Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample. A Thesis Presented

Similar documents
Comparing Two Procedures to Teach Conditional Discriminations: Simple Discriminations With and Without S- Stimuli Present. A Thesis Presented

Emergence of Equivalence Relations: Comparing Sorting and Match-to-Sample Procedures. Lindsay J. Grimm. The New England Center for Children

Comparison of Direct and Indirect Reinforcement Contingencies on Task Acquisition. A Thesis Presented. Robert Mark Grant

Effects of Increased Exposure to Training Trials with Children with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Melissa A. Ezold

The Effects of Video Modeling on New Staff Training. of Discrete Trial Instruction. A thesis presented. Tamarra Forbes

Determining the Reinforcing Value of Social Consequences and Establishing. Social Consequences as Reinforcers. A Thesis Presented. Hilary A.

The Effects of Varying Levels of Procedural Integrity during Prompting. on Conditional Discrimination Performance. A Thesis Presented.

Assessment of Delayed Matching in Preschoolers with Autism. A Thesis Presented. Hannorah O. Thurman

DOES STIMULUS COMPLEXITY AFFECT ACQUISITION OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS AND THE EMERGENCE OF DERIVED RELATIONS? Tiffani L. Martin, B.A., B.S.

A Comparison of Edible, Social, and No Contrived Reinforcement on the. Acquisition of a Behavior Chain. A Thesis Presented.

Running Head: Improving Parent Implementation of Discrete Trial Teaching

The Impact of Formatting Skill Acquisition Procedures and Treatment Integrity. A Thesis Presented. Kara E. Lamb

Differential Reinforcement of Prompted and Independent Responses: An Alternative. Procedure to Decrease Prompt Dependency. A Thesis Presented

A Comparison of Methods for Marking Correct Responses. in Matching to Sample. A Thesis Presented. Kara Schmidt

TRAINING STRUCTURE, NAMING AND TYPICALITY EFFECTS IN EQUIVALENCE CLASS FORMATION. Jeanette E. Wilson. Department of Psychology

Title: Stimulus Pairing Training in. Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Kosuke Takahashi a. Jun ichi Yamamoto b

COMPARING THE EFFECTS OF ECHOIC PROMPTS AND ECHOIC PROMPTS PLUS MODELED PROMPTS ON INTRAVERBAL BEHAVIOR AMBER L. VALENTINO

Determining Preference for Social Interaction. A Thesis Presented. Hillary S. Balog. In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy ISSN: Universidad de Almería España

TEACHING SIMPLE AUDITORY DISCRIMINATIONS TO STUDENTS WITH AUTISM. Kristine L. Marino, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE

Teaching a Scanning Response to a Child with Autism

AN EVALUATION OF CHOICE ON INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICACY AND INDIVIDUAL PREFERENCES AMONG CHILDREN WITH AUTISM KAREN A. TOUSSAINT TIFFANY KODAK

AN EVALUATION OF PREFERENCE FOR VIDEO AND IN VIVO MODELING KANEEN B. GEIGER AND LINDA A. LEBLANC

The Effects of Question-Present Versus Item Present Conditions on Acquisition of Mands

Running Head: BEHAVIORAL ECONOMIC ANALYSIS OF CHOICE. A Behavioral Economic Analysis of Choice. A Thesis Presented. Jamie Leigh Lebowitz

Derived Textual Control in Recreational Activity Schedules with Children with Autism

A Comparison of Methods for Teaching Object Imitation: In-Vivo versus Video Modeling. A Thesis Presented. Amy N. Wick

Title: Experimental evaluation of a parent-implemented AAC intervention protocol for children with severe autism

SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL CONTROL OF MEMBERS OF AN EQUIVALENCE CLASS THESIS. Presented to the Graduate Council of the

Assessing the Effects of Matched and Unmatched Stimuli on the Persistence of Stereotypy. A Thesis Presented. Sarah Scamihorn

UNIVERSITY OF AKUREYRI

Running head: UTILITY OF TWO DEMAND ASSESSMENTS 1. An Evaluation of the Relative Utility of Two Demand Assessments

Using Pivotal Response Training with Peers in Special Education to Facilitate Play in Two Children with Autism

Abstract. Authors. Shauna Whiteford, Ksusha Blacklock, Adrienne Perry. Correspondence. Keywords

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Running head: EVALUATING INHIBITORY STIMULUS CONTROL. Increasing the Reinforcing Efficacy of Low-Preference Teachers and Caretakers

THE EFFECTS OF THERAPIST-MEDIATED SIGNALS AND PARTICIPANT-MEDIATED RESPONSES ON THE DELAY TOLERANCE OF TYPICALLY DEVELOPING PRESCHOOL CHILDREN

Masaki Ishizuka, Tetsumi Moriyama. Tokiwa University, Mito, Japan

THE POWER OF ONE REINFORCER. Mary E. Hunter, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of MASTER OF SCIENCE UNIVERSITY OF NORTH TEXAS. August 2013 APPROVED:

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves

Teaching Staff Who Work With Children With Autism Spectrum Disorders to Evaluate the Treatment Integrity of Discrete-Trials Teaching Sessions

CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION AND STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE: EFFECTS OF SUPPRESSING DERIVED SYMMETRICAL RESPONSES ON THE EMERGENCE OF TRANSITIVITY

Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention: Strategies to Enhance Effectiveness of Home Teams

INCREASING THE PROBABILITY OF STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE WITH ADULTS WITH MILD MENTAL RETARDATION

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

A Search for Derived Stimulus Relations in Rats

Stimulus Control & Generalization

How Does the ProxTalker Speech- Generating Device Compare to PECS?

