The Broad Autism Phenotype: Birth Order as it Relates to. Pragmatic Language and Self-Competence in Siblings. Sarah Griffin

Similar documents
The Autism Families Research Study: Siblings of Children with ASD. Research Summary Report

SIBLINGS OF CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY 1

Siblings of children on the autism spectrum

The Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire

What is Down syndrome?

The broad autism phenotype predicts child functioning in autism spectrum disorders

T. Rene Jamison * and Jessica Oeth Schuttler

Depression, Temperament and their Relationship to Other Characteristics in Children with Asperger's Disorder. Abstract

Social Communication in young adults with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi

The Nuts and Bolts of Diagnosing Autism Spectrum Disorders In Young Children. Overview

Misunderstood Girls: A look at gender differences in Autism

Inclusive Education. De-mystifying Intellectual Disabilities and investigating best practice.

Pragmatic language in fragile X syndrome, autism, and Down syndrome

Teaching Students with Special Needs in Inclusive Settings: Exceptional Learners Chapter 9: Autism Spectrum Disorders

Social and Pragmatic Language in Autistic Children

Is Asperger Syndrome The Same As Autism?

Language and Speech Delays in Children with an Autism Spectrum Disorder. Bootsie. Auburn University School of Nursing

Autism in U.S. children on the rise

AAA. Report #10 A Profile of Adolescent and Adult Siblings. - Principal Investigators Marsha Mailick Seltzer, Ph.D.

Autism/Autism Spectrum Disorders

The Needs of Young People who have a Sibling with Cancer.

Running Head: VISUAL SCHEDULES FOR STUDENTS WITH AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER

Everyone Managing Disability in the Workplace Version 1

Broader Autism Phenotype in Siblings of Children with ASD A Review

ONE IN ONE HUNDRED AND TEN PERSONS!! 5/5/2010

Overview. Clinical Features

The DSM-IV-TR diagnoses autism as having the symptoms of. qualitative impairment in social interaction, qualitative impairments in

People with autism need specialist care and education. EMIS 2010, as distributed onhttp:// Autism.htm Used with permission.

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

The Knowledge and Awareness of Autism Spectrum Disorders within the Hispanic Community.

THE SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL ADJUSTMENT OF SIBLINGS OF CHILDREN WITH DISABILITIES. By Rhianna M. Gerfin. A Thesis. Submitted in Partial Fulfillment

From: What s the problem? Pathway to Empowerment. Objectives 12/8/2015

Discussion Starter: Autism Awareness. A mini-reader & Lesson Ideas Created by: Primarily AU-Some 2013 & 2014

National Inspection of services that support looked after children and care leavers

Autism Diagnosis as a Social Process

Kids Booklet 5 & on Autism. Create an autism awareness ribbon! Tips for parents & teachers. Activities puzzles

Participation in play and leisure occupations in children who have a sibling with autism

Pragmatic language impairments

Enjoying music therapy class. By Sheri S. Dollin, M.Ed., Lori Vincent, M.Ed.,

AUTISM PARENT HANDBOOK. Answers to common questions. Artwork: Hey Diddle Diddle, by Eytan Nisinzweig, an artist with autism.

17q12 Review. What s in a name? Spring Parental Testing: What, Why, & How. Conference Updates

10/18/2016. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Third Edition 1. Meet Dr. Saulnier. Bio. Celine A. Saulnier, PhD Vineland-3 Author

A Longitudinal Pilot Study of Behavioral Abnormalities in Children with Autism

By Rebbie Straubing

Views of autistic adults on assessment in the early years

topic : Co-Morbid Conditions by Cindy Ring, MSW, LSW and Michele LaMarche, BCBA

What Do We Know: Autism Screening and Diagnosis and Supporting Families of Young Children

Diagnostic tests for autism may miss many girls

When 6 is Bigger Than 10: Unmasking Anorexia Through Externalisation. By Hugh Fox

Getting through a diagnosis of Autism How to support family members

Autism, my sibling, and me

Running head: ARTICLE CRITIQUE 1

Autism Lab. The MSU Autism Lab was founded in 2007 by Brooke Ingersoll, PhD, BCBA. Our lab is interested in studying socialcommunication

Study on Family Adaptation to Fragile X Syndrome

Sign Language and Early Childhood Development

TTI Success Insights Emotional Quotient Version

Personal Statement Workshop Guidelines, Tips, & Suggestions

Towson University Center for Adults with Autism Towson, MD Adventure Pursuits for Adults with Autism

Background on the issue Previous study with adolescents and adults: Current NIH R03 study examining ADI-R for Spanish speaking Latinos

The University of Akron University Council Satisfaction Survey Report May 9, 2017

Introduction. Preliminary POV. Additional Needfinding Results. CS Behavioral Change Studio ASSIGNMENT 2 POVS and Experience Prototypes

Age of diagnosis for Autism Spectrum Disorders. Reasons for a later diagnosis: Earlier identification = Earlier intervention

This report summarizes the stakeholder feedback that was received through the online survey.

