.55, 0.61] 0.71 b [0.69, 0.74] US 0.45 b [0.43, 0.48] 0.52 c [0.49, 0.54] 0.61 c [0.59, 0.64] F(df 1, df 2 ) 7.59***(5, 2413) 16.64*** (5, 2413) 26.22*** (5, 2413) η 2 0.02 0.03 0.05 Notes. Means with different superscripts within a column are significantly different from each other at p < 0.05 (after Bonferroni correction). *** p < 0.001. Education level was included as a covariate. 3.3 Differences between countries regarding value orientations In addition, differences between countries regarding the three value orientations were investigated (Table S6). Results of the MANOVA suggested significant differences among countries on egoism, altruism, and biospherism (see Table S6). More exactly, the participants from China had significantly higher scores on egoism than the ones from other countries. NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange 9

They were followed by people from Canada, the UK, and the US while German and Swiss participants appeared to have least interest in self-importance and self-benefits. Chinese and Swiss respondents were found to be most altruistic in our research. Respondents from the United States, by contrast, found it least important to consider other people when making decisions. Likewise, respondents from China and Switzerland reported significantly higher biospheric values than the respondents from other countries. Table S6. Egoism, altruism, and biospherism: Means and 95% confidence intervals per country. Egoism value Altruism value Biospherism value M 95%CI M 95%CI M 95%CI Canada 2.57 b [2.41, 2.73] 5.59 a,b [5.46, 5.72] 5.53 b [5.39, 5.67] China 4.24 a [4.07, 4.41] 5.76 a [5.62, 5.91] 6.04 a [5.89, 6.19] Germany 2.10 c [1.94, 2.26] 5.66 a,b [5.52, 5.80] 5.55 b [5.40, 5.69] Switzerland 2.23 b,c [2.07, 2.39] 5.73 a [5.59, 5.87] 5.88 a [5.73, 6.02] UK 2.66 b [2.49, 2.83] 5.66 a,b [5.52, 5.80] 5.51 b [5.37, 5.66] US 2.54 b [2.38, 2.70] 5.40 b [5.26, 5.53] 5.28 b [5.14, 5.42] F (df 1, df 2 ) 81.80*** (5, 2413) 3.59** (5, 2413) 14.17*** (5, 2413) η 2 0.15 0.01 0.03 Notes. Means with different superscripts within a column are significantly different from each other at p < 0.05 (after Bonferroni correction). *** p < 0.001. Education level was included as a covariate. 3.4 Differences between countries regarding concern about climate change A one-way ANCOVA revealed significant differences between countries about the concern about climate change. As shown in Table S7, European respondents appeared to be rather concern about climate change and Chinese participants were substantially more concerned about climate change than people from the other countries. Participants from the US seemed to be the least worried about climate change. The results seem to be in line with previous studies 9-11. 10 NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange

Table S7. Concern about climate change: Means and 95% confidence intervals per country. Concern about climate change M 95%CI Canada 4.84 b [4.72, 4.95] China 5.37 a [5.25, 5.49] Germany 4.90 b [4.78, 5.01] Switzerland 4.95 b [4.83, 5.06] UK 4.78 b [4.66, 4.90] US 4.51 c [4.39, 4.62] F (df 1, df 2 ) 22.37*** (5, 2413) η 2 0.04 Notes. Means with different superscripts within a column are significantly different from each other at p < 0.05 (after Bonferroni correction). *** p < 0.001. Education level was included as a covariate. 3.5 Hierarchical regression analyses on concern about climate change According to the results of the hierarchical regression analyses of the cross-country study, the introduction of demographics and value orientations in Model 1 was significant and explained a great part of the variance (17% 34% explained variances) in concern about climate change across the countries (Table S8). The inclusion of knowledge about physical characteristics, causes, and consequences of climate change (Model 2) further increased the explained variance of concern about climate change across the countries. That is, including the three types of knowledge about climate change significantly improved the explained variance with 2% to 18% (Table S8). In China, different types of knowledge about climate change only marginally improved the regression model with 2%, F change (3, 378) = 2.55, p = 0.056. A possible explanation for this finding may be that the Chinese participants in general had a higher level of knowledge about the causes of climate change than people from the other countries (Table S4), the inclusion of the three types of knowledge therefore did not additionally improve their concern about climate change. In all six countries, the correlations NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange 11

