Beyond cognitivism. Loughborough University Institutional Repository

Similar documents
Post-cognitive psychology

Making psychology relevant

Open Research Online The Open University s repository of research publications and other research outputs

Psychology, sociology and interaction: disciplinary allegiance or analytic quality? - a response to Housley and Fitzgerald

Cognition as context (whose cognition?)

A model of discourse in action

How to study experience

The longer you ve been here I think the more they get moulded : Identity regulation, identification and resistance

Child Mental Health: A Review of the Scientific Discourse

Discursive psychology

School of Nursing, University of British Columbia Vancouver, British Columbia, Canada

THEORIES OF EPISTEMOLOGICAL BELIEFS AND COMMUNICATION: A UNIFYING ATTEMPT

J. Discursive social psychology: from attitudes to evaluative practices.

Thinking the environment aurally An enactive approach to auditory-architectural research and design

Introduction: the evolution of discursive psychology: from classic to contemporary themes

PSYCHOLOGICAL CONSCIOUSNESS AND PHENOMENAL CONSCIOUSNESS. Overview

Principles of Sociology

Choose an approach for your research problem

Justifying the use of a living theory methodology in the creation of your living educational theory. Responding to Cresswell.

Psy2005: Applied Research Methods & Ethics in Psychology. Week 14: An Introduction to Qualitative Research

HELPLESSNESS IN DEPRESSION: THE UNBEARABLE RIDDLE OF THE OTHER

Theorizing Interviews within Qualitative Research

Qualitative Research Theoretical Orientations. ScWk 240 Week 10 Slides

What is analytical sociology? And is it the future of sociology?

Qualitative Research Methods in Marketing Types of Qualitative Research. Sammy Toyoki Assistant Professor Ph.D. Consumer Research

Discursive Psychology

Crossing boundaries between disciplines: A perspective on Basil Bernstein s legacy

Comments on David Rosenthal s Consciousness, Content, and Metacognitive Judgments

Attitudes and evaluative. food assessments

5.8 Departure from cognitivism: dynamical systems

Discursive Psychology

PALPABLE EXISTENTIALISM: AN INTERVIEW WITH EUGENE GENDLIN

P H E N O M E N O L O G Y

Research Methodology in Social Sciences. by Dr. Rina Astini

CALL FOR PAPERS: THE 8th MIDTERM CONFERENCE ON EMOTIONS, EDINBURGH, 2018

INTERVIEWS II: THEORIES AND TECHNIQUES 5. CLINICAL APPROACH TO INTERVIEWING PART 1

Environmental Psychology in Context 1. Special Issue of the Journal of Environmental Psychology. Edited by David Uzzell and Gabriel Moser

Instructor s Test Bank. Social Research Methods

Theory and Methods Question Bank

Erasmus & Visiting Students: Modules & Assessments

The essence of professional vision

Audio: In this lecture we are going to address psychology as a science. Slide #2

Ideas RESEARCH. Theory, Design Practice. Turning INTO. Barbara Fawcett. Rosalie Pockett

Qualitative interviews in psychology: problems and possibilities

Research Ethics and Philosophies

Subject, object and practice: Three fundamental concepts in Kurt Danziger's work. Adrian C. Brock. University College Dublin

Conclusion. The international conflicts related to identity issues are a contemporary concern of societies

Cognitive & Linguistic Sciences. What is cognitive science anyway? Why is it interdisciplinary? Why do we need to learn about information processors?

The Significance of Mixed Methods Research in Information Systems Research

A critical look at the use of SEM in international business research

A review of conversational practices in psychotherapy. Vehviläinen, Sanna. Cambridge University Press 2008

INTRODUCTION TO SYMBOLIC INTERACTION: GEORGE HERBERT MEAD PART ONE: THEORETICAL APPROACH OF SYMBOLIC INTERACTIONISM

Critical review (Newsletter for Center for Qualitative Methodology) concerning:

VISUAL ANALYSIS AS A QUALITATIVE RESEARCH CONCEPT

Narrative and Psychology Symposium Proposal, BPS Centenary Conference, Glasgow, March/April 2001

