Behavioral measurement of background adaptation in infants. RONALD M. HANSEN

Similar documents
Scotopic contrast sensitivity in infants evaluated by evoked potentials

field, the increment-threshold functions have two distinct branches. It was shown,

Temporal Feature of S-cone Pathway Described by Impulse Response Function

The lowest level of stimulation that a person can detect. absolute threshold. Adapting one's current understandings to incorporate new information.

7. Sharp perception or vision 8. The process of transferring genetic material from one cell to another by a plasmid or bacteriophage

The relationship between visual sensitivity and rhodopsin density in retinitis pigmentosa

Fluctuations on the Humphrey and Octopus Perimeters

OPTO 5320 VISION SCIENCE I

Mr. Silimperi Council Rock High School South Chapter 5 Sensation Sensation II

2. METHODS. 2.1 Apparatus

Normal and amblyopic contrast sensitivity functions in central and peripheral retinas

The color of night: Surface color categorization by color defective observers under dim illuminations

Modifiers and Retransmitters (Secondary Light Sources)

Human scotopic dark adaptation: Comparison of recoveries of psychophysical threshold and ERG b<wave sensitivity

Basic Electrophysiology, the Electroretinogram (ERG) and the Electrooculogram (EOG) - Signal origins, recording methods and clinical applications

A Psychophysical Technique For Measuring Cone Photopigment Bleaching

Electro Retinogram Basics and Major Clinical Applications

Vision Seeing is in the mind

Psychophysical estimates of opponent-process response functions*

Psy393: Cognitive Neuroscience. Prof. Anderson Department of Psychology Week 3

Test Bank Chapter 2: The Beginnings of Perception

SENSES: VISION. Chapter 5: Sensation AP Psychology Fall 2014

Evidence that a-wave Latency of the Electroretinogram Is Determined Solely by Photoreceptors

TUMBLING E RESOLUTION PERIMETRY IN GLAUCOMA

Orcadian Rhythm in Goldfish Visual Sensitivity

Construction of the Visual Image

REACTION TIME AS A MEASURE OF INTERSENSORY FACILITATION l

Perimetric testing is used clinically to detect visual field

Vision Research. Very-long-term and short-term chromatic adaptation: Are their influences cumulative? Suzanne C. Belmore a, Steven K.

Foveal cone photopigment bleaching in central serous retinopathy

Seeing Color. Muller (1896) The Psychophysical Axioms. Brindley (1960) Psychophysical Linking Hypotheses

Specific deficits of flicker sensitivity in glaucoma and ocular hypertension

SPATIAL AND TEMPORAL DETERMINANTS OF VISUAL BACKWARD MASKING 1

What do we perceive?

Lighting Conditions and Retinal Development in Goldfish: Absolute Visual Sensitivity

Dark-Adapted Thresholds in Children With Histories of Mild Retinopathy of Prematurity

Guidelines for clinical electroretinography in the dog: 2012 update

From last week: The body is a complex electrical machine. Basic Electrophysiology, the Electroretinogram ( ERG ) and the Electrooculogram ( EOG )

Cambridge, Massachusetts 02138, U.S.A.

Definition Slides. Sensation. Perception. Bottom-up processing. Selective attention. Top-down processing 11/3/2013

= add definition here. Definition Slide

Neural circuits PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 05. Rods and cones

Laboratory Exercise in Sensory Physiology Student Lab Manual

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Spring 2003 Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination

Accepted Manuscript. The neural retina in retinopathy of prematurity. Ronald M. Hansen, Anne Moskowitz, James D. Akula, Anne B.

Psychology of Perception Psychology 4165, Fall 2001 Laboratory 1 Weight Discrimination

Comparison of Pattern VEPs and Preferential-Looking Behavior in 3-Month-Old Infants

Richman Face Dot Test Paddle

ID# Exam 1 PS 325, Fall 2001

CAROL 0. ECKERMAN UNIVERSITY OF NORTH CAROLINA. in which stimulus control developed was studied; of subjects differing in the probability value

backgrounds in driving the visual system up its intensity-response function. A

Color vision deficiency in retinitis pigmentosa

eye as a camera Kandel, Schwartz & Jessel (KSJ), Fig 27-3

Night vision in a case of vitamin A deficiency due to malabsorption

Answer three questions out of four questions.

Selective changes of sensitivity after adaptation to simple geometrical figures*

LEA Color Vision Testing

Senses are transducers. Change one form of energy into another Light, sound, pressure, etc. into What?