between the three types of knowledge were small (r < 0.4) and variance inflation factors (VIF) were slightly bigger than 1 (1.10 < VIF < 1.47), so that multicollinearity was not a concern 7,12. Table S8. Hierarchical regression analyses predicting public concern about climate change in the cross-country survey. Models and predictors per country R 2 ΔR 2 F (df 1, df 2 ) ΔF (df 1, df 2 ) Canada Model 1(without knowledge as 0.25 20.35*** (7, 418) predictor) Model 2 (with knowledge as 0.35 0.10 22.55*** (10, 415) 20.91*** (3, 415) predictor) China Model 1 0.20 13.54*** (7, 381) Model 2 0.22 0.02 10.35*** (10, 378) 2.55 (3, 378) Germany Model 1 0.21 15.06*** (7, 402) Model 2 0.30 0.09 17.07*** (10, 399) 17. 45*** (3, 399) Switzerland Model 1 0.17 11.22*** (7, 385) Model 2 0.35 0.18 20.42*** (10, 382) 34.94*** (3, 382) UK US Model 1 0.34 27.72*** (7, 371) Model 2 0.46 0.11 30.75*** (10, 368) 25.18*** (3, 368) Model 1 0.29 24.11*** (7, 415) Model 2 0.43 0.14 31.05*** (10, 412) 33.88*** (3, 412) Notes. Model 1 included gender, age, dummy variables for low education vs. high education and for middle education vs. high education, egoistic values, altruistic values, and biospheric values; Model 2 additionally included knowledge about physical characteristics, causes, and consequences of climate change; *** p < 0.001. In the additional Swiss study, the regression analysis with concern about climate change as dependent variable, the introduction of cultural worldviews, in addition to demographics, value orientations and three types of knowledge, slightly but significantly increased the 12 NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange

explained variance of the dependent variable with 6.9%. Individualism was negatively related to concern about climate change while hierarchy had no significant relation (see Model 3(a) in Table S9). Similarly, we included self-assessed knowledge after the demographics, value orientations, and three types of knowledge. No significant impact of subjective measured knowledge was found on concern about climate change (see Model 3(b) in Table S9). Additionally, the main results remained the same after including cultural worldviews and subjective measured knowledge at the same time (see Model 4 in Table S9). In short, including cultural worldviews somewhat improved the model fit and therefore seem to have a unique relation with concern about climate change, which is independent of the relation between value orientations and concern about climate change. The additional explained variance of cultural worldviews to the model is however not as large as that of the value orientations and knowledge together (R 2 = 0.35). It is thus likely that there is some overlap between cultural worldviews and value orientations. Nevertheless, an interesting question to address in a future study would be whether cultural worldviews are more important in explaining public concern about climate change in some countries than in other countries. NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange 13

