PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Report WE SAW IT ALL ALONG: VISUAL HINDSIGHT BIAS IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS

Similar documents
Object identification in preschool children and adults. Daniel M. Bernstein, Geoffrey R. Loftus and Andrew N. Meltzoff

The Saw-it-All-Along Effect: Demonstrations of Visual Hindsight Bias*

Does scene context always facilitate retrieval of visual object representations?

Is subjective shortening in human memory unique to time representations?

Leading Words and Estimation

Percent-produced effects in recognition 1. The production effect in recognition memory: Weakening strength can strengthen distinctiveness

Influence of Implicit Beliefs and Visual Working Memory on Label Use

The Curse of Knowledge in Reasoning about False Beliefs. Susan A. J. Birch. University of British Columbia. and. Paul Bloom.

Visual memory decay is deterministic

Hindsight Bias: A Cross-Cultural Analysis. STEVEN J. HEINE and DARRIN R. LEHMAN University of British Columbia, Canada

Do Adults Show a Curse of Knowledge in False-Belief Reasoning? A Robust Estimate of the True Effect Size

Fluency Misattribution and Auditory Hindsight Bias

WHAT CAN CHILDREN TELL US ABOUT HINDSIGHT BIAS: A FUNDAMENTAL CONSTRAINT ON PERSPECTIVE TAKING?

Scale Invariance and Primacy and Recency Effects in an Absolute Identification Task

Changing expectations about speed alters perceived motion direction

Grace Iarocci Ph.D., R. Psych., Associate Professor of Psychology Simon Fraser University

Conformity. Jennifer L. Flint. The University of Southern Mississippi

Empirical Knowledge: based on observations. Answer questions why, whom, how, and when.

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

How do people process information over the life span? Class Objectives. What is Information Processing? 3/22/2010. Chapter 7 Information Processing

Auditory hindsight bias: Fluency misattribution versus memory reconstruction

Recognition of Faces of Different Species: A Developmental Study Between 5 and 8 Years of Age

Fukuoka University of Education

Lecturer: Rob van der Willigen 11/9/08

Running Head: TRUST INACCURATE INFORMANTS 1. In the Absence of Conflicting Testimony Young Children Trust Inaccurate Informants

Viewpoint dependent recognition of familiar faces

How Many Colors Can You Remember? Capacity is about Conscious vs unconscious memories

Selective attention and asymmetry in the Müller-Lyer illusion

Analogy-Making in Children: The Importance of Processing Constraints

Aesthetic preferences in the size of images of real-world objects

Supplementary Materials

Natural Scene Statistics and Perception. W.S. Geisler

The Effects of Action on Perception. Andriana Tesoro. California State University, Long Beach

DO STIMULUS FREQUENCY EFFECTS OCCUR WITH LINE SCALES? 1. Gert Haubensak Justus Liebig University of Giessen

Minimizing Uncertainty in Property Casualty Loss Reserve Estimates Chris G. Gross, ACAS, MAAA

Reduce Tension by Making the Desired Choice Easier

What matters in the cued task-switching paradigm: Tasks or cues?

Culture Differences in an Inattentional Blindness Study

Egocentrism and Automatic Perspective Taking in Children and Adults

Insight Assessment Measuring Thinking Worldwide

Non-categorical approaches to property induction with uncertain categories

BACK TO THE FUTURE AND FORWARD TO THE PAST: EXPLORING RETROSPECTIVE BIASES AND CULTURE

Neuron, Volume 63 Spatial attention decorrelates intrinsic activity fluctuations in Macaque area V4.

Decomposing the Mean: Using Distributional Analyses to Provide a Detailed Description of Addition and Multiplication Latencies

Left Handed Split Brain. Learning Objectives Topics

Visual Memory Decay Is Deterministic Jason M. Gold, 1 Richard F. Murray, 2 Allison B. Sekuler, 3 Patrick J. Bennett, 3 and Robert Sekuler 4

Modeling Human Perception

What Matters in the Cued Task-Switching Paradigm: Tasks or Cues? Ulrich Mayr. University of Oregon

MODELLING CHARACTER LEGIBILITY

Source memory and the picture superiority effect

Types of questions. You need to know. Short question. Short question. Measurement Scale: Ordinal Scale

Do I know that you know what you know? Modeling testimony in causal inference

Why Does Similarity Correlate With Inductive Strength?