PIII and derived PII analysis in a patient with retinal dysfunction with supernormal scotopic ERG

Fundamentals of Psychophysics

ASSUMPTION OF COGNITIVE UNIFORMITY

M Cells. Why parallel pathways? P Cells. Where from the retina? Cortical visual processing. Announcements. Main visual pathway from retina to V1

Biological Psychology. Unit Two AD Mr. Cline Marshall High School Psychology

Practice Test Questions

CP116. Mixed-Color LED Lighting with Circadian Benefits

(Received 19 April 1977)

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus

Sensation and Perception. A. Sensation: awareness of simple characteristics B. Perception: making complex interpretations

Human colour perception and its adaptation

A. Acuity B. Adaptation C. Awareness D. Reception E. Overload

Visual Physiology. Perception and Attention. Graham Hole. Problems confronting the visual system: Solutions: The primary visual pathways: The eye:

COURSE: OPTO 328 Physiology of Vision I

CS294-6 (Fall 2004) Recognizing People, Objects and Actions Lecture: January 27, 2004 Human Visual System

The effect of stimulus duration on the persistence of gratings

Lateral interactions in visual perception of temporal signals: cortical and subcortical components

Reading With Rods: The Superiority of Central Vision for Rapid Reading

Neuroscience - Problem Drill 13: The Eye and Visual Processing

Concurrent measurement of perceived speed and speed discrimination threshold using the method of single stimuli

Reports 981. Reflection of light by small areas of the ocular fundus. ROBERT W. FLOWER, D.

THE VISUAL WORLD! Visual (Electromagnetic) Stimulus

Supplementary Appendix

Reliability and Significance of Measurements of a-wave Latency in Rats

Carlson (7e) PowerPoint Lecture Outline Chapter 6: Vision

Cambridge CB2 3EG (Received 8 November 1972)

The Visual System. Retinal Anatomy Dr. Casagrande February 2, Phone: Office: T2302 MCN

V1 (Chap 3, part II) Lecture 8. Jonathan Pillow Sensation & Perception (PSY 345 / NEU 325) Princeton University, Fall 2017

Characteristics of Frequency-of-Seeing Curves in Normal Subjects, Patients With Suspected Glaucoma, and Patients With Glaucoma

Hartline, 1935). J. Physiol. (I95I) II4, 8-I5. wave-length, luminosity curves for various animals under different conditions

The Effects of Luminance on FPL and VEP Acuity in Human Infants DALE ALLEN, * PATRICK J. BENNETT, * MARTIN S. BANKS

The effects of subthreshold synchrony on the perception of simultaneity. Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität Leopoldstr 13 D München/Munich, Germany

Psychology Unit 3 Test

Characteristic ERG Flicker Anomaly in Incomplete Congenital Stationary Night Blindness

Development of retinal synaptic arrays in the inner plexiform layer of dark-reared mice

Sensation and Perception. Chapter 6

Electroretinographic abnormalities and advanced multiple sclerosis

Dark and light adaptation: a job that is accomplished mainly in the retina

Contrast discrimination with sinusoidal gratings of different spatial frequency

Supporting Information

Frequency & Amplitude Ranges for Bioelectric Signals

Transcription:

Volume 21 Number 4 Reports 625 11. Ehinger B: Biogenic monoamines as transmitters in the retina. In Transmitters in the Visual Process, Bonting SL, editor. New York, 1976, Pergaman Press, p. 145. 12. Dowling JE: The site of visual adaptation. Science 155:273, 1967. 13. Marmor MF and Lurie M: Light-induced electrical responses of the retinal pigment epithelium. In The Retinal Pigment Epithelium, Marmor MF and Zinn KM, editors. Cambridge, Mass., 1979, Harvard University Press, p. 226. 14. Rodieck RW: Components of the electroretinogram a reappraisal. Vision Res 12:773, 1972. Behavioral measurement of background adaptation in infants. RONALD M. HANSEN AND ANNE B. FULTON. Thresholds for detecting blue test flashes in the darkadapted condition and on steady red background fields were measured in 2- to 18-week-old human infants by a two-alternative forced-choice preferential looking method. The results show that dark-adapted sensitivity increases and background adaptation develops during the early postnatal weeks. Thus, the retinal mechanisms that underlie (1) detection of brief flashes and (2) neural processing in background adaptation appear to mature postnatally. (INVEST OPHTHALMOL VIS SCI 21:625-629, 1981.) Recently Powers et al.' showed that the scotopic sensitivity of human infants increased postnatally. This demonstration, along with the finding that background adaptation in infant rats continued to develop after distal retinal morphogenesis was nearly complete 2 (as in neonatal humans 3 ), raised the possibility that scotopic background adaptation also developed postnatally in human infants. To find out, we have modified the two-alternative forced-choice preferential looking method 4 to measure the effects of steady backgrounds on thresholds for detecting flashes of light. Preliminary results indicate that background adaptation in very young infants is different from that of adults but matures during the early postnatal weeks. Methods. Three 500 W tungsten sources projected test stimuli and backgrounds onto a 107 by 107 cm rear projection screen in a dark room. One source produced a diffusely illuminated 110 diameter circular background field centered on the screen. The other beams, one on the right and one on the left, produced 10 diameter test spots located 20 to the right and left of the center of the screen. The sources, lenses, and stops were mounted in light-tight boxes to prevent stray light from reaching the screen. Test stimuli were 1 sec duration blue (Ilford 621)flashes;the steady background beam was red (Wratten 29). Both background and test-spot intensity were controlled by neutral density filters. Luminance measurements were made" with a calibrated photodiode (UDT; Model 11 A). The unattenuated test beams had a luminance of 2.11 log ml, and the background had a luminance of 2.86 log ml. After dark adaptationfor30 min, the infant was held 50 cm in front of the dark rear projection screen by an adult (the holder). A second adult (the experimenter, positioned behind the screen) attracted the infant's gaze to the center of the screen by flickering a dim yellow flashlight (30 min arc diameter). A third adult (the observer) watched the child with an infrared viewer (FJW Industries; Model 3420). The observer, hidden in black curtains adjacent to the screen, could view the infant from above, below, or to the right or left of the screen. When the observer reported that the infant was alert and looking at the center of the screen, the experimenter extinguished the fixation light and presented a test spot. The observer faced the infant and could not see the stimuli. Based on the infant's looking behavior, the observer reported stimulus location on every trial. After each response, the experimenter told the observer whether the judgment was correct. We assume that the infant looked toward the flash if it was detected. Stimuli were repeated, when necessary, to allow the observer to make a decision. Initially the infant was shown stimuli that were 2 to 3 log units above the adult dark-adapted threshold. These stimuli were detected readily and enabled the observer to become familiar with the infant's looking behavior. If the observer correctly reported spot location on the first five trials (100% correct), test-spot intensity was reduced by 0.6 log units.* Target intensity was reduced further until the observer made an error. Then, ten trials were run at that and each succeeding test-flash intensity. Stimulus intensity was reduced in 0.3 log unit steps *The probability of guessing stimulus location correctly is 0.5 in a two-alternative forced-choice task. Assuming that each trial was an independent event, the probability that the observer correctly guessed test-spot location on five trials in a row was 0.031 (from the binomial probability distribution with n = 5, r = 5, and p = 0.5). Thus it was unlikely that the observer's percent correct was due to guessing, but rather reflected the infant's visual performance. 0146-0404/81/100625+05$00.50/0 1981 Assoc. for Res. in Vis. and Ophthal., Inc.

626 Reports Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. October 1981 A 75 "- -2-3 LOG TO \ 50 60 6-5 70 \ 50 60 6-5 70 70 7-5 80 7-5 80 8-5 LOG FLASH INTENSITY (QUANTA/ CM 2 / SEC) B 8 T -3-2 -i o +1 + 2 LOG BACKGROUND INTENSITY (SCOTOPIC TD) Fig. 1. Results of psychophysical measurement of retinal adaptation to red backgrounds in a 2-week-old infant. A, Psychometric functions from six adaptation conditions. Observer's percent correct is plotted as a function of log test-flash intensity (in quanta sec" 1 cm"" 2 at 502 nm incident at the cornea) in the no-background (dark-adapted) condition and in each of five background conditions producing retinal illuminations of 3.3 to +0.7 log scotopic trolands. Arrows indicate interpolated thresholds for 75% correct responses. B, Results of this experiment were summarized by plotting log threshold test-flash intensity as a function of log background intensity. Open square (on the axis), Threshold in the dark-adapted (no-background) condition. until performance fell to chance (50% correct). With adult subjects, this order and rate of stimulus presentation did not alter the state of adaptation. We assume that this was also the case with infants. Occasionally, when test stimuli were near threshold, the experimenter added brighter test flashes to keep the infant alert and interested. A dim red background of about 3 log scotopic trolands was added and a second psychometric function was measured. The steady background was then increased by 1.0 log unit and another function was measured. This process was continued up to a background of about +2 log scotopic trolands. The 120 to 150 trials from which Weber- Fechner functions 5 were derived were completed in 1 day. Total test time for infants was about 2 hr; adults completed testing in less than an hour. One infant was tested at age 2 weeks, one at 4

