Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons

Similar documents
Birds' Judgments of Number and Quantity

Avian detection and identification of perceptual organization in random noise

The Comparative Psychology of Avian Visual Cognition

Dynamic shifts of pigeon local/global attention

PSY 402. Theories of Learning Chapter 8 Stimulus Control How Stimuli Guide Instrumental Action

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Competing Frameworks in Perception

Perception. Chapter 8, Section 3

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Supporting Information

(Visual) Attention. October 3, PSY Visual Attention 1

This article appeared in a journal published by Elsevier. The attached copy is furnished to the author for internal non-commercial research and

Selective Attention. Inattentional blindness [demo] Cocktail party phenomenon William James definition

Musculoskeletal System. Terms. Origin (Proximal Attachment) Insertion (Distal Attachment)

Same/different discrimination learning with trial-unique stimuli

Replacing the frontal lobes? Having more time to think improve implicit perceptual categorization. A comment on Filoteo, Lauritzen & Maddox, 2010.

Chapter 5: Perceiving Objects and Scenes

Goodness of Pattern and Pattern Uncertainty 1

Journal of Comparative Psychology

Spatial context learning in Pigeons (Columba Livia)

Effects of Geon Deletion, Scrambling, and Movement on Picture Recognition in Pigeons

Perception of Angle in Visual Categorization by Pigeons (Columba livia)

CATEGORIZING A MOVING TARGET IN TERMS OF ITS SPEED, DIRECTION, OR BOTH WALTER T. HERBRANSON, THANE FREMOUW, AND CHARLES P. SHIMP

Cognition. Shape from shading in pigeons. Robert G. Cook, Muhammad A.J. Qadri, Art Kieres, Nicholas Commons-Miller. abstract

Hierarchical Bayesian Modeling of Individual Differences in Texture Discrimination

Chapter 8: Visual Imagery & Spatial Cognition

Pattern recognition in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): The role of the spatial organisation of stimulus parts

Salient Feature Coding in Response Selection. Circularity?

EFFECTS OF NOISY DISTRACTORS AND STIMULUS REDUNDANCY ON VISUAL SEARCH. Laurence D. Smith University of Maine

Memory Systems Interaction in the Pigeon: Working and Reference Memory

Testing the translational-symmetry hypothesis of abstract-concept learning in pigeons

Dynamics of Color Category Formation and Boundaries

Supplementary Materials

Stream/bounce perception and the effect of depth cues in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes)

Contrast and the justification of effort

Introduction to Computational Neuroscience

Training and generalization of complex auditory-visual conditional discriminations in individuals with autism: New procedures using dynamic stimuli.

Two-item same different concept learning in pigeons

THE ENCODING OF PARTS AND WHOLES

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

Key questions about attention

Mental Imagery. What is Imagery? What We Can Imagine 3/3/10. What is nature of the images? What is the nature of imagery for the other senses?

LEARNING-SET OUTCOME IN SECOND-ORDER CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS

A STUDY OF FUNCTIONAL EQUIVALENCE IN RATS USING CLASS-SPECIFIC REINFORCERS AND OLFACTORY STIMULI. Rebecca M. Rayburn-Reeves

Disparity- and velocity- based signals for 3D motion perception in human MT+ Bas Rokers, Lawrence K. Cormack, and Alexander C. Huk

9.65 Sept. 12, 2001 Object recognition HANDOUT with additions in Section IV.b for parts of lecture that were omitted.

Comparison of Grouping Abilities in Humans (Homo sapiens) and Baboons (Papio papio) With the Ebbinghaus Illusion

Publications Blough, D. S. Dark adaptation in the pigeon. Doctoral dissertation, Harvard University, Ratliff, F., & Blough, D. S.

How other kinds of animals think about the world we share is one of the most interesting and

Global and Local Processing in Adult Humans (Homo sapiens), 5-Year-Old Children (Homo sapiens), and Adult Cotton-Top Tamarins (Saguinus oedipus)

Jennifer J. McComas and Ellie C. Hartman. Angel Jimenez

Biologically-Inspired Human Motion Detection

Today: Visual perception, leading to higher-level vision: object recognition, word perception.

Attention shifts during matching-to-sample performance in pigeons

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Fleishman s Taxonomy of Human Abilities

Two Competing Attentional Mechanisms in Category Learning

The Simon Effect as a Function of Temporal Overlap between Relevant and Irrelevant

DOES STIMULUS COMPLEXITY AFFECT ACQUISITION OF CONDITIONAL DISCRIMINATIONS AND THE EMERGENCE OF DERIVED RELATIONS? Tiffani L. Martin, B.A., B.S.