Intentional Horizons

24.09 Minds and Machines Fall 11 HASS-D CI

Chapter 4: Qualitative data analysis

Commentary On Mossio and Taraborelli: is the enactive approach really

Positive Psychologists on Positive Psychology: Alex Linley

Process in broad terms. Observation and Participant Observation. Some concepts. Researchers role (Raymond Gold) Before field phase

Advances in Environmental Biology

The four chapters in Part I set the stage. Chapter 1 moves from the implicit common sense theories of everyday life to explicit theories that are

Psychology Syllabus. First Year. General Neuropsychology. Workload: 128 hs (64 per semester) Lectures / Laboratory practical classes

Explaining an Explanatory Gap Gilbert Harman Princeton University

Behavioral and Brain Sciences Personal narratives as the highest level of cognitive integration

The Thesis Writing Process and Literature Review

VIEWS AND EXPERIENCES OF DEAF TEACHERS ON INCLUSIVE EDUCATION AND DISABILITY

Behaviorism: An essential survival tool for practitioners in autism

Eliminative materialism

THE RESEARCH PROPOSAL

Damasio s error. Aaron Sloman. (In The Philosophy Magazine 2004)

FORUM: QUALITATIVE SOCIAL RESEARCH SOZIALFORSCHUNG

Ian Owen Neil Morris The Husserlian Phenomenology of Consciousness and Cognitive Science: We Can See the Path But Nobody Is On It

Participant as Ally - Essentialist Portraiture. Methodology/philosophy or research approach ORIGINAL PARADIGM

AGENT-BASED SYSTEMS. What is an agent? ROBOTICS AND AUTONOMOUS SYSTEMS. Today. that environment in order to meet its delegated objectives.

*** ACCEPTED FOR PUBLICATION IN QUALITATIVE HEALTH RESEARCH, JULY PLEASE DO NOT QUOTE FROM THE PAPER WITHOUT PERMISSION OF THE AUTHOR***

The Faith of the Psychiatrist

Methods on the Move: Q methodology. Professor Paul Stenner Open University

Grounded Theory s Contested Family of Methods: Historical and Contemporary Applications

Qualitative and Quantitative Approaches Workshop. Comm 151i San Jose State U Dr. T.M. Coopman Okay for non-commercial use with attribution

CONSTELLATIONS AND LVT

Discourse and Identity. Saskia Nolte & Miriam Brandel

What role should heroes, saints and sages play within moral theory? While it would be unfair to

Developing a more empirical approach to culture, attitude and motivation in construction management research: a critique and a proposal

The scope of perceptual content, II: properties

CONCEPTUAL FRAMEWORK, EPISTEMOLOGY, PARADIGM, &THEORETICAL FRAMEWORK

Our previous accounts of perceptual experience accepted the phenomenal principle:

PHENOMENAL CONSCIUOSNESS: QUALIA & QUINNING QUALIA. Prof. Rajakishore Nath, Department of Humanities & Social Science, IIT Bombay

What Constitutes a Good Contribution to the Literature (Body of Knowledge)?

A Brief Discussion and Application of Interpretative Phenomenological Analysis in the Field of Health Science and Public Health

Whose psychological concepts?

A Level Sociology. A Resource-Based Learning Approach

Reduce Tension by Making the Desired Choice Easier

COUNSELING INTERVIEW GUIDELINES

Effective Intentions: The Power of Conscious Will

Emotional & social Skills for trainers

Perceptual Imagination and Perceptual Memory

COMP329 Robotics and Autonomous Systems Lecture 15: Agents and Intentions. Dr Terry R. Payne Department of Computer Science

PARADIGMS, THEORY AND SOCIAL RESEARCH

Transcription:

Loughborough University Institutional Repository Beyond cognitivism This item was submitted to Loughborough University's Institutional Repository by the/an author. Citation: POTTER, J., 1999. Beyond cognitivism. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 32 (1-2), pp. 119-127 Additional Information: This article was published in the journal, Research on Language and Social Interaction [ c Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc]. The definitive version is available at: http://www.tandfonline.com/doi/abs/10.1080/08351813.1999.9683615 Metadata Record: https://dspace.lboro.ac.uk/2134/9435 Version: Accepted for publication Publisher: c Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. Please cite the published version.