Volume 11 Number 4 Reports 627 LOG BACKGROUND INTENSITY (SCOTOPIC TROLANDS ) Fig. 2. Increment threshold curves at various ages. Log threshold test-flash intensity is plotted as a function of log background intensity for infants tested at age 2, 4, 8, 12, and 18 weeks. The final graph presents the averaged results from five normal adult subjects tested with the same apparatus and procedure; error bars show ±1 S.D. For comparison, the average adult curve is plotted as a dashed line on each of the other graphs. weeks, one at 8 weeks, one at 12 weeks, and one at 18 weeks. All infants tested were born at term, and ophthalmic examination revealed no abnormalities. Increment threshold curves were also obtained from five adults using the same apparatus and order of stimulus presentation; adults named the position of the stimuli, right or left. For all experiments, both adult and infant subjects' pupils werefixedand dilated with Cyclogyl 1% or Mydriacyl 1%. Pupillary diameter, measured at the end of each test session, was used to calculate retinal illumination. Results. Psychometric functions from an infant tested at age two weeks are presented in Fig. 1, A; each function summarizes data from a single adaptation condition. Observer's percent correct detection varied from 100% correct for bright stimuli that were well above threshold to chance (50% correct) for dim stimuli. Threshold was defined as the stimulus intensity that was reported correctly 75% of the time. Thresholds for all subjects were determined graphically from psychometric functions such as those in Fig. 1, A. Thresholds of the 2-week-old infant in each adaptation condition are plotted as a function of log background intensity in Fig. 1, B. The results from the 2-week-old and other infants are summarized in Fig. 2. For comparison, thresholds obtained from five adult subjects are also shown in Fig. 2. The dark-adapted threshold for the 2-week-old infant was equivalent to 6.3 log quanta sec"' cm" 2 at 502 nm incident on the cornea; this is about 1.4 log units above the adult dark-adapted threshold.* The sensitivity of dark-adapted infants increased with age (Fig. 2). At age 18 weeks, dark-adapted sen- *Retinal illuminance at threshold was calculated with the relation n(\) = I T tx (Ap/A s ) where n(\) is the number of quanta sec" 1 cm" 2 (502 nm) at the retina, I T is the threshold test flash in quanta sec"' cm" 2 incident at the cornea, tx 502 is the effective transmittance for adults 6 or infants, 7 A p is the pupil area in cm, 2 and A s is the retinal area of the stimulus in cm 2. I T (502 nm) was calculated from the output of the source measured with a calibrated photodiode in the position of the subject's eye; the energy distribution of the source and relative sensitivity of the diode were taken into account. Scotopic matches for the blue filter were set by adult observers and the attenuation caused by the filter determined assuming (1) the output of the source was constant across the wavelengths passed by the blue filter and (2) all wavelengths were attenuated uniformly. Age-related changes in eye size 8 were taken into account. At age 2 weeks, n(x) = 6.86 log quanta sec" 1 cm" 2, 6.84 at 4 weeks, 6.42 at 8 weeks, 6.13 at 12 weeks, 5.67 at 18 weeks, and 5.09 for the average adult threshold.