J-01 State intervention goals in observable and measureable terms.

EMERGENCE OF COMPLEX CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS BY JOINT CONTROL OF COMPOUND SAMPLES

CARBONE CLINIC VIVIAN ATTANASIO VERBAL BEHAVIOR INSTITUTE AND TAMARA KASPER CENTER FOR AUTISM TREATMENT

I give special thanks to Ed Sbardellati, who has been a pivotal support, during this study, and throughout my career as a behavior analyst.

DIFFERENTIAL REINFORCEMENT WITH AND WITHOUT BLOCKING AS TREATMENT FOR ELOPEMENT NATHAN A. CALL

DERIVED CONDITIONAL POSITION DISCRIMINATION IN INDIVIDUALS WITHOUT NAMI NG SKILLS

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

METHOD Participants and Setting The participants in this study were 11 children (10 boys, 1 girl) with an independent

Evaluation of Salient Stimulus Cues During. Brief Functional Analyses in a Classroom Setting. Kelly L. McConnell. B.S., University of Florida

A PRACTICAL VARIATION OF A MULTIPLE-SCHEDULE PROCEDURE: BRIEF SCHEDULE-CORRELATED STIMULI JEFFREY H. TIGER GREGORY P. HANLEY KYLIE M.

Assessing Relational Networks: An Evaluation of Derived Relational Responding With Children With ASD and Typically Developing Children

The Effect of Choice-Making Opportunities during Activity Schedules on Task Engagement of Adults with Autism

A SORTING-TO-MATCHING METHOD TO TEACH COMPOUND MATCHING TO SAMPLE RACHEL S. FARBER

Program. PECS (cont.) Speech-Generating Devices (SGDs) SGDs (Cont.) 11/19/2010. Wendt, Boesch, & Subramanian ASHA Convention 2010

Evaluation of a Self-Instructional Package for Teaching Tutors to Conduct Discrete Trials Teaching with Children with Autism.

REVERSAL OF EQUIVALENCE RELATIONS

THE EFFECTS OF OUTCOME REVERSALS ON CHILDREN S CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATION, EQUIVALENCE, AND REINFORCER-PROBE PERFORMANCES. Natalie B.

A Comparison of Functional Analysis Results When Conducted in Contrived and. Home Settings. A Thesis Presented. Dana DeIngenis

Story Time Language Program Summary Child Language Intervention Program Vanderbilt University

THE EFFECTS OF RESPONSE TOPOGRAPHY ON FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE CLASS FORMATION

Implementing Discrete Trial Teaching

TRANSFER OF GOOD AND BAD FUNCTIONS WITHIN STIMULUS EQUIVALENCE CLASSES. Jessica Madrigal-Bauguss, B.A. Thesis Prepared for the Degree of

Evaluating & Teaching Yes/No Responses Based on an Analysis of Functions. Jennifer Albis, M.S., CCC-SLP

THE EFFECTS OF INTERSPERESED TRIALS AND DENSITY OF REINFORCEMENT ON ACCURACY, LOOKING AWAY, AND SELF- INJURIOUS BEHAVIOR OF A CHILD WITH AUTISM

Concurrent Identity Training is not Necessary for Associative Symmetry in Successive Matching

INCREASING INSTRUCTIONAL EFFICIENCY BY PRESENTING ADDITIONAL STIMULI IN LEARNING TRIALS FOR CHILDREN WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS JASON C.

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli.

JOURNAL OF APPLIED BEHAVIOR ANALYSIS 2012, 45,

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

Reinforcing Effects of Social Stimuli on Responding After. Implementation of A Pairing Procedure. A Thesis Presented. Amanda J.

Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Do not make copies of this test Please contact J&K Seminars for additional tests

The Role of Modeling and Feedback in. Task Performance and the Development of Self-Efficacy. Skidmore College

The Role of Joint Control in Teaching Complex Behavior to Children with Autism

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Running Head: VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR STUDENTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

ESTABLISHING FRACTION-DECIMAL EQUIVALENCE USING A RESPONDENT-TYPE TRAINING PROCEDURE.

Conditioned Reinforcement and the Value of Praise in Children with Autism

What is Autism? Katherine Lamb, Ph.D., CCC/SLP GSHA2018 1

Value transfer in a simultaneous discrimination by pigeons: The value of the S + is not specific to the simultaneous discrimination context

EFFECTS OF PRESENTATION METHOD ON ACQUISITION SPEED OF AN IDENTITY MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE TASK

Erik Arntzen, Terje Grondahl, and Christoffer Eilifsen. Akershus University College

Parent initiated Why evaluate (3) Questions about program appropriateness School initiated Conflict resolution Component of a regular peer review proc

Olfactory Matching-To-Sample In Rats Using a Novel Apparatus. Rhiannon D. Thomas

ASSESSMENT OF THE FUNCTIONS OF VOCAL BEHAVIOR IN CHILDREN WITH DEVELOPMENTAL DISABILITIES: A REPLICATION MICHAEL E. KELLEY

Bryan Roche. Simon Dymond

Fact and Fiction: Sorting through the

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA

A Comparison of Two Interventions to Treat Food Selectivity. A Thesis Presented. Shannon C. Garvey

USING VIDEO MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT TO TEACH PERSPECTIVE-TAKING SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM LINDA A. LEBLANC AND ANDREA M.

