Human sex differences in social and non-social looking preferences, at 12 months of age

Similar documents
in 18- and 24-month-old infants

Cognitive styles sex the brain, compete neurally, and quantify deficits in autism

Foetal testosterone and autistic traits

Prenatal versus postnatal sex steroid hormone effects on autistic traits in children at 18 to 24 months of age

Exploring essential skills of CCTV operators: the role of sensitivity to nonverbal cues

Melissa Heydon M.Cl.Sc. (Speech-Language Pathology) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Joanna Bailes M.Cl.Sc (SLP) Candidate University of Western Ontario: School of Communication Sciences and Disorders

Peer Perception in Autism. Kathryn McVicar, MD Assistant Professor Clinical Pediatrics and Neurology Albert Einstien College of Medicine

Fetal testosterone and sex differences

= add definition here. Definition Slide

High-Functioning Autism & Asperger Syndrome in Girls. Prevalence of ASD. Gender Gap. Late Diagnosis

Research Proposal 2D:4D and Perceived Aggression: a Human Subjects Study Maya Frost-Belansky. Introduction

Supporting Online Material for

Fukuoka University of Education

Edinburgh Research Explorer

Enhanced Visual Search for a Conjunctive Target in Autism: A Research Note

Sex differences in emotion recognition by observing body movements: A case of American students

Verbal Operants as Predictors for Children with Autism in Inclusive Settings

The DSM-IV-TR diagnoses autism as having the symptoms of. qualitative impairment in social interaction, qualitative impairments in

Can Asperger Syndrome Be Diagnosed at 26 Months Old? A Genetic High-Risk Single-Case Study

Understanding emotions from standardized facial expressions in autism and normal development

Valarie Kerschen M.D.

Using pantomime to teach discrimination of social cues to a youth with Aspergers Syndrome

The effect of little bird program in decreasing problem behaviors of autistic children

The Impact of Autism on Child Development

Fetal testosterone and empathy

The Effect of Robotic Intervention on the Imitation Skills of a Child with Autism

SHORT REPORT The Development of Perceptual Expertise for Faces and Objects in Autism Spectrum Conditions

40

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Enhanced discrimination in autism

Pearson Edexcel Level 3 GCE Psychology Advanced Paper 2: Applications of psychology

Temporal Context and the Recognition of Emotion from Facial Expression

What is Autism? -Those with the most severe disability need a lot of help with their daily lives whereas those that are least affected may not.

Massachusetts Institute of Technology and University of Cambridge Sir Henry Wellcome postdoctoral fellow

Online Courses for Parents and Professionals Who Want to Know More About Children who are Deaf or Hard of Hearing

Sound Localization PSY 310 Greg Francis. Lecture 31. Audition

Kayla Ortiz November 27, 2018

USING VIDEO MODELING AND REINFORCEMENT TO TEACH PERSPECTIVE-TAKING SKILLS TO CHILDREN WITH AUTISM LINDA A. LEBLANC AND ANDREA M.

The Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in Japan: A Cross-Cultural Comparison

Developmental Psychology

Empathizing and systemizing cognitive traits in the sciences and humanities

Statistical Reasoning in Public Health Biostatistics 612, 2009, HW#3

Stability or Change?

Prof. Greg Francis 7/8/08

",! " ", " " " " "!!! " $.

Investigation of Genomic Imprinting in the X- linked Transketolase-like Protein 1 (TKTL1) in Human and the Implications for Autism

Autism/Autism Spectrum Disorders

The Effects of Social Reward on Reinforcement Learning. Anila D Mello. Georgetown University

1/30/2018. Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Disclosures. Learning Objectives

Categorical states of Autism by using ADCL (Autism Detecting Check-List) in Dhaka

AUTISM SPECTRUM DISORDER: DSM-5 DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA. Lisa Joseph, Ph.D.

Empathy, estradiol and androgen levels in 9-year-old children

ECI WEBINAR SERIES: PRACTICAL STRATEGIES FOR WORKING WITH CHILDREN WITH AUTISM. Kathleen McConnell Fad, Ph.D.