628 Reports Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. October 1981 sitivity equaled that of adults. The average adult dark-adapted threshold (4.9 log quanta sec" 1 cm" 2 at 502 nm incident at the cornea) agreed well with thresholds reported by Denton and Pirenne 9 for subjects viewing large (45 diameter), long duration (5 sec) stimuli binocularly. The eigengrau (intersection of asymptotes to the horizontal and rising limbs of the Weber-Fechner function 5 ) for adults was 1.7 log scotopic trolands (S.D. = 0.2 log scotopic trolands; n = 5); this is somewhat higher than the value found in the best of psychophysical test conditions. 0 Although some of the curves are difficult to interpret, the eigengrau for most infant subjects was approximately equal to that of adults; no systematic changes with increasing postnatal age were seen. The slope of the linear portion of the adult curve (Fig. 2) was +0.96 (S.D. = 0.04; n = 5) and is similar to that reported by others. 5 The steepest portion of the incremental threshold curve for the 2-week-old infant was +0.6. Between ages 2 and 18 weeks the slope increased, and by age 18 weeks it was +0.9. Discussion. The progressive decrease in the dark-adapted threshold and increase in the slope of the increment threshold curves suggests that adaptive processes develop in the early postnatal weeks. These age-ralated changes might be attributed to increasing attentiveness to dim stimuli as the infants became older. However, throughout the test sessions for infants of all ages, the observer's score remained at or close to 100% correct for bright stimuli and decreased in a regular fashion to 50% as the stimuli were made dimmer (Fig. 1, A). Also, direct surveillance by the observer ensured that the infant was indeed alert and in good position whenever a trial was initiated. Thus attentional factors are unlikely to have influenced the functions obtained. The absolute sensitivity of our dark-adapted 4- and 12-week-old infants was in reasonable agreement with those of 1- and 3-month-old infants studied by Powers et al.' In that study, the spectral sensitivity indicated that the responses were rod mediated; thus the dark-adapted thresholds of our infants must also be rod mediated. One would be suspicious of cone intrusion in the various background conditions, partly because anatomical maturation of peripheral cones precedes that of rods 3 and in adults rod-cone interactions occur with blue test flashes in the presence of red backgrounds. 10 However, responses are rod mediated if, as in the present experiment, background fields are large and peripheral retina is tested. 5 ' u In those adult experiments 5 ' " in which it was possible to exploit fully spectral and directional sensitivities, the scotopic increment threshold curves obtained were similar to those of our adults. Direct observation of the infants suggests that they also used peripheral retina to detect the test flashes; the infants appeared tofixatethe center of the screen at the start of each trial, but after presentation of the 20 eccentric test flash, typically turned head or eyes, or both, toward the flash on the right or the left. Thus we suspect that the infants' increment thresholds also represent scotopic function, although the contribution of peripheral cones to visual sensitivity and the role of rod-cone interactions in the early postnatal period cannot yet be specified. The increase in slope of the linear portion of the incremental threshold curves that occurs between age 2 and 18 weeks in human infants is similar to that of young rats between ages 18 and 40 days. 2 The scotopic b-wave that was used to assess sensitivity in the rats depends mainly on distal retinal activity; the increasing slope of the incremental b-wave sensitivity curves is thought to reflect maturation of the functional organization of the rat retina. 2 The changes in the slope of the human infants' Weber-Fechner functions 5 may also be caused by postnatal development of retinal function. Adaptive mechanisms in infants can be tested further in experiments that vary temporal, spatial, and spectral properties of stimuli; retinal organization of adults is such that these parameters influence the shape of Weber-Fechner functions. 5 Meanwhile, the procedure as described allows measurement of the dark-adapted threshold and steady-state retinal adaptation in pediatric patients who cannot have retinal function evaluated by other psychophysical methods. 12 From the Department of Ophthalmology, Children's Hospital Medical Center, and Harvard Medical School, Boston, Mass. Supported by grants from the Whittaker Health Sciences Fund, the National Society to Prevent Blindness, and National Institutes of Health grant R01 EYHD 03211-01. Submitted for publication May 27, 1981. Reprint requests: Anne B. Fulton, M.D., Department of Ophthalmology, CHMC, 300 Longwood Ave., Boston, Mass. 02115. Key words: human infants, vision, retinal adaptation, development REFERENCES 1. Powers MK, Schneck M, and Teller DY: Spectral sensitivity of human infants at absolute visual threshold. Vision Res 21:1005, 1981.

Volume 21 Number 4 Reports 629 2. Fulton AB and Graves AL: Background adaptation in developing rat retina: an electroretinographic study. Vision Res 20:819, 1980. 3. Mann I: The neural ectoderm. In Development of the Human Eye. New York, 1969, Grune & Stratton, Inc., pp. 68-149. 4. Teller DY: The forced-choice preferential looking method: a psychophysical technique for use with human infants. Infant Behav Dev 2:135, 1979. 5. Barlow HB: Dark and light adaptation: psychophysics. In Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. 7: Visual Psychophysics, Hurvich LM and Jameson D, editors. Berlin, 1972, Springer Verlag, ch. 4, pp. 1-28. 6. Von Norren D and Vos JJ: Spectral transmission of the human ocular media. Vision Res 14:1237, 1974. 7. Werner JS: Developmental changes in scotopic sensitivity and the absorption spectrum of the human ocular media. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Brown University, 1979.) 8. Larsen J: The sagittal growth of the eye. Ultrasonic measurement of the axial length of the eye from birth to puberty. Acta Ophthalmol 49:873, 1971. 9. Denton EJ and Pirenne MH: The absolute sensitivity and functional stability of the human eye. J Physiol 123:417, 1954. 10. Makous W and Booth R: Cones block signals from rods. Vision Res 14:285, 1974. 11. Latch M and Lennie P: Rod-cone interaction in light adaptation. J Physiol 269:517, 1977. 12. Barg D, Hansen R, Fulton A, Shwachman H, and Underwood B: Dark adaptation and vitamin A in cystic fibrosis. INVEST OPHTHALMOL VIS SCI 20 (ARVO Suppl.):145, 1981.