MAKING IT WORK AS AN ABA THERAPIST

Transcription:

1 Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample A Thesis Presented by Kimberly M. Walter The Department of Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science In the field of Applied Behavior Analysis Northeastern University Boston, MA June, 2010

2 NORTHEASTERN UNIVERSITY Bouvé College of Health Sciences Graduate School Thesis Title: Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample Author: Kimberly M. Walter Department: Counseling and Applied Educational Psychology Approved for Thesis Requirements of Master of Science Degree (Paula Braga-Kenyon M.S., BCBA) (Chata Dickson Ph.D., BCBA) (Meca Andrade M.S., BCBA)

3 Running head: VARIATIONS OF GO/NO-GO AND MATCH-TO-SAMPLE Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/no-go and Match-to-Sample by Kimberly M. Walter B.A., Michigan State University Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Applied Behavior Analysis in the Bouvé College of Health Sciences Graduate School of Northeastern University, June 2010

4 Acknowledgements I would like to thank Paula Braga-Kenyon for her guidance, knowledge, and assistance throughout the process of developing, conducting, and writing my thesis. This manuscript would not have been feasible without her endless support. I would also like to thank Chata Dickson and Meca Andrade for assisting in manuscript revisions and providing helpful guidance and feedback.

5 Abstract Match-to-sample procedures (MTS) are often implemented to train conditional relations and to test for emergent relations among arbitrary stimuli. In 2007, Debert, Matos, and McIlvane evaluated if a single-key procedure (go/no-go) could be an alternative to the traditional MTS procedures to teach conditional discriminations and to test for emergent relations in humans. The current study replicated and extended Debert et al. The acquisition of conditional relations and the emergence of derived relations with arbitrary stimuli were compared using three different procedures: go/no-go, go/no-go with printed words yes and no as a component, and MTS. Three typically developing females, age 23-25, participated in visualvisual tasks during a series of table-top presentations. AB and BC relations were directly trained, and BA, CB, AC, and CA relations were tested. Depending on the procedure, training and testing occurred with stimuli sets 1-3, 4-6, or 7-9. The three participants demonstrated the emergency of new, untrained relations with all three procedures. The current results replicated the findings of Debert et al. and extended previous findings by evaluating a go/no-go with a yes/no component which may serve as an alternative method to the traditional MTS. Additionally, the current study provided a within participant comparison across all three procedures.

6 Table of Contents A. Abstract..5 B. Introduction..7 C. Method 10 D. Results...18 E. Discussion.22 F. References.25 G. Tables.27 H. Figure Captions.34 I. Figures 35

7 Conditional Relations among Abstract Stimuli: Outcomes from Three Procedures- Variations of Go/No-go and Match-to-Sample Many individuals diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and other developmental disabilities have difficulty demonstrating equivalence relations among stimuli. From a practical standpoint, stimulus equivalence is a way that individuals learn new concepts such as reading (Sidman, 1971). Equivalence among stimuli can be verified through positive demonstrations of the properties of reflexivity, symmetry, and transitivity. In reflexivity, a learner, without prior training or programmed consequences for doing so, when presented with a sample stimulus, selects a comparison stimulus that is the same as the sample stimulus (A=A). This type of relation has also been called identity matching (Sidman & Tailby, 1982). Symmetry is an untrained, derived relation; it requires that the relation holds bi-directionally between each pair of related items, and the sample/comparison role to be interchangeable (A=B, then B=A) (Sidman & Tailby, 1982). Transitivity, the third property required for equivalence, is also an untrained, derived relation (if A=B, and B=C, then C=A) that emerges as a product of training two other relations (A=B, B=C). As with reflexivity and symmetry, this relation must emerge without additional instruction or reinforcement. Taken together, positive demonstrations of reflexivity, symmetry, and transitivity, indicates that the stimuli are members of an equivalence class (Sidman & Tailby, 1982). When training conditional relations and testing for emergent relations, researchers often employ MTS procedures (Sidman, 1994). Typically, MTS

8 procedures involve the presentation of a sample stimulus (conditional stimulus) either before the presentation of comparison stimuli, or at the same time as the presentation of comparison stimuli. The comparison stimuli (discriminative stimuli) can be presented in several different locations and there are typically at least three comparison stimuli in order to avoid selection based on exclusion (Green, 2001). A response is scored as the participant selecting one of the comparison stimuli. These procedures often use arbitrary stimuli that the participant does not have prior experience or history with (Cumming & Berryman, 1961). One alternative procedure for training conditional relations and testing for the emergence of relations is the go/no-go procedure. In this procedure, two stimuli are presented on the same key, as a compound stimulus. The two stimuli that are presented together are either members of the same stimulus class (related stimuli) or not members of the same stimulus class (non-related stimuli). In 1971, Mallot, Mallot, Svinicki, Kladder, and Ponicki conducted a study where pigeons performances indicated responding based on matching. In their procedure, stimuli (colors) were presented on two halves of the same response key. If the halves were the same color and the pigeon pecked the key, reinforcers were delivered. Reinforcers were not delivered if the pigeons selected the response key when the two halves were different colors. The pigeons were exposed to discrimination training using two colors. During testing, two new colors were presented on the response key under extinction. The results indicated that the pigeons who were trained to respond to matching halves responded more when keys with new colors had matching halves then when they had non-matching halves with the new colors during testing.