ECI WEBINAR SERIES: PRACTICAL STRATEGIES FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Kathleen McConnell Fad, Ph.D.

Instructional Practices for Students with Autism A.. Kimberly Howard M.Ed.

Autism 101: An Introduction for Families

Stuttering. Risk factors that predict a chronic problem rather than spontaneous recovery include:

AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS. Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician

Collaboration in neurodivergent teams

Include Autism Presents: The Volunteer Handbook

Transition Time: Helping Individuals on the Autism Spectrum Move Successfully from One Activity to Another

The Relationship between YouTube Interaction, Depression, and Social Anxiety. By Meredith Johnson

A Qualitative Study of the Effects of the University of Arkansas Autism Support Program

A Comparison Between High-Risk and Low-Risk Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

Deborah E. Schadler, PhD, PRSE. Gwynedd Mercy Unversity Director, Autism Institute

Siblings of Children with Autism: Sibling Support Intervention Program. Keisha Gafoor. A Thesis in The Department of Education

The needs of iranian families of children with autism spectrum disorder, cross-cultural study

Clinical Validity of the NIMHANS Sentence Completion Test for Children and Adolescents

Understanding Autism. Julie Smith, MA, BCBA. November 12, 2015

TSC AND AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDERS

Buckinghamshire Adult Autism Diagnostic Service

Quality of life and autistics: the critical roles of social support and subjective well-being. Steven Kapp

Simons VIP Phenotyping: What we ve learned so far. Ellen Hanson, Ph.D. and Raphael Bernier, Ph.D. Family Meeting Summer, 2015

April is Autism Awareness Month! What is Autism? Quick Facts & Stats. Bri Klibbe Jill Paul

MEETING #5: FINDING OUR VOICES

SAMPLE. Autism Spectrum Rating Scales (6-18 Years) Teacher Ratings. Interpretive Report. By Sam Goldstein, Ph.D. & Jack A. Naglieri, Ph.D.

Pragmatics and Disordered Speech. Mackenzie Salt, M.A., H.B.A., D-TEIL

Helping your Child with ASD Adjust to New Siblings. Af ter the baby s birth

Asperger's Syndrome WHAT IS ASPERGER'S? Article QUICK LINKS :

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Diagnosis Advancements. Licensee OAPL (UK) Creative Commons Attribution License (CC-BY) Research study

Author's response to reviews

Understanding Children with Autism Tracey Teo and Robert Jackson Edith Cowan University Western Australia

Hearing Loss and Autism. diagnosis and intervention

Autism and Physical Education: Strategies for Success JUSTIN A. HAEGELE, PHD, CAPE OLD DOMINION UNIVERSITY

Family-centered Stress Management for Childhood Cancer: A Multimodal Intervention for Children Newly Diagnosed with Cancer and their Families

Beattie Learning Disabilities Continued Part 2 - Transcript

Improving Communication in Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) Eniola Lahanmi Speech & Language Therapist

An Introduction to Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD)

Getting Started: Introducing Your Child to His or Her Diagnosis of Autism or Asperger Syndrome

Transcription:

Running Head: BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE The Broad Autism Phenotype: Birth Order as it Relates to Pragmatic Language and Self-Competence in Siblings Sarah Griffin University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill segriffi@live.unc.edu April 7 th, 2015 A thesis presented to the faculty of The University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Bachelor of Science degree with Honors in Psychology. Advisor Dr. J Steven Reznick Reader Dr. Lauren Turner- Brown Reader Dr. Elizabeth Pungello

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 2 Abstract One in 68 children in the United States were diagnosed with Autism Spectrum Disorder in 2014 (CDC, 2014). This is a 55% increase from 2002, in which 1 in 150 children in the U.S. were diagnosed. Using U.S. census data, this means that around 300,000 parents and siblings are living with the challenge of raising and growing up alongside a child with autism. This study delves into the sibling experience, examining birth order as it relates to pragmatic language functioning and selfcompetence in typically developing siblings of children with autism. There was no effect of birth order on the pragmatic language subset of the BAPQ; however, there was a significant negative correlation between pragmatic language scores, where higher scores indicate lower pragmatic function, and parent-reported scores of their typically-developing child s self-competence. Together, these findings suggest that typically-developing siblings who don t use social language as proficiently as other children may have lower levels of self-competence and be targeted for increased support. birth order Keywords: autism, siblings, self-competence, pragmatic language, Broad Autism Phenotype,

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 3 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Firstly, I thank my advisor, Dr. J Steven Reznick, for his guidance and support throughout this entire process. I very much appreciate his willingness to make time in his extremely busy schedule to make sure I was knowledgeable about each step in the research process. I am deeply honored to have been mentored by him and I will take the lessons learned from this process throughout my entire research career. I would also like to thank Dr. Lauren Turner-Brown for her continued support throughout this year and for her participation on my committee. In addition, I would like to thank Dr. Elizabeth Pungello for her positivity and participation on my committee. Additionally, thanks must be given to Renée Clark and the N.C. Autism Registry for providing access to participants. I would like to thank generally those who have had a guiding presence throughout my undergraduate research career, namely Dr. Linda Watson, Dr. Brian Boyd, Caroline McCarty, Dr. Stephanie Reszka, Katie Belardi, and Anna Ollinger. Lastly, I d like to thank my friends and family for their support throughout this process. Even if they didn t quite understand the inner workings of my project, their support means just as much. Specifically, I d like to send extra thanks to my sisters for inspiring this Honors Thesis topic and my wonderful parents for immersing our family in the autism community. Finding my calling so early in life is a blessing for which I will never be able to fully express my gratitude.