The short forms of the Empathy Quotient (EQ-short) and the Systemizing Quotient (SQ-short)

The Onset Of Autism During Infancy Insights From Sibling Studies

Language Outcomes for Preverbal Toddlers with Autism

The effect of social stories on reduction of challenging behaviours in autistic children

Laura Welsh, B.S. & Tina K. Veale, Ph.D. Eastern Illinois University

Autism Spectrum Disorder. Aims for Session 3 03/04/2018. Neurodevelopmental Disorders Professor Pamela Heaton. Session 3

Enhanced Discrimination of Novel, Highly Similar Stimuli by Adults with Autism During a Perceptual Learning Task

Investigating a Robot as a Therapy Partner for Children with. Autism

An exploration of autistic children s ability to recognise emotions in single word and sentence language conditions

Psychiatric problems common in siblings of people with autism

Include Autism Presents: The Volunteer Handbook

Brief Report: Prompted Pretend Play in Autism1

Fetal testosterone and empathy: Evidence from the Empathy Quotient (EQ) and the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test

Innovative Teaching Symposium

Hearing Loss and Autism. diagnosis and intervention

8/23/2017. Chapter 21 Autism Spectrum Disorders. Introduction. Diagnostic Categories within the Autism Spectrum

Adaptive Behavior Profiles in Autism Spectrum Disorders

Introductory Workshop. Research What is Autism? Core Deficits in Behaviour. National Autistic Society Study - UK (Barnard, et. al, 2001).

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Early Hum Dev. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2009 December 1.

Vision and Action. 10/3/12 Percep,on Ac,on 1

Sibling studies reveal early signs of autism

Session 2: Sensory Development

AUTISM: THEORY OF MIND. Mary ET Boyle, Ph.D. Department of Cognitive Science UCSD

Infants Differential Processing of Female and Male Faces Jennifer L. Ramsey-Rennels 1 and Judith H. Langlois 2

What is Autism? Laura Ferguson, M.Ed., BCBA.

Language is the behavior of the brain; communication is the behavior of the body and brain. Financial Disclosure

Theory of Mind in ASD, SLI, & Hearing Impairment Groups

Digit ratio (2D:4D) and prenatal/perinatal sex hormones: A response to Manning and Fink (2017)

Clinical case scenarios for professionals working with children and young people

Red flag signs for Autism

The Development of Theory of Mind in Early Childhood*

Neurodevelopmental Disorders

OVERVIEW OF PRESENTATION

ENGAGEMENT IN ROLE PLAY, PRETENSE, AND ACTING CLASSES PREDICT ADVANCED THEORY OF MIND SKILL IN MIDDLE CHILDHOOD*

ASSAF HAREL, PHD CURRICULUM VITAE Department of Psychology Wright State University

Overview. Basic concepts Theories of emotion Universality of emotions Brain basis of emotions Applied research: microexpressions

Social Competence in Children with Learning and Autism Spectrum Disorders

Critical Review: Using Video Modelling to Teach Verbal Social Communication Skills to Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder

AUTISM SCREENING AND DIAGNOSIS PEARLS FOR PEDIATRICS. Catherine Riley, MD Developmental Behavioral Pediatrician

Gender Differences in Nonverbal, Interpersonal Sensitivity. Across Three Cultures: Japan, India, and the United States

Agenda. Making the Connection. Facts about ASD. Respite Presentation. Agenda. Facts about ASD. Triad of Impairments. 3 Diagnoses on spectrum

A Critique of Thurber, Sheehan, & Valtinson s (2007) Appliance of a Piagetian Framework in Analyzing a Subject with Autism. Frederick Herrmann

Visual Processing in Autism: From Perception to Cognition

CO ADDM 2008 ASD Prevalence Results 4/26/2012. Describe what autism is. Describe the Autism and Developmental. and its methodology

Autism. Laura Schreibman HDP1 11/29/07 MAIN DIAGNOSTIC FEATURES OF AUTISTIC DISORDER. Deficits in social attachment and behavior

CURRICULUM VITAE Michelle A. Aldridge, Ph.D., C.C.C Inwood Rd. 214/ (home)

Transcription:

Infant Behavior & Development 25 (2002) 319 325 Short communication Human sex differences in social and non-social looking preferences, at 12 months of age Svetlana Lutchmaya, Simon Baron-Cohen Autism Research Centre, Departments of Experimental Psychology and Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Downing Site, Cambridge CB2 3EB, UK Received 5 July 2001; received in revised form 30 November 2001; accepted 1 January 2002 Abstract Twelve-month-old infants (n = 60) were presented with a video of cars moving, or a face moving, in a looking preference experimental design. This tested the prediction from our earlier work that attention in males is drawn more to mechanical motion, whilst attention in females is drawn more to biological motion. Results supported this prediction. These findings are discussed in relation to social and biological determinism. 2002 Published by Elsevier Science Inc. Sociality seems to be sexually dimorphic in humans. The evidence for this comes from several sources: (1) At birth, boys look longer than girls at a mobile, whilst girls look longer than boys at a face (Connellan, Baron-Cohen, Wheelwright, Ba tki, & Ahluwalia, 2001). (2) At age 12 and 24 months, girls make more eye contact with their mother than boys do, and this amount of eye contact is inversely correlated with level of prenatal testosterone (Lutchmaya, Baron-Cohen, & Raggett, 2002). (3) At age 4, girls pass tests of theory of mind earlier than boys do (Happe, 1995). (4) At ages 7 12, boys lag behind girls by as much as 2 years in terms of the development of social sensitivity to faux pas (Baron-Cohen, O Riordan, Jones, Stone, & Plaisted, 1999a). (5) In adulthood, women score higher on emotion recognition tasks (Baron-Cohen, Jolliffe, Mortimore, & Robertson, 1997; Hall, 1978; Rosenthal, Hall, DiMatteo, Rogers, & Archer, 1979). (6) Both girls and women have different styles of social interaction, conversation, and degree of intimacy, compared to boys and men (Geary, 1998; Maccoby, 1966). Corresponding author. Tel.: +44-1223-333-557; fax: +44-1223-333-564. E-mail address: sb205@cam.ac.uk (S. Baron-Cohen). 0163-6383/02/$ see front matter 2002 Published by Elsevier Science Inc. PII: S0163-6383(02)00095-4

320 S. Lutchmaya, S. Baron-Cohen / Infant Behavior & Development 25 (2002) 319 325 Whilst many of these sex differences in sociality may to some extent be the result of socialisation, the first two of the results above (differences at birth, and the correlation with prenatal testosterone) strongly suggest biological (including genetic) factors play a role. The idea that biology may play some part in sex differences in sociality gathers further support from the findings that medical conditions such as autism and Turner s Syndrome affect sociality (Baron-Cohen, Ring, et al., 1999; Skuse et al., 1997). The present paper reports an experiment that tested if there are sex differences in looking preference for social or non-social stimuli at 12 months of age. Looking preference is widely used in studies of infant development (Fantz, 1963). The stimuli employed were social and non-social video clips (two of each). The social video clips showed human faces. The first clip was of a man and a woman talking in a social context. The second clip consisted of a head and shoulders view of a man reading out loud from a child s storybook. The non-social video clips showed mechanical objects. The first clip was footage from a motor racing event, consisting of cars travelling at high speed. The second clip was of a single stationary vehicle with its windscreen wipers switched on. So within each category (social and non-social) there was one clip containing a single target stimulus and one clip containing a number of target stimuli. The videos had no sound, to avoid introducing this as a potentially confounding variable. Parents were instructed not to direct their child s attention during the testing session. Subjects were 60, 12-month-old infants (33 boys and 27 girls). None of the subjects had younger siblings. The age of the infants on the day of testing ranged from 357 to 431 days (M = 387.43 days, SD = 18.33). There was no difference in age, or on any of the background variables listed in Table 1, between the sexes. Infants were shown the four silent video clips in sequence, which were each 2 min in length. The video clips were presented in a random order, alternating between social and non-social stimuli. The parents were asked not to guide the child s attention, but to react to the child as they normally would if approached. The child was free to move around the testing room, and the parent was asked to encourage them to stay in the field of view of the videocamera. The layout of the laboratory is shown in Appendix A. The amount of time that the subject spent looking at each video clip was measured using a stopwatch. Looking preference was expressed as the looking preference ratio (LPR) the ratio of time spent looking at the social stimuli to time spent looking at the non-social stimuli. The greater the value of the ratio, the stronger the preference for the social stimuli. LPR values Table 1 Descriptive data for boys and girls together Both sexes (n = 60) n a Range M SD Number of siblings 57 0 3 1.1 1.0 Maternal age at child s birth 60 24.0 43.0 35.5 4.5 Paternal age at child s birth 52 28.0 53.0 37.8 5.7 Level of education attained by parents 51 4.0 10.0 6.6 1.6 a Refers to the number of subjects for whom the data was available.