9 However, these results could have been due to generalization along the color continuum (Mallot et al., 1971). In an effort to replicate and extend the results of Mallot et al. (1971), Zentall and Hogan (1975) used a similar procedure, where in one experiment the pigeons were trained to respond to matching stimuli (response keys with red/red and green/green) and not peck if the response key displayed different colored halves (red/green and green/red). Following training, the birds were exposed to new matching and non-matching stimuli using response key combinations of yellow and blue. In the second experiment, the response keys were divided into either bright or dim key illumination to test if responding was controlled by generalization along the color continuum. In both experiments, transfer to new colors or brightness was better for birds who completed the same task as in training than for birds who were exposed to the other task. Taken together, the results of these two studies indicated that pigeons are capable of demonstrating identity relations using a go/no-go procedure. Recently, Debert, Matos, and McIlvane (2007) used a go/no-go procedure to train conditional relations and test for emergent relations with adult humans. In their study, twp arbitrary stimuli were displayed on a response key. Responding to related compounds, or members of the experimenter identified stimulus class resulted in points earned which were later exchanged for back-up reinforcers. Responding to non-related compounds, or members of different stimulus classes, did not produce programmed consequences. Following training of A1-3B1-3 and B1-3C1-3 relations using differential programmed consequences, Debert et al. tested for symmetry, transitivity, and equivalence relations under extinction. Notably, 6/6 of their

10 participants demonstrated symmetry and 4/6 participants demonstrated transitivity and equivalence relations. The purpose of the current study was to replicate and extend the findings of Debert et al. (2007) by comparing participant performances with a go/no-go procedure, (as presented in Debert et al.) a go/no-go procedure where the participant selects printed words yes or no instead of touching the related compound stimuli and not touching the non-related compound stimuli, and MTS. The addition of the yes/no component was examined during the go/no-go procedure as it was anticipated that it may be difficult for some individuals to refrain from responding (not touch the stimuli) and just wait for the presentation of the next trial. Previous literature has also not compared these three procedures within and across participants. Method Participants The participants were three females between 23 and 25 years of age (NF, LEC, and LC) employed by a school for children diagnosed with ASD. All of the participants had experience running MTS procedures with their students; but had never completed a MTS procedure as a participant, nor a go/no- go procedure. The participants also had no prior familiarity with stimulus control literature or research. Participants were told they would receive a $30 gift certificate of their choice, regardless of their performance, following the completion of their participation in the study.

11 Apparatus and stimuli Stimuli were presented on a table in front of the participant (table-top presentation) and data collection was conducted manually. Sessions took place in a room with a table and chairs. During sessions, participants sat across the table from the experimenter and the stimuli were presented to the participant according to the prescribed phase of the study. During MTS sessions, all four stimuli (one sample and three comparison stimuli) were presented simultaneously on a sheet of paper that was presented in front of the participant for each trial. During go/no go trials, the compound stimuli were presented on a flashcard that was also presented in front of the participant on the table. Pre-printed data sheets were used to collect and score data. Stimuli used during the go/no-go procedures and MTS were three sets of nine arbitrary forms designated as A1-9, B1-9, and C1-9 for purposes of identification only, and comprised the three-member stimulus classes; the designations were not displayed to the participants (Table 1). The experimental stimuli were judged to be physically dissimilar with respect to form; stimulus-stimulus relations were arbitrarily assigned by the contingencies programmed by the experimenter. One set of stimuli (class 1-3) was the same stimuli used in both Markham and Dougher, (1993) and Debert et al., (2007). Prior to the beginning of training, a short questionnaire was sent to participants requesting them to indicate their preferences for edible items. Indicated edible items were delivered following the completion of sessions for that day.

12 Following the completion of the entire study, the participant also received a $30 gift certificate of their choice. In the go/no-go with yes/no component, selection of yes/no could be considered the same topographical response. Therefore, at the end of the experiment, before debriefing occurred, each compound stimuli used during the go/no go with yes/no component were presented and the participant was asked to say yes or no when the experimenter held up each compound stimulus that was presented during the procedure. Debriefing with each participant was conducted at the end of the last session. Sessions were run once per week and lasted approximately three hours. During the three hours, one of the procedures (go/no-go, go/no-go with yes/no, MTS) was run in its entirety. Procedure During a pre-experimental phase, participants were exposed to all of the relations under extinction to demonstrate a lack of prior history with the relations among the stimuli. This pre-experimental phase was conducted prior to the introductions of each procedure (go/no-go, go/no-go with yes/no, MTS). Following the pre-experimental phase, Phase I, baseline training, was introduced. Phase II introduced the first test, the test for symmetry, and Phase III tested for transitivity and equivalence. Participants were exposed to the procedures in the following order: go/no-go, go/no-go with yes/no, and MTS. All of the

13 participants completed the procedures in the same order, but the stimuli sets (1-3, 4-6, 7-9) were rotated across procedures. In each phase, all trials began with a 4s presentation of stimuli. This was introduced in order to control for the duration of exposure to each stimulus across procedures. Interobserver agreement and procedural integrity Sessions were either video-taped, or a second observer was present during sessions and recorded interobserver and procedural integrity data independently from the experimenter. Interobserver agreement (IOA) and procedural integrity (PI) data were recorded in 33% of sessions for each participant. IOA was calculated by subtracting the number of disagreements from the number of agreements, dividing that number by the total number of trials, and multiplying that number by 100. IOA agreement for all participants during go/no-go, go/no-go with yes/no, and MTS procedures was (,, and respectively). Data was also recorded for PI during 33% of sessions for each participant across procedures. Data collected on experimenter behavior included: were the correct stimuli presented, was the location of the stimuli correct, was the correct feedback given contingent upon participant responding, and whether the stimuli were presented for approximately 4s. In 33% of sessions, the experimenter presented the correct stimuli in of opportunities, presented the stimuli in the correct location in of opportunities, provided the correct feedback contingent upon participant responding in 99% of opportunities, and presented experimental stimuli for approximately 4s in 98% of opportunities.