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 4 The Broad Autism Phenotype: Birth Order as it Relates to Pragmatic Language and Self-Competence in Siblings With the rise of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) diagnoses, more and more children are being targeted for intervention and support. The families of these children are also in need of support. While much research has focused on the implications of the ASD diagnosis on parents, there has been less focus on its effect on siblings. The sibling relationship is one of the most influential relationships for children during development. At present, more children grow up in a home with a sibling than in a home with a father (McHale, Updegraff, & Whiteman, 2012). Most children, therefore, are exposed to a sibling relationship and the consequent effects on their development. Sibling relationships provide the basis through which social, emotional, and cognitive skills are generalized to interactions with others across the lifespan through interactions like turn-taking and sharing. The nature of sibling interactions can especially impact how a child creates peer relations. Sibling relationships where one member has a developmental disability show marked differences from typical sibling dyads. The sibling relationship including a child with ASD plays an integral role in the typically developing (TD) child s development and adjustment (Petalas et al., 2012). These results point towards a need for sibling support and an increased understanding of the complex factors involved. TD siblings of children with ASD exhibit higher stress levels than either TD siblings of TD children or children with Down Syndrome (McHale, Sloan, & Simeonsson, 1986). High stress levels can have a negative effect on general health and functioning, putting these siblings at risk for these outcomes. As compared to these same groups, siblings of children with ASD have less social interaction (Kaminsky & Dewey, 2001). If siblings of children with ASD are also living with many other demographic risk factors, including low socioeconomic status, they are at risk for negative

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 5 psychosocial and emotional adjustment as compared to children of a TD sibling (Macks & Reeve, 2007). Understanding the effects that children with ASD have on their siblings is a critical step in the study of any other sibling outcomes. One factor involved in sibling relationship quality within a TD-ASD dyad is the Broad Autism Phenotype (BAP). The BAP contains subtle personality and language characteristics that resemble those of ASD, in a milder but quantitatively similar way in non-autistic relatives of individuals with ASD (Hurley et al., 2007; Petalas et al., 2012). These characteristics can generally go undetected, presenting themselves as idiosyncrasies in specific domains. These areas include aloof personality, rigid personality, and pragmatic language deficits (Hurley et al., 2007). For example, individuals who exhibit BAP characteristics often display untactful behavior and have trouble adapting to changes in the environment (Losh et al., 2008). The three subsets of functioning mirror those of the ASD diagnosis: social deficits, stereotyped-repetitive behaviors, and communication deficits. Research indicates that 10-20% of TD siblings of children with autism exhibit BAP characteristics (Bolton, MacDonald, Pickles & Rios, 1994). The BAP has been examined through both a genetic and environmental lens, as it is believed that a diagnosis of autism includes both genetic and environmental etiological components. TD siblings of children with ASD can be influenced both by sharing genes and sharing environments with a child with autism. TD siblings of multiplex families, ones containing more than one child with autism, have significant differences in conversation and expressiveness than TD siblings of simplex families (Gerdts et al., 2013). As the amount of shared genetics increases, the more BAP traits are exhibited. Many researchers assert that these component factors, genetics and environment, cannot be separated, and the influence should be looked at as a whole. To explain the differences found in ASDlike traits between simplex and multiplex families, either the genetic cause of ASD or the shared

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 6 environmental effects affect behavioral presentation (Gerdts et al., 2013). At present, there is no way to determine the relative impact of these possibilities; therefore, the study of the BAP must be conducted with reference to an integration of genetics and environment. One theory of genetic-environment interaction in the BAP operates using the diathesis-stress model, where genetic predisposition interacts with environmental stress to produce a specific outcome (Bauminger & Yirmiya, 2001). Because TD siblings have similar genetics to their sibling with autism coupled with environmental events that exacerbate genetic predisposition lead to the presentation of more BAP traits. The amount of these ASD-like traits exhibited by TD siblings of children with autism can affect sibling functioning. One very important area of development that can be affected by having a sibling with ASD is pragmatic language use. Pragmatics refers to the use of language in communication, namely in social settings. This skill is one of the three areas of impairment for individuals with autism. It is also one of the three areas of comparison when examining the BAP using the Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire (BAPQ) (Hurley et al., 2007). In fact, pragmatics seems to be the only linguistic ability impaired by the presence of BAP characteristics (Ben-Yizhak et al., 2011). A person with BAP characteristics and no co-existing language deficits would have a normal control of the inner workings of their language, such as syntactic structure, but may have difficulties using their language to communicate with others socially. Research indicates that 25% of TD siblings of children with autism had a history of delayed language development, increasing to 75% if their sibling also had mental retardation (Chuthapisith et al., 2007). These results clearly indicate that genetic predisposition related specifically to IQ can influence TD siblings pragmatic language abilities. Having a sibling with ASD can also impact the way a TD sibling feels about his or her own