This item was submitted to Loughborough s Institutional Repository (https://dspace.lboro.ac.uk/) by the author and is made available under the following Creative Commons Licence conditions. For the full text of this licence, please go to: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/2.5/

BEYOND COGNITIVISM JONATHAN POTTER Discourse and Rhetoric Group Email: J.A.Potter@lboro.ac.uk Department of Social Sciences Tel: 01509 223384 Loughborough University Fax: 01509 223944 Loughborough Leicestershire, LE11 3TU I would like to thank Charles Antaki, Derek Edwards, Alexa Hepburn and Celia Kitzinger for comments on a previous draft of this paper. For: Language and Social Interaction Study at the Century's Turn, special issue of Research on Language and Social Interaction 1 March, 2012

1 What is the appropriate relationship between the fields of language and social interaction (LSI) research and cognitive psychology? I wish to speculate about one possible future where LSI hijacks much of the action which would have been considered the province of cognitive psychology, respecifying and reorienting it as it does so. In this future LSI becomes a foundational discipline in the social sciences rather than existing merely to service one of a wide array of different topic areas. It is a future of ambition and creative argument. LSI might need a name change, though. TWO MODELS OF LSI Let me start by distinguishing between two extreme models of LSI and its relationship to other areas of social science. On the one hand, there is a model in which social life is an array of intersecting modules and levels and the different social sciences sub-fields study these different modules and levels to improve understanding. They provide complementary analyses of topics such as semantics, registers, cognitive representations and ideology which can be articulated together to contribute to a larger picture. Here the field is defined by topic rather than by method or theory. This model of LSI is presented in both the introduction and the organization of Teun van Dijk's (1997) (excellent) volumes on discourse studies. I will call this the Complementary Model of LSI. On the other hand, there is a model of the relationship between LSI and other areas of social science which, instead of offering complementary analyses, proposes instead a wholesale respecification of topics, methods and questions. Ethnomethodological conversation analysis is one perspective in LSI which is characterised by the respecification of a wide range of social science notions; post-structuralism offers another perspective calling for a root and branch disciplinary overhaul. For example, in conversation analysis, the

2 attempt is not to relate institutions, as prior existing and clearly identifiable phenomena, with the more ephemeral waxing and waning of talk; rather institutional realities are treated as constituted in talk in a variety of ways as participants construct and orient to institutional goals and identities (Drew & Sorjonen, 1997; Heritage 1997). I will call this the Respecification Model of LSI. Quite apart from any theoretical and analytic grounds for preferring one model over another, the Respecification Model has the virtue of promoting potentially productive debate across the social sciences. So far, this model has had its most powerful effects in sociology, where ethnomethodological conversation analysis has developed a radically different approach to issues of social structure. However, there has, as yet, been relatively little argument at the intersection of LSI and cognitive psychology. DISCOURSE AND COGNITION The boundary of LSI and cognitive psychology has been colonised by the thriving research tradition of discourse processes ( discourse signalled linguistic issues that go beyond the sentence; processes suggested that these issues might be dealt with by cognitive processes of one kind or another). The Complementary Model is at work here to sustain a seamless transition between cognitive psychology and LSI research with the boundary area covered by ROLSI s sister journal Discourse Processes, by the sorts of social cognition work (in speech accommodation and so on) published in the Journal of Language and Social Psychology, and by the various books in the Advances in Discourse Processes series. In the Respecification Model things are more complicated. There has been a tradition of Wittgenstein influenced critique of cognitivism in ethnomethodology for some time (Button, et al., 1995; Coulter, 1990). Although much of this was a conceptual reworking of