14 Phase I In Phase I, AB and BC relations were trained in inter-mixed trials. Go/no-go procedures: The order and presentation of the compounds with related and non-related components varied randomly across trials. Eighteen different compound stimuli were developed from the combination of nine abstract stimuli (Table 2). There were 12 blocks of 24 trials, equaling a total of 288 trials in one training session. Each block was composed of two presentations of each related stimulus (go) and one presentation of each non-related stimulus (no-go). Related and non-related compounds appeared equally often in a block, and the same type of relation (related or non-related) could not occur more than three times successively. If the participant selected a related compound stimulus, they were told Correct, the compound stimulus was removed and the next prescribed compound stimulus was presented. If the participant refrained from touching a non-related compound stimulus, they were also told Correct, the compound stimulus was removed and the next prescribed compound stimulus was presented. If the participant did not respond to a related compound stimulus, or if they selected a non-related compound stimulus, the experimenter remained silent, removed the compound stimulus, and presented the next prescribed compound stimulus. At the beginning of the phase, the following instructions were read to the participants: This study is not about intelligence testing and will not evaluate your intellectual abilities. At the end of the study you will be provided with

15 a full explanation. I will be in the room during the entire session. Following the completion of today s sessions, you will receive an edible item that you have previously selected. I will present a card to you with two symbols on it; you will have approximately 4-s to respond. Your task is to touch the card when you think the correct two symbols are shown, and to refrain from touching the card when you think incorrect symbols are shown. If you touch the card when the correct symbols are shown, or refrain from selecting incorrect symbols, I will tell you correct. If you touch the card when incorrect symbols are shown, or do not respond when correct symbols are shown, I will not say anything. Please pay attention even when you think the task seems very simple. Do you have any questions? When you are ready to begin, please let me know. Thank you for your participation. In the go/no-go with yes/no component, the procedure was the same with the exception that the printed words yes and no were placed on the table in front of the participant. The printed word yes was placed on the table in the participant s left-side and the printed word no was placed on the table in front of the participant s left-side, and was not rotated throughout sessions. The following instructions were read to the participants for this procedure: This study is not about intelligence testing and will not evaluate your intellectual abilities. At the end of the study you will be provided with a full explanation. I will be in the room during the entire session. Following the completion of today s sessions, you will receive an edible item that you have previously selected. I will present a card to you with two symbols on it; you will have approximately 4-s to respond. Your task is to touch the yes card when you think the correct two symbols are shown, and to touch the no card when you think incorrect symbols are shown. If you touch the yes card when the correct symbols are shown, I will tell you correct and if you touch the no card when incorrect symbols are shown I will tell you correct. If you select the yes card when incorrect symbols are shown, or select the no card when correct symbols are shown I will not say anything. Please pay attention even when you think the task seems very simple. Do you have any questions? When you are ready to begin, please let me know. Thank you for your participation.

16 MTS procedure: The order of the stimuli presentation was semi-random and counterbalanced across trials. There were eight blocks, each composed of 18 trials, equaling 144 trials in one training session. Each relation was presented three times within a block, with the location of the comparison stimuli rotated across the left, center, and right positions. Sample and comparison stimuli were presented simultaneously on a printed note-card. Given a particular sample, if the participant selected the correct, experimenter identified comparison stimulus, the experimenter stated, Correct, the stimuli were removed and the next set of sample and comparison stimuli were presented as prescribed on the data sheet. If the participant did not select the correct comparison stimulus, the experimenter did not provide verbal feedback, removed the stimuli and the next prescribed set of sample and comparison stimuli were presented. For this procedure, the following instructions were given to the participant: This study is not about intelligence testing and will not evaluate your intellectual abilities. At the end of this study you will be provided with a full explanation. I will be in the room during the entire session. Following the completion of today s sessions, you will receive an edible item that you have previously selected. I will present a card to you with four symbols on it; you will have approximately 4-s to respond. Your task is to select one of the three symbols displayed on the bottom of the card that you think belongs with the symbol that is on the top of the card. If you select the correct symbol along the bottom, I will say correct, and if you select an incorrect symbol I will not say anything. Please pay attention even when you think the task seems very simple. Do you have any questions? When you are ready to begin, please let me know. Thank you for your participation. It should be noted that the difference in the number of training trials between the go/no-go procedures and MTS was due to the non-related compounds that were presented in the go/no-go procedure. Overall, related stimulus-stimulus relations

17 were presented an equal number of times for both the go/no-go procedures and MTS procedure. Mastery criteria Training continued until the participant made less than 24 errors throughout the entire 288 or 144 trials in training (go/no-go procedures or MTS respectively), and scored correct responding during the last six blocks. Phase II Phase II introduced the test for symmetry. The BA and CB relations were presented in inter-mixed trials. Again, stimuli were presented for 4s, regardless of participant responding. During this phase, the test was carried out under extinction or, there were no programmed, differential consequences following responses. This phase was conducted for one session only, regardless of performance. Go/no-go procedures: Sessions consisted of six blocks, with 24 compound stimulus presentations (two presentations of each related compound, and one presentation of each non-related compound), equaling 144 total trials in one session. MTS procedure: Sessions consisted of four blocks, with 18 stimulus presentations (each sample and comparison relation presented once, across the left, center, and right comparison stimulus positions), equaling 72 total trials. For all procedures, the instructions read to participants were:

18 This is a new phase of the study and your task has been modified. Respond based on what you think is correct. I will not say correct at any point during this phase, but you should know that at least some of the cards presented will be correct, so you should still respond based on what you think is correct. Again, you will have approximately 4s to respond. Do you have any questions? When you are ready to begin, please let me know. Phase III The third phase consisted of a test for transitivity and equivalence (relations AC and CA), carried out under extinction and presented in inter-mixed trials. This phase was conducted for one session, regardless of participant performance. Go/no-go procedures: Sessions consisted of six blocks, with 24 compound stimulus presentations (two presentations of each related compound, and one presentation of each non-related compound), equaling 144 total trials in one session. MTS procedure: Sessions consisted of four blocks, with 18 stimulus presentations (each sample and comparison relation presented once, across the left, center, and right comparison stimulus positions), equaling 72 total trials. For all procedures, the instructions read to participants were the same as those provided for phase II. Results Table 3 displays results for participant NF across the three procedures. This table breaks the performances into session summaries for training and test for symmetry and then by blocks within session for tests of transitivity and equivalence, followed by a session summary. Participant NF met mastery criteria for each of the

19 procedures within two sessions. Tests for symmetry across each of the procedures demonstrated positive results (responding correctly to stimuli on 90% or more of trials). During go/no-go, participant NF exhibited scores that ranged between 54.2% and 83.3% consistent with transitivity and equivalence during individual blocks within session. Averaged across blocks, responding during tests for transitivity and equivalence for go/no-go was 72.2%. While this percentage of correct responding is higher than chance level (50%), it did not meet the 90% criterion set by the experimenter as demonstration of emergent relations. Further, during the test for transitivity and equivalence correct responding never reached. Participant NF did demonstrate positive results for transitivity and equivalence in the go/no-go with yes/no procedure (average of 95.8% and correct responding during the last three test blocks). Finally, during the MTS procedure for tests for transitivity and equivalence, the participant demonstrated positive results. Data averaged across blocks (83.3%) appears to indicate negative results in comparison to the 90% correct responding criterion, however data from the last three test blocks show correct responding. Table 4 displays results for participant LEC across the three procedures. Participant LEC met mastery criteria for each of the procedures within the first session of training. Tests for symmetry across each of the procedures demonstrated positive results ( correct responding for each procedure). Participant LEC also demonstrated the emergence of transitivity and equivalence with scores of 91%, 99.3%, and, respectively.

20 Table 5 depicts the results for participant LC across procedures. The mastery criterion for training was reached within two sessions for go/no-go and one session for go/no-go with yes/no and MTS. Tests for symmetry were positive ( correct responding across all three procedures). During the go/no-go procedure, participant LC demonstrated emergent relations for transitivity and equivalence. Again, while data averaged across blocks (83.3%) at first appears to indicate negative results in comparison to the 90% correct responding criterion, correct responding during the last four test blocks was maintained above 90% (91.7%, 95.8%, 95.8%, 95.8%). Participant LC also demonstrated transitivity and equivalence relations during go/no-go with yes/no ( correct responding) and MTS ( correct responding). Figure 1 displays the data from Tables 3-5. Again, it is notable that 3/3 participants demonstrated symmetry across all three procedures. During tests for transitivity and equivalence, in the go/no-go procedure, 2/3 participants had positive demonstrations for those relations, 3/3 participants demonstrated emergent relations during the go/no-go with yes/no, and 3/3 participants demonstrated these relations during the MTS procedure. Figure 2 depicts errors per block across the three procedures for each participant during training. As one would expect, more errors were made across all procedures during the blocks near the beginning of training, as participants learned the contingencies the experimenter had programmed. For all participants, the go/nogo procedure produced the highest number of errors across blocks while

21 performances in the MTS procedure resulted in the lowest number of errors across blocks. Table 6 presents a more detailed analysis of the errors that participants NF made during tests for transitivity and equivalence in the go/no-go procedure. Participant NF initially made more errors to related compounds than non-related compounds, which may have been predicted, as a correct response to a non-related compound during the go/no-go is simply not touching the compound and waiting for the next trial presentation. Notably, during this procedure, in the test for transitivity and equivalence, she never responded correctly to the relations A3C3 and C3A3. Last, Table 7 presents data for two types of errors (omission and commission) across participants during the go/no-go procedure. If the number of commission errors was higher than omission errors, it would indicate that the addition of the yes/no component during the go/no-go procedure could be useful as an added feature to attempt to reduce participant errors made by responding to non-related compounds. This hypothesis is based on the assumption that refraining from responding is a challenging response for participants. For example, participants LEC and LC had a higher percent of commission errors in comparison to omission errors during the go/no-go procedure, indicating that refraining from responding to non-related compound stimuli may have been difficult.