Left Handed Split Brain. Learning Objectives Topics

1/20/2015. Maximizing Stimulus Control: Best Practice Guidelines for Receptive Language Instruction. Importance of Effective Teaching

Discrimination Trials to Influence Self-Awareness

Exemplar Memory and Discrimination

PSY 310: Sensory and Perceptual Processes 1

Recognizing Scenes by Simulating Implied Social Interaction Networks

DIFFERENTIAL OUTCOMES FACILITATE RELATIONAL ASSOCIATIONS

Interference from Irrelevant Features. on Visual Discrimination by Macaques (Macaca fuscata): A Behavioral Analogue of the Human Stroop Effect

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

ESTABLISHING FUNCTIONAL CLASSES IN A CHIMPANZEE (PAN TROGLODYTES) WITH A TWO-ITEM SEQUENTIAL-RESPONDING PROCEDURE MASAKI TOMONAGA

Oddity-from-Sample Abstract-Concept Learning by Pigeons. Thomas A. Daniel

Within-event learning contributes to value transfer in simultaneous instrumental discriminations by pigeons

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

A Model of Biological Motion Perception from Configural Form Cues

Attentional requirements in perceptual grouping depend on the processes involved in the organization

Effect of Visuo-Spatial Working Memory on Distance Estimation in Map Learning

Study of Imagery. Banned by behaviorists

Task Preparation and the Switch Cost: Characterizing Task Preparation through Stimulus Set Overlap, Transition Frequency and Task Strength

Transitive Inference and Commonly Coded Stimuli

The impact of item clustering on visual search: It all depends on the nature of the visual search

The Meaning of the Mask Matters

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance

A Memory Model for Decision Processes in Pigeons

Identify these objects

PAUL S. MATTSON AND LISA R. FOURNIER

Observation is the capacity of the individual to know the environment by the use of his senses. There are two steps in the process of observation:

SPATIAL UPDATING 1. Do Not Cross the Line: Heuristic Spatial Updating in Dynamic Scenes. Markus Huff. Department of Psychology, University of Tübingen

Sensation. I. Basic Concepts II. Characteristics of Sensory Systems III. The Visual System

June 16, The retina has two types of light-sensitive receptor cells (or photoreceptors) called rods and cones.

Framework for Comparative Research on Relational Information Displays

Perceptual Learning of Motion Direction Discrimination with Suppressed and Unsuppressed MT in Humans: An fmri Study

Instrumental Conditioning I

Cognitive Processes PSY 334. Chapter 2 Perception

THE PERCEPTION AND MEMORY OF STEREOSCOPIC DEPTH INFORMATION IN NATURALISTIC OBJECTS

The Analysis of Visual Cognition in Birds: Implications for Evolution, Mechanism, and Representation

The influence of visual motion on fast reaching movements to a stationary object

Psychology of visual perception C O M M U N I C A T I O N D E S I G N, A N I M A T E D I M A G E 2014/2015

Excerpt from LABORATORY MANUAL PRINCIPLES OF PSYCHOLOGY: EXPERIMENTAL FOUNDATIONS PSYCHOLOGY

Rules of apparent motion: The shortest-path constraint: objects will take the shortest path between flashed positions.

Cross-Trial Priming of Element Positions in Visual Pop-Out Search Is Dependent on Stimulus Arrangement

Transcription:

Entire Set of Printable Figures For Hierarchical Stimulus Processing by Pigeons Cook In one experiment, Cerella (1980) trained pigeons to discriminate intact drawings of Charlie Brown from normal drawings of other Peanuts characters. Then, Cerella reorganized Charlie Brown by altering the relations between the head, torso, and legs. He discovered that the pigeons responded to scrambled versions of Charlie Brown in the same manner as the original, intact drawings. Therefore, he concluded that the pigeon must be insensitive to global organizational properties of objects. Insensitivity to global object properties is one attribute of a particulate perceiver. Example of Color Texture Stimulus.

Example of Shape Texture Stimulus Example of Conjunctive Texture Stimulus.

Video clips can't be printed. Example of Two-Dimensional Feature Stimulus (Color) tested by Cook et al. (1996). Example of Two-Dimensional Feature Stimulus (Orientation) tested by Cook et al. (1996).

Example of Two-Dimensional Conjunctive Stimulus tested by Cook et al. (1996). Example of Three-Dimensional Feature Stimulus (Color) tested by Cook et al. (1996).

Example of Three-Dimensional Feature Stimulus (Orientation) tested by Cook et al. (1996). Example of Three-Dimensional Feature Stimulus (Size) tested by Cook et al. (1996).

Example of Three-Dimensional 1-Shared Conjunctive Stimulus tested by Cook et al. (1996). Example of Three-Dimensional 2-Shared Conjunctive Stimulus tested by Cook et al. (1996).

Comparison of RT measures for pigeons and humans when tested with different types of feature and conjunctive displays (Redrawn from Cook, Cavoto, & Cavoto, 1996).

Example of a Dot Density Display. Example of a Dot Spacing Display.