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 7 abilities. In order to assess children s feelings about themselves over a variety of contexts, one can measure perceived competence. Perceived competence in school-aged children refers to how they believe they function in domains such as academic, social, and physical competence (Harter, 1982). Children usually do not feel equally competent in each of these domains, so breaking up the broad concept of perceived competence into discrete modules can capture this competence discrepancy. One must note that self-competence, perceived competence, and self-concept are interchangeable terms, which are vague and unclear in the literature, as they are not concretely defined. For the purposes of this study, self-competence is the term given to refer to children s evaluation of their abilities, although other studies may refer to this same term as perceived competence or self-concept. There are mixed results associated with studying the self-competence of TD siblings of children with autism. Some results assert that there is no difference in self-competence between TD siblings of children with ASD, Down Syndrome, or TD children (Rodrigue, Geffken, & Morgan, 1993). Other studies find that adolescent TD female siblings of children with high-functioning autism have more positive-self concept than TD siblings of TD children (Verte et al., 2003). This disparity could be because of the variance in definition of the variable of interest and the measures used. Another variable noted for its effect on siblings is birth order. Birth order effects found for TD siblings of children with autism can be used as evidence for environmental impact in susceptibility to autism (Spiker et al., 2001). If ASD could be explained by a purely genetic mechanism, then birth order would not have much of an effect on outcomes on the expression of BAP characteristics. Outcomes tend to be more positive when the TD sibling is older than the child with autism. Older TD siblings report lower conflict scores and describe their siblings with ASD more favorably than do younger TD siblings of a child with autism (Petalas et al., 2012; Pilowsky et al., 2004). Younger TD siblings exhibit poorer pragmatic language performance and worse social scores than do older TD

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 8 siblings of a child with autism (Ben-Yizhak et al., 2011; Reichenberg et al., 2007). Younger TD siblings of a child with autism also exhibit higher externalizing behaviors, outward behavior problems, when their sibling with ASD has had externalizing behaviors (Tomeny et al., 2014). These results point towards environmental context due to birth order as a factor in development of a TD sibling of a child with autism, one that favors older TD siblings. This trend may be because the older TD sibling generally spends ample time with parents, allowing for proper scaffolding of developmental skills, such as self-regulation and sociability. However, a younger TD sibling of a child with autism may have to sacrifice this time, as parents tend to be very busy caring for their older child with ASD. This could shift the younger TD sibling s role to one of an additional caretaker, which could have consequences for that sibling s development. This point is especially true for younger siblings looking for a role model from which to adapt behavior. In order to incorporate the aforementioned variables into a cohesive model of sibling functioning, two research questions were examined: What are the effects of birth order on the pragmatic language use in siblings of children with ASD? Based on existing research, the hypothesis states that younger TD siblings of children with ASD will have higher BAPQ pragmatic language subset scores, indicating lower pragmatic function. The second question asks, does the pragmatic language use as indicated by score on the subset of the BAPQ predict perceived self-competence scores? Is birth order a moderator of this effect? Prior research leads to the hypothesis that higher pragmatic language scores on the BAPQ will be correlated with lower perceived self-competence scores, with the effect being stronger for younger TD siblings of a child with autism. Method Participants

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 9 Thirty-two parents and TD siblings of a child with autism completed this study. Six parent/sibling pairs participated in the pilot study and twenty-six parent/sibling pairs participated in the formal data collection. The data collected from the pilot study was not used for analysis because the surveys were modified according to their feedback before the surveys were sent out to the North Carolina Autism Registry via email. Eighteen reported on a female TD sibling, while eight reported on a male TD sibling. Five reported on a female ASD sibling, while twenty-one reported on a male ASD sibling. This reflects the general autism population gender distribution, the 4:1 ratio of boys to girls diagnosed. In ten out of the twenty-six cases, the TD sibling gender matched that of the ASD sibling gender. Eleven reported on older TD siblings, while twelve reported on younger TD siblings, and three parents reported on twins. Participants were recruited through the North Carolina Autism Registry and were sent emails inviting those who have typically developing children age 10-14 years with a fullblooded sibling with autism with no dual-diagnoses to visit the study website and complete the surveys. Measures The Pragmatic Language Subset of the Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire (BAPQ), along with a section based on top-down construct validity of additional BAPQ items (Pragmatics+), comprises the pragmatic language measure. The survey includes the informant-report version of the BAPQ for the parents of the TD siblings. There are 17 items, with 5 of those being in Pragmatics+. Pragmatics+ items are those items that were categorized within other constructs in the BAPQ, but were deemed part of a pragmatic construct by the PI of this study. Items are designed to assess the degree of deficits in social aspects of language (Hurley et al., 2007). For example, one item in the Pragmatic Language Subset of the informant-version reads, My child seems disconnected or out of sync in conversations with others. This item in particular targets the difficulties in social