3 Wittgensteinian notions in the light of ethnomethodological thinking, it also spawned Lucy Suchman s influential work. Suchman challenged the traditional cognitive psychology of goals and plans by highlighting the interactional uses of plans when using and maintaining a photocopier plans served a normative role in justifying conduct but did not explain, let alone causally constrain, the sorts of ad hoc situated practices involved in running the copier (Suchman, 1987). Conversation analysts have had an ambivalent response to cognition. Some have expressed considerable caution about taking cognitivist notions such as strategies at face value (Heritage, 1990/1), while others have argued that some kind of cognitive notions are inescapable in conversation analytic work, so it is important to deal with them clearly and explicitly (Pomerantz, 1990/1). This ambivalence is picked up in analytic practice. Some conversation analytic research has reworked cognitivist notions in terms of conversational routines. An example of this is Emanuel Schegloff s (1992) transformation of the notion of intersubjectivity from a phenomenological merging of consciousness into a set of interaction procedures involving the management of repair. Other research has taken cognitivist notions as an unproblematic start point for research. For example, Paul Drew (1995) seems happy to do business with the idea of anticipatory interactive planning. Why has there been this difference in approach to social structure and cognition? It may be because conversation analysis has its origins and much of its disciplinary base in sociology. Conversation analysts have rethought social structure as they have been forced to deal with sociology s dominant conceptions of society and to counter mainsteam attacks on their legitimacy. In contrast, conversation analysts have found cognitive psychology to be a rather distant discipline, and a discipline, moreover, which has almost entirely ignored it. When cognitive psychologists have wanted to understand language and interaction they have almost invariably looked to the sorts of pragmatic work characteristic of the discourse

4 processes tradition (although Clark, 1992, is a rare exception). There is something of the reverse pattern where a respecification model of discourse analysis developed psychology. DISCOURSE ANALYSIS AND DISCURSIVE PSYCHOLOGY Traditionally LSI work in social psychology was cognitivist, and strongly influenced by what, in North America anyway, became the dominant social cognition tradition by the start of the 1990s. In Britain in particular a type of discourse analysis emerged in social psychology under the influence of a mixture of conversation analysis, post-structuralism and rhetoric (Billig, 1987; Potter and Wetherell, 1987). This was very much within the Respecification Model of LSI, fundamentally transforming the social cognitivist assumptions of social psychology. Consider the example of attitudes. Traditional social psychologists theorized attitudes as underlying mental constructs which influence behaviour and can be used to index how one individual differs from another. Discourse researchers have criticised this notion on both analytic and conceptual grounds, moving the focus on to people s heterogeneous practices of evaluation in particular settings (Potter, 1998). The notion of practice is crucial here the concern is what is being done with evaluations rather than their role as evidence of putative underlying explanatory entities. Over the past few years this kind of respecifying critique has been developed beyond the sub discipline of social psychology, and in 1997 Derek Edwards published a major work which drew out the implications of themes in the sociology of scientific knowledge and Harvey Sacks thinking for the prevailing cognitivist view of psychology (Edwards, 1997; see also, Edwards, 1995). Edwards offers a sweeping reappraisal of the central thesaurus of cognitivist notions, including shared knowledge, scripts, categories, memory, emotion and

5 animal and child cognition. Its vision is no less than a full scale discursive psychology. For example, he takes the cognitivist construction of shared knowledge and shows the virtue analytically and theoretically of respecifying it in terms of practical, situated accomplishments in interaction; that is, of pragmatic intersubjectivity. And he highlights the confused state of traditional research which has made judgements about shared knowledge from a range of more or less technical arenas of interaction without addressing the pragmatics of intersubjectivity. Reworking cognitive psychology in this way has a range of methodological implications. The most notable is that is provides a principled, theoretically grounded reason for addressing topics that were previously dealt with in exclusively cognitivist terms by focusing on action. Rather than concentrating on putative inner entities and processes that might be occurring within an actor abstracted from action and interaction, the interest is in how cognitive notions are constructed, managed and oriented to in action. Discursive psychologists ask: what does the notion of memory do in some interaction? How is a version of the past constructed to sustain some action? The overwhelming practice of cognitive psychology involves abstracting the person from interaction as far as possible. In discursive psychology, interaction becomes a, perhaps the, fundamental site for studying (a respecified) cognition. Put another way, rather than LSI being a rather marginal subfield of cognitive psychology, cognitive psychology becomes a subfield of LSI. COGNITION, REALITY AND CONSTRUCTIONISM One of the phenomena highlighted in discursive psychological research is the close interrelationship between constructions of cognitive entities of various kinds and constructions of the world (see Edwards, 1997; Edwards & Potter, 1992; Potter, 1996).