S. Lutchmaya, S. Baron-Cohen / Infant Behavior & Development 25 (2002) 319 325 321 Fig. 1. The distribution of LPR scores for each sex. can be thought of as lying on a continuum, from strong social looking preference to strong non-social looking preference. A value of 1 means no preference (i.e., equal looking times for both types of stimulus) and lies in the middle of the continuum. Results indicated that girls had a significantly higher LPR than boys. The mean LPR for girls was greater than 1 (indicating a mean social preference) and the mean LPR for boys was less than 1 (indicating a mean non-social preference). Of the independent variables listed in Table 1, sex was the best predictor of LPR. The mean LPR for girls (M girls = 1.86, SD = 2.27) was significantly higher than that for boys (M boys = 0.89, SD = 0.1) (t = 3.10, df = 58, p <.001). 1 Looking times were re-measured for 10% of subjects in order to generate a test retest reliability statistic. The first and second sets of recorded looking times were highly significantly correlated (r =.99, p<.001). A Mann Witney U-test correlating LPR and sex revealed U = 248.5, p =.003. LPR for each sex separately, and showing the overlap between the sexes, is shown in Figs. 1 and 2. Seventy-nine percent of boys and 37% of girls had LPR scores below 0.9 (non-social preference), and 18% of boys and 56% of girls had LPR scores above 1.1 (social preference). Three percent of boys and 7% of girls had LPR scores between 0.9 and 1.1 (neutral preference). This neutral category was considered here because, if the value of LPR was close to 1 (i.e., between 0.9 and 1.1) the subject could not be said to show a strong preference in either direction. Although most of the boys showed a non-social preference, only about half of the girls showed a social preference. This suggests that the boys as a group showed a strong non-social preference, whilst the girls as a group did not show such a strong preference in either direction. The following factors were then considered: number of siblings; maternal age; paternal age; sex; and parental education level. A backward stepwise linear regression method (entry criteria,

322 S. Lutchmaya, S. Baron-Cohen / Infant Behavior & Development 25 (2002) 319 325 Fig. 2. Graph to show the distribution of LPR scores for each sex. p =.05, removal criteria, p =.10) was used to find the best fit for the dependence of LPR. All factors were discarded from the model apart from sex. No unusual data points were identified by regression diagnostic techniques. The residuals were approximately normally distributed and there was no systematic relationship between the residuals and predicted values. The findings Table 2 Correlation matrix showing relationships between the dependent variable (LPR) and the independent variables (except for sex) Spearman s ρ Looking preference ratio (LPR) Number of siblings Correlation coefficient.06 Significance (2-tailed).63 n 57 Maternal age Correlation coefficient.13 Significance (2-tailed).33 n 60 Paternal age Correlation coefficient.13 Significance (2-tailed).36 n 52 Parental education level Correlation coefficient.11 Significant (2-tailed).46 n 51