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 10 communication exhibited with individuals with BAP characteristics. Cronbach s α for the Pragmatic Language Subset is 0.85, inferring high inter-item reliability (Hurley et al., 2007). These items are rated according to how frequently statements like the one above describe their child. This 6-point scale measures responses from 1-very rarely, 2- rarely, 3-occasionally, 4- somewhat often, 5-often, and 6- very often. Several items were reverse coded (see Appendix A). The Self-Description Questionnaire (SDQ) is a multidimensional measure intended to elicit information about a child s self-concept (Marsh et al., 1981). Seven separate factors are included within this 70 question measure, including Relationships with Peers, Relationships with Parents, Physical Abilities, and All School Subjects. These four factors are used both individually and compositionally for analysis. Separate factor analyses were similar for each construct and demonstrated the areas the SDQ is designed to measure, showcasing high construct validity. The reliability of each factor is also in the 0.8-0.9 range (Marsh et al., 1981). The Relations with Peers construct includes items such as, Other kids want me to be their friend, while the All School Subjects construct includes items like, I look forward to all school subjects. These items are scored on a scale of False, Mostly False, Sometimes False, Sometimes True, Mostly True, and True. There are 36 items in this edited construct (see Appendix B). Procedures The North Carolina Autism Registry recruited 2,361 families who received informational emails regarding the purpose of the study and the link to the study website. They were instructed via the email to visit the website if they have a TD child between 10 and 14 years of age. Once they entered the website and read the information on the top of the screen, they clicked the parent portal. They were immediately directed to an informed consent text for both themselves and their TD child.

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 11 Once completing this consent form, they were immediately directed to a brief demographic survey, including items about gender of the siblings, age of the TD sibling, and the birth order of the siblings. Next, they were directed to the Pragmatic Language survey. When the parents completed this survey, they were directed to the SDQ informant-version. When the parents finished their surveys, the TD children were asked to complete an assent form and were then directed to the self-report version of the SDQ. Results Hypothesis Testing In order to test the hypothesis that younger TD siblings have higher pragmatic language scores on the BAPQ, indicating poorer pragmatic function, we ran a single sample t-test that compared birth order status on both the Pragmatic Language Subset of the BAPQ and Pragmatics+ variables. Older TD siblings did not report differences in Pragmatic Language Subset scores from younger TD siblings, t(21) = -0.61, p = 0.547. Similarly there were no reported differences between birth order groups on the Pragmatics+ measure, t(21) = -0.44, p = 0.664. A simple correlation tested the hypothesis that pragmatic language scores predicted selfcompetence (i.e., lower pragmatic functioning predicted lower self-competence) score. This correlation included tests on both the Pragmatic Language Subset of the BAPQ and Pragmatics+ with a composite self-competence score and all four individual constructs from the SDQ from both the parent and sibling reports. The results of this test are listed in Table 1. The correlations between the Pragmatic Language Score and overall self-competence, the Relationships with Peers construct, and the Relationships with Parents construct were significant for the parent report of the SDQ. These correlations were significant with Pragmatics+ as well, with the addition of a significant correlation between Pragmatics+ and the Physical Abilities construct in the parent report.

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 12 Exploratory Analysis In order to examine the claims by Verte et al. (2003) that adolescent TD female siblings of children with high-functioning autism have more positive self concept than TD siblings of TD children, we ran single sample t-tests comparing TD gender and self-competence outcomes to look at gender outcomes in this clinical sample. Female TD siblings did not report higher overall self competence, t(23) = 0.45, p = 0.65, or pragmatic language functioning on either the Pragmatic Language Subset of the BAPQ, t(24) = 0.26, p = 0.80, or Pragmatics+, t(24) = 0.36, p = 0.724 than did male TD siblings. To test the effect of shared gender between siblings on self-competence and pragmatic language use, we ran single sample t-tests to examine whether the fact that siblings are the same gender has an effect on the outcome variables. There was no effect on either the parent report of selfcompetence, t(24) = 0.83, p = 0.412, or the child report, t(24) = 0.36, p = 0.719. Nor was there an effect of same gender on the Pragmatic Language Subset of the BAPQ, t(24) = 0.12, p = 0.902, or Pragmatics+, t(24) = 0.57, p = 0.573. To examine any effect that age of the TD sibling would have on the outcome measures of selfcompetence and pragmatic language use, we ran additional correlations. There was a moderately strong positive correlation between age and overall self-competence on the parents measure, r(26) = 0.3096, p = 0.124 and weak negative correlations between age and self-competence on the child measure, r(25) = -0.085, p = 0.686, the Pragmatic Language Subset of the BAPQ, r(26) = -0.163, p = 0.428, and Pragmatics+, r(26) = -0.135, p = 0.510. In order to interpret the parent and child self-competence measure as one entity, we ran correlations of the overall measures and each of the constructs for the parent and child reports. The results of this test are found in Table 2. The overall measures were significantly highly positively