22 Discussion The goal of this study was to replicate the results of Debert et al. (2007), and extend those results by comparing three procedures (go/no-go, go/no-go with yes/no components, and MTS) within and across participants. All participants in this study demonstrated symmetry across all three procedures. During transitivity and equivalence tests in the go/no-go procedure 2/3 participants demonstrated equivalence, while 3/3 participants demonstrated emergent relations during the go/nogo with yes/no, and MTS procedures. The results of the current study are promising in that all participants demonstrated transitivity and equivalence during the go/no-go with yes/no component, confirming that this procedure is another potential teaching alternative to the MTS procedure. One limitation to this study was that go/no-go was always the first procedure presented to participants, and therefore the first exposure the participants had to tests for transitivity and equivalence. For all participants, tests for these relations produced better outcomes in the subsequent phases where other procedures were used. Further research should rotate procedure order. In the current study, participant performances improved, even without providing programmed consequences for responding within session for tests of transitivity and equivalence. For example, during the go/no-go procedure for participant LEC, her scores ranged from 50% correct responding in the first block to correct responding in the second block. A similar pattern was observed with participants LC (scores ranging from 33.3% in block 1 to 95.8% in block 6) and NF

23 (scores ranging from 54.2% in block 1 to 83.3% in block 6). Lazar, Davis-Lang, and Sanchez (1984) reported similar results during test sessions. While it still remains unclear as to why this occurs, it may be that participants may simply require exposure to the testing procedure during tests for transitivity and equivalence. Green (2001) described three components that must occur during a MTS procedure in order to produce conditional discriminations. The participant must (a) discriminate among sample stimuli across trials, (b) discriminate among comparison stimuli within each trial, and (c) relate each comparison stimulus with only one specific sample stimulus. In a MTS procedure, the sample stimulus is the conditional stimulus whereas the comparison stimuli serve as discriminative stimuli. It should be noted that in MTS (as presented in the current study) all three comparison stimuli were presented together at one time (one S+ and two S-), perhaps facilitating the discrimination among comparison stimuli within each trial. In comparison, during go/no-go procedures, the conditional stimulus was presented next to the discriminative stimulus (either the S+ or the S-). It is interesting that the positive results for equivalence using go/no-go procedures suggest that equivalence can be demonstrated even when there were not two S- present when the conditional stimulus was presented. This arrangement may present a more difficult discrimination of conditional stimuli. Further research should further investigate this hypothesis. When examining the three procedures, it is important to note that the procedures teach participants to engage in different tasks while learning the conditional discriminations during training and therefore which teaching procedure is

24 more appropriate or even more efficient is likely to vary across individuals. For example, in MTS procedures, participants were required to observe four stimuli simultaneously and then select one comparison stimulus contingent upon which sample stimulus was presented. This procedure may be more appropriate for individuals who are capable of scanning multiple stimuli before engaging in a response. In comparison, in the go/no-go procedure, participants must have the ability to refrain from responding when non-related compounds are presented and wait until the next trial is presented before again deciding if they should respond. This procedure may not be appropriate for individuals who have a bias of always responding (selecting stimuli). Finally, the go/no-go procedure with yes/no component allows participants to always respond to stimuli, but only requires the participant to examine the compound stimulus, and does not require scanning across more than two stimuli at the same time. Future research is needed to further refine which methodology would be more appropriate for other populations of individuals, such as individuals with developmental disabilities and more closely examine the behavioral processes that underlie the three teaching methodologies. Additionally, future research should also explore using alternative stimuli to respond to during go/no-go procedures instead of yes/no. It is likely that participants have pre-experimental histories with the words yes and no which could influence participant responding. It would be interesting to examine responding and emergent relations if the yes/no were replaced with other arbitrary stimuli. It would also be beneficial to test if these arbitrary stimuli also enter into the stimuli classes.

25 References Cumming, W. W., & Berryman, R. (1961). Some data on matching behavior in the pigeon. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 4, 281-284. Debert, P., Matos, M. A., & McIlvane, W. (2007). Conditional relations with compound abstract stimuli using a go/no-go procedure. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 87, 89-96. Green, G. (2001). Behavior analytic instruction for learners with autism: Advances in stimulus control technology. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities, 16, 72-85. Lazar, R. M., Davis-Lang, D., & Sanchez, L. (1984). The formation of visual stimulus equivalences in children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 41, 251-266. Mallot, R. W., Mallot, K., Svinicki, J. G., Kladder, F., & Ponicki, E. (1971). An analysis of matching and non-matching behavior using a single key, free operant procedure. The Psychological Record, 21, 545-564. Markham, M. R., & Dougher, M. J. (1993). Compound stimuli in emergent stimulus relations: Extending the scope of stimulus equivalence. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 60, 529-542. Sidman, M. (1971). Reading and auditory-visual equivalences. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 5-13.

26 Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative. Sidman, M. & Tailby, W. (1982). Conditional discrimination vs. matching to sample: An expansion of the testing paradigm. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 5-22. Zentall, T. R., & Hogan, D. E., (1975). Concept learning in the pigeon: Transfer to new matching and non-matching stimuli. American Journal of Psychology, 88, 233-244.

27 Table 1 Stimuli used during Go/no-go, Go/no-go with Yes/no, and Match-to-Sample Procedures, Comprising the Three-member Stimulus Classes (1-3, 4-6, 7-9) Class 1 A B C Class 2 Class 3 Class 4 Class 5 & Class 6 ॐ Class 7 Ω Class 8 Ψ σ Class 9» ξ ρ

28 Table 2 Related and Non-related Compound Stimuli Presented in Go/no-go Procedures across Phases Go (Related Compounds) No-go (Non-related Compounds) Training A1B1 A1B2, A1B3 A2B2 A2B1, A2B3 A3B3 A3B1, A3B2 B1C1 B1C2, B1C3 B2C2 B2C1, B2C3 B3C3 B3C1, B3C2 Symmetry B1A1 B1A2, B1A3 B2A2 B2A1, B2A3 B3A3 B3A1, B3A2 C1B1 C1B2, C1B3 C2B2 C2B1, C2B3 C3B3 C3B1, C3B2 Transitivity A1C1 A1C2, A1C3 A2C2 A2C1, A2C3 A3C3 A3C1, A3C2 Equivalence C1A1 C1A2, C1A3 C2A2 C2A1, C2A3 C3A3 C3A1, C3A2