Table S9. Hierarchical regression analysis predicting public concern about climate change in the additional Swiss survey. Model 2 Model 3(a) Model 3(b) Model 4 B 95%CI B 95%CI B 95%CI B 95%CI Gender # -0.26* [-0.46, -0.06] -0.22* [-0.42, -0.03] -0.26* [-0.46, -0.05] -0.22* [-0.42, -0.03] Age 0.00 [-0.01, 0.01] 0.00 [0.00, 0.01] 0.00 [-0.01, 0.01] 0.00 [-0.01, 0.01] Low education Ϯ Middle education Ϯ Egoistic values 0.06 [-0.33, 0.46] 0.26 [-0.12, 0.64] 0.06 [-0.34, 0.46] 0.26 [-0.12, 0.64] 0.08 [-0.15, 0.31] 0.17 [-0.05, 0.39] 0.08 [-0.15, 0.31] 0.17 [-0.05, 0.39] 0.02 [-0.04, 0.09] 0.00 [-0.06, 0.07] 0.02 [-0.04, 0.09] 0.01 [-0.06, 0.07] Altruistic values -0.01 [-0.11, 0.09] -0.02 [-0.12, 0.09] -0.01 [-0.11, 0.09] -0.02 [-0.12, 0.09] Biospheric values 0.30*** [0.20, 0.40] 0.27*** [0.17, 0.36] 0.30*** [0.20, 0.40] 0.27*** [0.17, 0.36] Physical knowledge -0.08 [-0.44, 0.29] -0.05 [-0.40, 0.30] -0.08 [-0.45, 0.30] -0.05 [-0.41, 0.30] Causes knowledge 1.00*** [0.64, 1.37] 0.84*** [0.49, 1.19] 1.00*** [0.64, 1.37] 0.84*** [0.49, 1.19] Result knowledge 0.35 [-0.06, 0.75] 0.36 [-0.03, 0.75] 0.35 [-0.07, 0.76] 0.36 [-0.03, 0.75] Individualism -0.30*** [-0.40, -0.20] -0.30*** [-0.40, -0.20] Hierarchy -0.03 [-0.15, 0.10] -0.03 [-0.16, 0.10] Self-assessed knowledge 0.00 [-0.10, 0.10] 0.00 [-0.09, 0.09] R 2 0.35 0.42 0.35 0.42 F(df 1, df 2 ) 17.40*** (10, 324) 19.28*** (12, 322) 15.77*** (11, 323) 17.74*** (13, 321) F(df 1, df 2 ) N.A. 19.02*** (2, 322) 0.00 (1, 323) 12.64*** (3, 321) Notes. # Gender: 0 = female, 1 = male. Ϯ Dummy variable with high education as reference group. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001. 14 NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange

References 1 Tobler, C., Visschers, V. H. & Siegrist, M. Consumers knowledge about climate change. Clim. Change. 114, 189-209 (2012). 2 Shi, J., Visschers, V. H. & Siegrist, M. Public perception of climate change: The importance of knowledge and cultural worldviews. Risk. Anal. 35, 2183-2201 (2015). 3 Kaiser, F. G. & Fuhrer, U. Ecological behavior's dependency on different forms of knowledge. Appl. Psychol. 52, 598-613 (2003). 4 De Groot, J. & Steg, L. General beliefs and the theory of planned behavior: The role of environmental concerns in the TPB. J. Appl. Soc. Psychol. 37, 1817-1836 (2007). 5 Kahan, D. M. et al. The polarizing impact of science literacy and numeracy on perceived climate change risks. Nat. Clim. Change. 2, 732-735 (2012). 6 Van Schuur, W. H. Mokken scale analysis: Between the Guttman scale and parametric item response theory. Pol. Anal. 11, 139-163 (2003). 7 Field, A. in Discovering statistics using IBM SPSS statistics. (Sage Publications, London, 2013). 8 Cohen, J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. (Academic press, New York, 2013). 9 Leiserowitz, A. Climate change risk perception and policy preferences: The role of affect, imagery, and values. Clim. Change. 77, 45-72 (2006). 10 Lorenzoni, I. & Pidgeon, N. F. Public views on climate change: European and USA perspectives. Clim. Change. 77, 73-95 (2006). 11 Bedford, D. Does Climate Literacy Matter? A Case Study of US Students Level of Concern about Anthropogenic Global Warming. Journal of Geography, 1-11 (2015). 12 Cohen, J., Cohen, P., West, S. G. & Aiken, L. S. Applied Multiple Regression/Correlation Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. (Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, New Jersey, 2003). NATURE CLIMATE CHANGE www.nature.com/natureclimatechange 15