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 13 correlated, r(25) = 0.646, p = 0.0005. Discussion Birth Order and BAPQ There were no differences between younger and older TD siblings on either pragmatic language measure, failing to confirm the hypothesis that younger TD siblings of children with autism would have lower pragmatic language abilities. These findings are consistent with those found by Spiker et al. (2011), in which there was no birth order effects on social impairment. However Spiker et al. (2011) found birth order effects for language functioning. If the results from this study are analyzed within this context, perhaps the Pragmatic Language Subscale from the BAPQ targets social functioning more than language functioning, which can explain the pattern of results. However, the results found in this study are not consistent with those found by Reichenberg et al. (2007). In their study assessing the effect of birth order on ASD domains, they found that 1 st born ASD siblings had higher useful phrase speech and better social scores than 2 nd born ASD siblings. The results from their study taken concurrently with the present study suggest that it is not a matter of measures, but a matter of methodology. Both Spiker et al. (2001) and Reichenberg et al. (2007) focused on autism multiplex families, rather than families with one child with autism and one TD child. This must be taken into account when interpreting the results of this study, as birth order effects may change based on diagnosis or lack thereof. Self-Competence and BAPQ There exist statistically significant correlations between both pragmatic language scales and parent reports of overall, Relationships with Peers, and Relationships with Parents self-competence. These results support the hypothesis that lower pragmatic language abilities, exhibited by higher pragmatic language subset scores, correlate with lower self-competence scores. This phenomenon is not exhibited by TD siblings of TD children in this study, and therefore, cannot

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 14 be generalized to results found by Rodrigue, Geffken, and Morgan (1993). However, the implications of this study s findings can inform interested parties concerning a TD sibling s well being. For instance, if they have trouble communicating socially, this behavior can be seen as an indicator of low self-competence. Limitations Although there are practical implications from the results of this study, limitations were present. As stated previously, self-competence is a vague term in the literature. This can lead to confusion regarding the generalizability of results. Also, the SDQ is a thirty-year-old measure, which can lead to some pragmatic difficulties interpreting the questions. For instance, one parent contacted the study because she thought the wording my child is dumb in all school subjects must have been a mistake. A more recent measure could bypass these issues to create a more streamlined participant experience. Another constraint on the strength of this study s findings is the small sample size. Although the study website link was sent to 2,361 families, only twenty-six parent/child pairs completed the survey. Because of this, there is a lesser ability to generalize findings to the siblings of the broader clinical population. Also, because only 10-20% of TD siblings of children with autism exhibit the BAP according to Bolton, MacDonald, Pickles & Rios (1994), the twenty-six child sample size would only include one or two siblings exhibiting BAP characteristics in pragmatic language. An increased sample size would ensure that a greater number of children exhibiting BAP characteristics would complete the measures. Future Directions Further research should investigate the birth order effect in a larger sample and replicate the self-competence findings in order to understand the support that siblings may need. A study examining multiplex autism families and simplex families with a TD sibling would be beneficial

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 15 to understand if the trends of one can be generalized to the other. The results of such a study could inform even more studies in the future about the BAP and TD siblings of children with autism, such that more appropriate hypotheses can be constructed and systematically tested. Implications Although there are limitations on the generalizability of this study s findings, there is still a takeaway: although there may not be birth order differences among TD siblings of children with autism, there should be self-competence support available for those who exhibit pragmatic language difficulties. This support could take the form of sibling-inclusive therapies in order to give the sibling a feeling of sovereignty. It could also take the form of sibling support groups, in which TD siblings of children with autism meet with one another to discuss their struggles and triumphs with their siblings. These groups could also serve another purpose: fostering social skills between these siblings. More socially adept siblings can scaffold those who may have pragmatic language difficulties, thereby increasing their social language use practices and possibly the correlated variable of self-competence.

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 16 Table 1 Summary of Correlations between Pragmatic Language Use and Self-Competence Pragmatic Language Subset Pragmatics+ r df β r df β Overall PSDQ -0.495 26 0.01* -0.539 26 0.005** Peer Relations PSDQ -0.42 26 0.03* -0.504 26 0.009** Parent Relations PSDQ -0.391 26 0.05* -0.397 26 0.04* School Subjects PSDQ -0.283 26 0.16-0.236 26 0.247 Physical Ability PSDQ -0.366 26 0.07-0.409 26 0.04* Overall CSDQ -0.201 25 0.34-0.221 25 0.29 Peer Relations CSDQ 0.054 25 0.8-0.025 25 0.91 Parent Relations CSDQ -0.109 25 0.6-0.128 25 0.54 School Subjects CSDQ -0.275 25 0.18-0.229 25 0.27 Physical Ability CSDQ -0.122 25 0.56-0.152 25 0.47 *p 0.05 ** p 0.01 *** p 0.001 **** p 0.0001