6 Versions of inner states can be part of establishing consequential facts about the world. Thus, characterising someone as sceptical may cast their version of the world as cautious but potentially believable; characterising a person as jealous can suggest distortions in his account of his relationship. Conversely, particular descriptions of external reality can be part of established consequential facts about inner states. Describing a woman as flirting can sustain a very different motivational story to just messin. I am making two points here. First, it is possible and fruitful to study the business done by descriptions as they appear in various kinds of interaction. Second, when we consider practices of talk, the boundary between what is cognitive and what is not is particularly permeable. LSI research need not start from the assumption that there must be consequential differences between descriptions of events, social institutions, or cognitive entities. The territorial boundaries of cognition, social structure, action and so on reflect a history of disciplinary colonisation. We have the opportunity to tear up the old maps. At the same time, there has been a flood of work in the last decade that dubs itself constructionist. Often what is being suggested by the construction metaphor is not clear what precisely is constructed, and how? As I have argued at length (Potter, 1996), constructionism can be given a coherent focus by considering how descriptions are constructed, how they are made factual and what sorts of practices they are involved with. Constructionism, too, could be given an LSI makeover. LANGUAGE AND SOCIAL INTERACTION AFTER THE CENTURY S TURN I have tried to outline a vision of the future of LSI which makes it a central, perhaps foundational, discipline in the social sciences. I have tried to promote the respecification model, and push it to extreme. In particular, I have wanted to show how a discursive

7 psychology can build on existing developments to provide a productive alternative to traditional cognitivist cognitive psychology. At the same time, it can provide a coherent focus for the heterogeneous and theoretically fragmented work of constructionism. This vision is an ambitious one, and there is a huge amount of disciplinary inertia in the way of its realization. However, merely having the arguments could generate rich new thinking. What sort of work would realize this vision? In methodological terms, it would primarily consist in studies of records (audio and video) of natural interaction, where natural means that the interaction was not got up by the researcher in one way or another. That means that it would be overwhelmingly non-experimental (not just because of the well established problems in the critiques of positivism, but because of their patent failure of deal with the reflexive and indexical nature of interaction), and would generally avoid the sorts of interview studies that have been popular in constructionist research (because of their slide into cognitivism, their peculiar interactional status, and their imposition of social science agendas on participants see Widdicombe and Wooffitt, 1995). It would probably look most similar to the burgeoning CA based work on talk and institutions (Drew and Heritage, 1992), although here the primary focus would not be on how institutions are constituted and oriented to, but on how mind and reality are simultaneously managed in particular settings. There is already high quality work starting to paint in the detail of this vision. Take two psychological topics: counselling and perception. Annsi Peräkylä (1995) has studied the production of HIV+ counselling as a practice and an institution, while at the same time considering how the participants construct their worlds and their mental life. Derek Edwards (1995) considered the way couples in relationship counselling produce versions of their own, and their partner s, problems in a way that managed issues of blame and therapeutic outcome. These studies are exemplary, and also illustrate the convergence of work in conversation analysis and discursive psychology. If counselling is an obvious focus for LSI work beyond

8 cognitivism, the topic of perception is something of a hard case. It is not easy to think of a less obviously discursive topic. Nevertheless, in a series of major studies Charles and Marjorie Goodwin (Goodwin, 1995, 1997; Goodwin & Goodwin, 1996, 1997) have shown how perception can be treated not as an inner psychological phenomenon, but as a feature of situated practices where airline workers, chemists and oceanographers have to see particular planes, color changes in reactions, or features of the ocean floor. These studies start to show how that perception is produced in interaction. Research on Language and Social Interaction is a journal that can be at the heart of these developments. My preference would be for its name not to be taken too seriously, though. Part of the task of the respecification model is to trouble both the notion of language as an abstract system, separable from practices, and the notion of social interaction, with its micro-sociology, leave the big stuff to the social theorists, overtones. A vibrant LSI should not be closed to issues of ideology (Billig, 1992) and social critique (Kitzinger and Frith, in press) and new ways of addressing these issues have been developed. Luckily names don t have to be taken too seriously Dartmouth (in Devon, not Kansas or Nova Scotia!) still names the town successfully even though the mouth of the river is now some way away. REFERENCES Billig, M. (1987). Arguing and thinking: A rhetorical approach to social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Billig, M. (1992). Talking of the royal family. London: Routledge. Button, G., Coulter, J., Lee, J. and Sharrock, W. (1995). Computers, Minds and Conduct. Oxford: Polity. Clark, H.H. (1992). Arenas of language use. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