29 Table 3 Percentage of Correct Responding for Participant NF during Training and Testing Participant NF Go/No-go, 123 Go/No-go, Y/N, 456 MTS, 789 Training Session 1 Blocks 1-12 (Go/No-go) Blocks 1-8 (MTS) Session 2 Blocks 1-12 (Go/No-go) Blocks 1-8 (MTS) 85.1% (245/288) 96.5% (278/288) 87.9% (253/288) 99.7% (287/288) 77.1% (111/144) 98.6% (142/144) Symmetry Test Session 1 93.1% (134/144) 97.9% (141/144) 98.6% (71/72) Block 1 54.2% (13/24) 91.7% (22/24) 33.3% (6/18) Transitivity and Equivalence Tests Block 2 Block 3 Block 4 Block 5 Block 6 Blocks 1-6 (Go/No-go) Blocks 1-4 (MTS) 66.7% (16/24) 75% (18/24) 79.2% (19/24) 75% (18/24) 83.3% (20/24) 72.2% (104/144) 91.7% (22/24) 91.7% (22/24) (24/24) (24/24) (24/24) 95.8% (138/144) (18/18) (18/18) (18/18) 83.3% (60/72)

30 Table 4 Percentage of Correct Responding for Participant LEC during Training and Testing Participant LEC Go/No-go, 789 Go/No-go, Y/N, 123 MTS, 456 Training Session 1 Blocks 1-12 (Go/No-go) Blocks 1-8 (MTS) 91.7% (264/288) 95.5% (275/288) 97.9% (141/144) Symmetry Test Session 1 (144/144) (144/144) (72/72) Block 1 50% (12/24) 95.8% (23/24) (18/18) Transitivity and Equivalence Tests Block 2 (24/24) Block 3 95.8% (23/24) Block 4 (24/24) Block 5 (24/24) Block 6 (24/24) Blocks 1-6 (Go/No-go) Blocks 1-4 (MTS) 91% 131/144 (24/24) (24/24) (24/24) (24/24) (24/24) 99.3% (143/144) (18/18) (18/18) (18/18) (72/72)

31 Table 5 Percentage of Correct Responding for Participant LC during Training and Testing Training Participant LC Go/No-go, 456 Go/No-go, Y/N, 789 MTS, 123 Session 1 Blocks 1-12 (Go/No-go) Blocks 1-8 (MTS) Session 2 Blocks 1-12 (Go/No-go) 90.3% (260/288) (288/288) 93.4% (269/288) 92.4% (133/144) Symmetry Test Session 1 (144/144) (144/144) (72/72) Block 1 33.3% (8/24) (24/24) (18/18) Transitivity and Equivalence Tests Block 2 87.5% (21/24) Block 3 91.7% (22/24) Block 4 95.8% (23/24) Block 5 95.8% (23/24) Block 6 95.8% (23/24) Blocks 1-6 (Go/No-go) Blocks 1-4 (MTS) 83.3% (120/144) (24/24) (24/24) 100.0% (24/24) 100.0% (24/24) 100.0% (24/24) (144/144) (18/18) (18/18) (18/18) (72/72)

32 Table 6 Percentage of Correct Performances and the Compound Stimuli to Which Participant NF Produced Errors during Tests for Transitivity and Equivalence during the Go/no-go Procedure Participant Related NF Go/No-go Block 1 33.3% (4/12) Block 2 50% (6/12) Block 3 50% (6/12) Block 4 58.3% (7/12) Block 5 58.3% (7/12) Block 6 66.6% (8/12) Not Related 75% (9/12) 83.3% (10/12) (12/12) (12/12) 91.7% (11/12) (12/12) Errors in related A1C1, A3C3, A3C3, C1A1, C2A2, C2A2, C3A3, C3A3 A3C3, A3C3, C2A2, C2A2, C3A3, C3A3 Errors in not related A1C3, A2C3, C3A1 A1C2, C3A1 A2C2, A3C3, A3C3, C2A2, C3A3, C3A3 - A2C2, A3C3, A3C3, C3A3, C3A3 - A3C3, A3C3, C2A2, C3A3, C3A3 A1C2 A3C3, A3C3, C3A3, C3A3 -

33 Table 7 Comparison across Participants for Commission and Omission Errors during Training Session(s) in the Go/no-go Procedure Go/No-Go, NF TRN 1-2 S+ S- Response CORRECT (266/576) 46.2% Commission (22/576) 3.8% No Response Omission (31/576) 5.4% CORRECT (257/576) 44.6% Go/No-Go, LEC TRN 1 S+ S- Response CORRECT (134/288) 46.5% Commission (14/288) 4.9% No Response Omission (10/288) 3.5% CORRECT (130/288) 45.1% Go/No-Go, LC TRN 1-2 S+ S- Response CORRECT (279/576) 48.4% Commission (19/576) 3.3% No Response Omission (9/576) 1.6% CORRECT (269/576) 46.7%

34 Figure Captions Figure 1. Percent of correct responding across training and testing for all three procedures, with the tests for transitivity and equivalence separated into blocks within session for each participant. Figure 2. Errors per block during training session(s) for all three procedures, across participants.

35

36