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 17 Table 2 Summary of Correlations between Child and Parent Measures of Self-Competence Overall PSDQ Peer Relations PSDQ Parent Relations PSDQ r df β r df β r df β Overall CSDQ 0.646 25 0.0005*** 0.308 25 0.134 0.552 25 0.001*** Peer Relations CSDQ 0.349 25 0.088 0.325 25 0.112 0.187 25 0.37 Parent Relations CSDQ 0.429 25 0.033* 0.174 25 0.404 0.725 25 <.0001**** School Subjects CSDQ 0.603 25 0.001*** 0.094 25 0.655 0.448 25 0.025* Physical Ability CSDQ 0.446 25 0.025* 0.346 25 0.09 0.112 25 0.594 School Subjects PDSQ Physical Ability PDSQ r df β r df β Overall CSDQ 0.603 25 0.001* 0.496 25 0.012* Peer Relations CSDQ 0.252 25 0.225 0.358 25 0.079 Parent Relations CSDQ 0.342 25 0.095 0.156 25 0.458 School Subjects CSDQ 0.922 25 <0.0001* 0.265 25 0.201 Physical Ability CSDQ 0.112 25 0.594 0.660 25 0.0003*** *p 0.05 ** p 0.01 *** p 0.001 **** p 0.0001

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 18 Appendix A: Pragmatic Language Survey 1-Very Rarely 2-Rarely 3-Occasionally 4- Somewhat Often 5- Often 6- Very Often 1. My child finds it hard to get his/her words out smoothly. 1 2 3 4 5 6 2. It is hard for my child to avoid getting sidetracked in the conversation. 1 2 3 4 5 6 3. My child would rather talk to people to get information than to socialize. 1 2 3 4 5 6 4. My child is in tune with other people during conversation.* 1 2 3 4 5 6 5. My child s voice has a flat or monotone sound to it. 1 2 3 4 5 6 6. My child seems disconnected or out of sync in conversations with others.* 1 2 3 4 5 6 7. People ask my child to repeat things he/she said because they don t understand. 1 2 3 4 5 6 8. My child talks too much about certain topics. 1 2 3 4 5 6 9. When my child makes conversation, it is just to be polite.* 1 2 3 4 5 6 10. My child speaks too loudly or too softly. 1 2 3 4 5 6 11. My child can tell when someone is not interested in what he/she is saying.* 1 2 3 4 5 6 12. My child is good at making small talk.* 1 2 3 4 5 6 13. Conversation seems to bore my child.* 1 2 3 4 5 6 14. My child leaves long pauses in conversations. 1 2 3 4 5 6 15. My child tends to lose track in his/her original point when talking to people. 1 2 3 4 5 6 16. My child can tell when it is time to change topics in conversation.* 1 2 3 4 5 6

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 19 17. My child enjoys chatting with people.* 1 2 3 4 5 6 Bolded responses comprise items on the BAPQ not included in the Pragmatic Language Subscale (Pragmatics+) *casual interactions with acquaintances Reverse scoring: 4, 11, 12, 16, 17

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 20 Appendix B: Self-Competence Survey- Parent Edition 1-False 2-Mostly False 3-Sometimes False 4- Sometimes True 5- Mostly True 6- True 1. My child is good at throwing a ball. 1 2 3 4 5 6 2. My child is interested in all school subjects. 1 2 3 4 5 6 3. My child likes all school subjects. 1 2 3 4 5 6 4. My child gets along with other kids easily. 1 2 3 4 5 6 5. Most other kids like my child. 1 2 3 4 5 6 6. My child and I have a lot of fun together. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7. My child likes me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 8. My child enjoys sports and games. 1 2 3 4 5 6 9. My child learns things quickly in all school subjects. 1 2 3 4 5 6 10. My child is dumb in all school subjects. 1 2 3 4 5 6 11. My child hates all school subjects. 1 2 3 4 5 6 12. My child and I spend a lot of time together. 1 2 3 4 5 6 13. My child is good at aiming at targets. 1 2 3 4 5 6 14. If my child has children of their own my child would want to bring them up like I raised him/her. 1 2 3 4 5 6 15. I like my child. 1 2 3 4 5 6 16. My child has lots of friends. 1 2 3 4 5 6 17. My child finds me easy to talk to. 1 2 3 4 5 6 18. My child is easy to like. 1 2 3 4 5 6 19. My child is popular with kids of his/her own age. 1 2 3 4 5 6

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 21 20. Work in all school subjects is easy for my child. 1 2 3 4 5 6 21. My child is good at all school subjects. 1 2 3 4 5 6 22. I understand my child. 1 2 3 4 5 6 23. My child likes to run and play hard. 1 2 3 4 5 6 24. My child enjoys doing work for all school subjects. 1 2 3 4 5 6 25. My child can run fast. 1 2 3 4 5 6 26. My child is a good athlete. 1 2 3 4 5 6 27. Other kids want my child to be their friend. 1 2 3 4 5 6 28. Most kids have more friends than my child does. 1 2 3 4 5 6 29. My child is good at sports. 1 2 3 4 5 6 30. My child makes friends easily. 1 2 3 4 5 6 31. My child looks forward to all school subjects. 1 2 3 4 5 6 32. My child gets along well with me. 1 2 3 4 5 6 33. My child gets good marks in all school subjects. 1 2 3 4 5 6 34. My child s body is strong and powerful. 1 2 3 4 5 6 Reverse Scoring: 10, 11, 28 Constructs Relationships with Peers 1. My child makes friends easily 2. My child gets along with other kids easily 3. My child has lots of friends 4. Other kids want my child to be their friend 5. Most other kids like my child 6. My child is popular with kids of his/her own age 7. My child is easy to like 8. Most kids have more friends than my child does