Acquisition results for four pigeons trained with dot texture displays varying in either dot density or dot. Effects of monitor distance on steady state discrimination of dot density displays. The red line is the mean performance for all four birds. The open symbols show performance for each of the four birds separately.

Animation can't be printed. Discrimination Acquisition with Hierarchical Stimuli and Size-Matched Controls (Cavoto & Cook, under review). Footnote 1. To interpret such a stimulus competition procedure, it is critical that the global and local information be equally discriminable. Besides the advantages outlined earlier, an additional benefit of training the pigeons with different sized stimuli was that it allowed us to select stimuli in which the discriminability (as reflected in choice accuracy) of each level was as similar as possible. Towards this end, all probe stimuli were created from sizes selected so that both levels supported equal levels of choice accuracy for a particular bird. For Birds #1H and #3N this was the medium stimulus size and for Birds #2R and #4B this was the small size, with the birds correctly discriminating the relevant letters at the global and local levels at 84% and 86.3% respectively in sessions collected just prior to the experiment. Besides the adjustment for size, this equivalence was bought about in part by the birds continued improvement with the global-relevant conditions during the time between Experiments 2 and 3 and an additional change made prior to Experiment 3. Regardless of the individual birds choice mapping, we found after Experiment 2 that the letters H and N were often confused, especially in the global-relevant condition. Because of this, we replaced the letter H with the block letter P as one of the relevant letters. For this and the remaining experiment, the relevant letters were now P, N, X, and T. After about 40 to 50 sessions in which the P replaced the H, mean accuracy with this letter across all stimulus conditions was 75.7%, and generally improved global-relevant performance relative to the other conditions. With these adjustments, we now felt in position to ask about how processing of the global and local level interacted with one another during the presentation of a single stimulus.

Relevant Letter Choices on Conflict Tests as a function of Presentation Time (Cavoto & Cook, 2001). Relevant Letter Choices on Conflict Tests as a function of Presentation Time for Individual Birds. Two birds (#1H & # 3N, the left two panels) consistently identified the local letter of the conflict stimuli more often across all stimulus durations, although this was reduced at the shortest durations. In contrast, the other two birds (#2R & #4B, the right two panels) showed systematic changes in the proportion of global and local choices as a function of stimulus duration. At the short durations, these birds reported the local letter most often, while at the longer durations the global letter was reported significantly more often. Thus, for two birds the local advantage was present across all durations, while the two other birds shifted to showing a global advantage at the longest duration (Cavoto & Cook, 2001).

Footnote 2. In a series of experiments examining two species of nonhuman primates, Fagot and his colleagues (Deruelle & Fagot, 1998, Fagot & Deruelle, 1997; Fagot & Tomonaga, 1999) have found that baboons and chimpanzees generally exhibit an effect opposite of humans, showing a local precedence effect in which higher accuracy or faster RTs are recorded with local relevant displays, especially with sparse element arrangements. Humans tested with the same stimuli as the primates revealed the standard global precedence effect. Figure 3. Pigeons viewed these complex stimuli with target stimuli at either the local or global level of perceptual analysis. See Figure 1 and related text in Fremouw, Herbranson, and Shimp (1998) for additional details.

Figure 10. Pigeons average reaction time to recognize a target at either a local or global level of perceptual analysis, as a function of which level was primed. See Figure 2 and related text in Fremouw, Herbranson, and Shimp (1998) for additional details. The two different axes of rotation tested in Cook & Katz (1999).

The effects of axis and rate of rotation on discrimination of cube and pyramid stimuli from Cook & Katz (1999). The red line indicates performance with Y-axis rotations of these stimuli. There was little effect of motion for this rotation, presumably because it added little new information to these unambiguous views of the stimuli. The green and blue lines show the effects of more complex motions that resulted in larger distortions in the appearance of the objects. In both of these two cases, there was a dynamic superiority effect for all three rates of rotation tested. Examples of the "kinetic depth" stimuli tested by Cook & Katz (1999). We found evidence that three of our four pigeons were better at discriminating the structure of these "objects" when they were in motion than when they were presented at random static angles. This suggest that kinematic factors helped to recover the shape of these ambiguous images.

Illustration of the trajectory and timing of the two motion pathways tested by Cook, Shaw, and Blaisdell (in press). The various objects used by Cook, Shaw and Blaisdell (in press) to used to create the two motion pathways. Video clips can't be printed.

Mean discrimination ratios for six transfer tests with novel objects for all five successfully-trained pigeons. The red bars show transfer performance and the blue bars show baseline performance. The first four tests for the two experienced involved reintroductions of the videos not discriminated during an earlier period of training. Mean discrimination ratios presented in two-session blocks for the baseline, coherent and randomized presentation conditions. The small T identifies non-reinforced transfer sessions. These results are from a our second test, in which only one fixed randomized sequence was tested, making it more comparable to the single coherent sequence tested. Only four birds were tested.