S. Lutchmaya, S. Baron-Cohen / Infant Behavior & Development 25 (2002) 319 325 323 Table 3 Regression model Model B SE Significance Constant 1.10.33.001 Sex.67.21.001 Dependent variable: ln(lpr); r 2 =.146; F = 9.75, p<.001; power is equal to approximately 0.82. are not explained by boys and girls spending different amounts of time looking at the television (regardless of stimulus) as there was no sex difference in total looking time (Mann Whitney U = 416.50, p =.67). In summary, this model revealed that sex was a significant predictor of LPR at 12 months of age (such that girls scored higher than boys). Other social factors were not found to have a significant effect on LPR over and above the effect of sex (see Tables 2 and 3). Allocation of subjects to groups according to whether they showed a social, non-social or neutral looking preference suggested that boys as a group showed a strong preference for the non-social stimuli, whilst the girls showed a less strong preference for either type of stimulus. This observation is consistent with findings that boys develop a preference for sex typical toys sooner than girls do (Servin, Bohlin, & Berlin, 1999). If the observed sex difference in looking preference is due to social influences such as parenting, these must operate very early. An alternative explanation is that these results in part reflect biological differences in neural development, influencing how attention is deployed by the male and female brain. The present results replicate the pattern seen in our experiment of one-day old neonates (Connellan et al., 2001), in whom socialisation clearly plays no significant role. If boys attend less to faces and more to systems, and if girls show the opposite pattern, partly for biological reasons, this may have some relevance for understanding the neurodevelopmental, genetic condition of autism, where the male pattern of looking preference is seen to an extreme degree (Baron-Cohen, 1999; Swettenham et al., 1998). Note 1. The LPR data was transformed using a natural logarithm before the t-test was carried out. Acknowledgments This work was submitted in part fulfilment of the degree of Ph.D. by the first author, to the University of Cambridge. We are grateful to the MRC for support during the period of this work, through a studentship to S.L. and a Program Grant to S.B.C.

324 S. Lutchmaya, S. Baron-Cohen / Infant Behavior & Development 25 (2002) 319 325 Appendix A. Layout of testing room used for infants at 12 months of age References Baron-Cohen, S. (1999). The extreme male-brain theory of autism. In H. Tager-Flusberg (Ed.), Neurodevelopmental disorders. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Baron-Cohen, S., Jolliffe, T., Mortimore, C., & Robertson, M. (1997). Another advanced test of theory of mind: Evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 813 822. Baron-Cohen, S., O Riordan, M., Jones, R., Stone, V., & Plaisted, K. (1999a). A new test of social sensitivity: Detection of faux pas in normal children and children with Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 407 418. Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Wheelwright, S., Bullmore, E., Brammer, M., Simmons, A., & Williams, S. (1999). Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: An fmri study. European Journal of Neuroscience, 11, 1891 1898. Connellan, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., Ba tki, A., & Ahluwalia, J. (2001). Sex differences in human neonatal social perception. Infant Behavior and Development, 23, 113 118. Fantz, R. (1963). Pattern vision in newborn infants. Science, 140, 296 297. Geary, D. C. (1998). Male, female: The evolution of human sex differences. Washington DC: American Psychological Association. Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin, 85, 845 858. Happe, F. (1995). The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Development, 66, 843 855.

S. Lutchmaya, S. Baron-Cohen / Infant Behavior & Development 25 (2002) 319 325 325 Lutchmaya, S., Baron-Cohen, S., & Raggett, P. (2002). Foetal testosterone and eye contact at 12 months. Infant Behavior and Development, 25, 327 335. Maccoby, E. E. (1966). The development of sex differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press. Rosenthal, R., Hall, J. A., DiMatteo, M. R., Rogers, P. L., & Archer, D. (1979). Sensitivity to nonverbal communication: The PONS test. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Servin, A., Bohlin, G., & Berlin, L. (1999). Sex differences in 1-, 3-, and 5-year-olds toy-choice in a structured play-session. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 40, 43 48. Skuse, D. H., James, R. S., Bisop, D. V. M., Coppin, B., Dalton, P., Aamodt-Leeper, G., Bacarese-Hamilton, M., Cresswell, C., McGurk, R., & Jacobs, P. A. (1997). Evidence from Turner s Syndrome of an imprinted X-linked locus affecting cognitive function. Nature, 387, 705 708. Swettenham, J., Baron-Cohen, S., Charman, T., Cox, A., Baird, G., Drew, A., Rees, L., & Wheelwright, S. (1998). The frequency and distribution of spontaneous attention shifts between social and non-social stimuli in autistic, typically developing, and non-autistic developmentally delayed infants. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 9, 747 753.