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 22 Relationship with Parents 1. I get along well with my child 2. My child and I have a lot of fun together 3. My child finds me easy to talk to 4. I like my child 5. My child and I spend a lot of time together 6. If my child has children of their own my child would want to bring them up like I raised him/her 7. I understand my child 8. My child likes me School Subjects 1. My child likes all school subjects 2. My child looks forward to all school subjects 3. My child is interested in all school subjects 4. My child enjoys doing work for all school subjects 5. *My child hates all school subjects 6. My child learns things quickly in all school subjects 7. My child gets good marks in all school subjects 8. Work in all school subjects is easy for my child 9. My child is good at all school subjects 10. My child is dumb in all school subjects Physical Abilities 1. My child is good at sports 2. My child is a good athlete 3. My child likes to run and play hard 4. My child enjoys sports and games 5. My child can run fast 6. My child is good at throwing a ball 7. My child is good at aiming at targets 8. My child s body is strong and powerful

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 23 References Bauminger, N., & Yirmiya, N. (2001). The functioning and well-being of siblings of children with autism: Behavioral-genetic and familial contributions. The development of autism: Perspectives from theory and research, 61-80. Ben-Yizhak, N., Yirmiya, N., Seidman, I., Alon, R., Lord, C., & Sigman, M. (2011). Pragmatic language and school related linguistic abilities in siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 41, 750-760. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-1096-6 Bolton, P., Macdonald, H., Pickles, A., Rios, P., Goode, S., Crowson, M.,... & Rutter, M. (1994). A case control family history study of autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35(5), 877-900. Chuthapisith, J., Ruangdaraganon, N., Sombuntham, T., & Roongpraiwan, R. (2007). Language development among the siblings of children with autistic spectrum disorder. Autism, 11(2), 149-160. doi: 10.1177/1362361307075706 Gerdts, J.A., Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Estes, A. (2013). The Broader Autism Phenotype in simplex and multiplex families. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43, 1597-1605. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1706-6 Harter, S., (1982). The Perceived Competence Scale for Children. Child Development, 53(1), 87-97. Hurley, R.S.E., Losh, M., Parlier, M., Reznick, J.S., & Piven, J. (2007). The Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1679-1690. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0299-3 Kaminsky, L., & Dewey, D. (2001). Siblings relationships of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 399-410. Marsh, H. & others. (1981). Self-concept: The Construct Validity of the Self Description Questionnaire. Educational Resources Information Center. Macks, R.J., & Reeve, R.E. (2007). The adjustment of non-disabled siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1060-1067. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0249-0 McHale, S. M., Sloan, J., & Simeonsson, R. J. (1986). Sibling relationships or children with autistic, mentally retarded, and nonhandicapped brothers and sisters. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16(4), 399-413. McHale, S. M., Updegraff, K. A., & Whiteman, S. D. (2012). Sibling relationships. In G. W. Peterson & K. R. Bush (Eds.), Handbook of marriage and family (3rd ed.). 329-351. New York: Springer. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-3987-5_15 Losh, M., Childress, D., Lam, K., & Piven, J. (2008). Defining key features of the broad autism

BROAD AUTISM PHENOTYPE 24 phenotype. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 147B(4). 424-433. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30612. Petalas, M.A., Hastings, R.P., Nash, S., Hall, L.M., Joannidi, H., & Dowey, A. (2012). Psychological adjustment and sibling relationships in siblings of children with Autism Spectrum Disorders: Environmental stressors and the Board Autism Phenotype. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 546-555. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.07.015 Pilowsky, T., Yirmiya, N. Doppelt, O., Gross-Tsur, V., & Shalev, R.S. (2004). Social and emotional adjustment of siblings of children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(4), 855-865. Reichenberg, A., Smith, C., Schmeidler, J., & Silverman, J.M. (2007). Birth order effects on autism symptom domains. Psychiatry Research, 150, 199-204. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2004.09.012 Rodrigue, J.R., Geffken, G.R., & Morgan, S.B. (1993). Perceived competence and behavioral adjustment of siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 23(4), 665-674. Spiker, D., Lotspeich, L.J., Dimiceli, S., Szatmari, P., Myers, R.M., & Risch, N. (2001). Birth order effects on nonverbal IQ scores in autism multiplex families. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(5), 449-460. Tomeny, T.S., Barry, T.D., & Bader, S.H. (2014). Birth order rank as a moderator of the relation between behavior problems among children with an autism spectrum disorder and their siblings. Autism, 18(2), 199-202. doi: 10.1177/1362361312458185 Verté, S., Roeyers, H., & Buysse, A. (2003). Behavioural problems, social competence, and selfconcept in siblings of children with autism. Child: Care, Health, & Development, 29(3), 193-205.