9 Coulter, J. (1990). Mind in action. Oxford: Polity. Drew, P. & Heritage, J.C. (Eds.) (1992). Talk at work: Interaction in institutional settings. Cambridge: University of Cambridge Press. Drew, P. & Sorjonen, M-L. (1997) Institutional dialogue. In van Dijk, T.A. (Ed.) Discourse as Interaction (Discourse Studies: A multidisciplinary introduction, volume 2) (pp 92-118). London; Sage. Drew, P. (1995). Interaction sequences and anticipatory interactive planning. In E. Goody (Ed.), The social origins of human intelligence. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Edwards, D. (1995a). Sacks and psychology, Theory and Psychology, 5, 579-596. Edwards, D. (1995b). Two to tango: Script formulations, dispositions, and rhetorical symmetry in relationship troubles talk, Research on Language and Social Interaction, 28, 319-350. Edwards, D. (1997). Discourse and cognition. London and Beverly Hills, CA: Sage. Edwards, D. & Potter, J. (1992). Discursive psychology. London: Sage. Goodwin, C. (1995) Seeing in depth, Social Studies of Science, 25, 237-74. Goodwin, C. (1997) The blackness of black: Color categories as situated practice. In Resnick, L.B., Säljö, R., Pontecorvo, C. & Burge, B. (Eds). Discourse, Tools, and Reasoning (pp. 111-140). Berlin; Springer. Goodwin, C. & Goodwin, M.H. (1996) Seeing as situated activity: Formulating planes. In Engeström, Y. & Middleton, D. (Eds) Cognition and communication at work (pp. 61-95). Cambridge; Cambridge University Press. Goodwin, C. & Goodwin, M.H. (1997) Contested vision: The discursive constitution of Rodney King. In Gunnarsson, B-L, Linell, P. & Nordberg, B. (Eds) The Construction of Professional Discourse (pp. 292-316). London; Longman.

10 Heritage, J.C. (1990/1). Intention, meaning and strategy: Observations on constraints in interaction analysis. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 24, 311-332. Heritage, J.C. (1997) Conversation analysis and institutional talk: Analysing data. In D. Silverman (Ed.) Qualitative research: Theory, method and practice (pp. 161-182) London; Sage. Kitzinger, C. and Frith, H. (in press) Just say no? Using conversation analysis to understand how young women talk about refusing sex, Discourse and Society. Peräkylä, A. (1995). AIDS counselling: Institutional interaction and clinical practice. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pomerantz, A. (1990/1991) Mental concepts in the analysis of social action, Research on Language and Social Interaction, 24, 299-310. Potter, J. & Wetherell, M. (1987). Discourse and social psychology: Beyond attitudes and behaviour. London: Sage. Potter, J. (1996). Representing reality: Discourse, rhetoric and social construction. London; Sage. Potter, J. (1998) Discursive social psychology: From attitudes to evaluations, European Review of Social Psychology, in press. Schegloff, E.A. (1992). Repair after next turn: The last structurally provided defence of intersubjectivity in conversation. American Journal of Sociology, 97 (5), 1295-1345. Suchman, L. (1987). Plans and situated actions: The problem of human-machine interaction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. van Dijk, T.A. (1997) Discourse as interaction in society. In T.A. van Dijk (Ed.) Discourse as Social Interaction (Vol. 2 of Discourse Studies: A multidisciplinary introduction). London; Sage.

11 Widdicombe, S. & Wooffitt, R. (1995). The language of youth subcultures: Social identity in action. Hemel Hempstead, UK: Harvester/Wheatsheaf.