Spectrograms (revisited)

Unit 1 Exploring and Understanding Data

Implicit Information in Directionality of Verbal Probability Expressions

Congruency Effects with Dynamic Auditory Stimuli: Design Implications

Differences of Face and Object Recognition in Utilizing Early Visual Information

Cultural Differences in Cognitive Processing Style: Evidence from Eye Movements During Scene Processing

Information Processing During Transient Responses in the Crayfish Visual System

9 research designs likely for PSYC 2100

Don t Look at My Answer: Subjective Uncertainty Underlies Preschoolers Exclusion of Their Least Accurate Memories

Why do Psychologists Perform Research?

The Role of Introspection in Children's Developing Theory of Mind. Christopher Gonzales

Age-Related Deficits in Reality Monitoring of Action Memories

Information Processing Mid-February 2007

Individual differences in working memory capacity and divided attention in dichotic listening

Misleading Postevent Information and the Memory Impairment Hypothesis: Comment on Belli and Reply to Tversky and Tuchin

Professional Coach Training Session Evaluation #1

Highlighting Effect: The Function of Rebuttals in Written Argument

Analogical Representations. Symbolic Representations. Culture as Cognition. Abstract mental representations. Includes: 9/15/2012

Everyday Problem Solving and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living: Support for Domain Specificity

Reactions to blatantly contradictory information

Supporting Information

Short article Detecting objects is easier than categorizing them

Encoding of Elements and Relations of Object Arrangements by Young Children

School of Psychology. Professor Richard Kemp. Cognitive Bias: Psychology and Forensic Science

A contrast paradox in stereopsis, motion detection and vernier acuity

Is it possible to gain new knowledge by deduction?

Integrating episodic memories and prior knowledge at multiple levels of abstraction

Object Substitution Masking: When does Mask Preview work?

Psychology of visual perception C O M M U N I C A T I O N D E S I G N, A N I M A T E D I M A G E 2014/2015

Perceptual grouping determines the locus of attentional selection

Observational Category Learning as a Path to More Robust Generative Knowledge

Memory in Everyday Life. Lesson 5

Decisions based on verbal probabilities: Decision bias or decision by belief sampling?

PARAMETERS OF ECHOIC MEMORY

STAGES OF PROFESSIONAL DEVELOPMENT Developed by: Dr. Kathleen E. Allen

Lesson 9: Two Factor ANOVAS

Rapid fear detection relies on high spatial frequencies

Trading Directional Accuracy for Realism in a Virtual Auditory Display

Interference with spatial working memory: An eye movement is more than a shift of attention

PSYC 441 Cognitive Psychology II

Assessing the influence of recollection and familiarity in memory for own- vs. other-race faces

CHAPTER ONE CORRELATION

The Color of Similarity

Rajeev Raizada: Statement of research interests

TTI Personal Talent Skills Inventory Coaching Report

Still important ideas

Transcription:

Research Report WE SAW IT ALL ALONG: VISUAL HINDSIGHT BIAS IN CHILDREN AND ADULTS Daniel M. Bernstein, Cristina Atance, Geoffrey R. Loftus and Andrew N. Meltzoff University of Washington, Seattle Abstract We traced the developmental origins and trajectory of the hindsight bias. Three-, four-, and five-year-old children and adults identified gradually clarifying images of degraded common objects on a computer. Half the time, observers did not know in advance what the object would become. Other times, observers knew in advance the object's identity, and estimated when a naïve same-age peer would identify the clarifying object. In two experiments, children and adults demonstrated hindsight bias by using advance knowledge to overestimate their same-age peers' ability to identify the objects. The magnitude of this bias declined across age in one experiment, but remained relatively stable over age in the other experiment. This is the first demonstration of hindsight bias in children. These findings may have important implications for children's theory of mind. When we try to estimate what another person or we would, should, or could have done in a particular situation, our judgment is often clouded by hindsight. Nearly three decades of research have established the existence of, and boundary conditions surrounding, hindsight bias in adults (e.g., Fischhoff, 1975; Hawkins & Hastie, 1990; Werth, Strack & Forster, 2002). Most of this work has used general knowledge questions, or problems with obscure solutions. The basic finding is that after obtaining information about the answer to a question, or the solution to a problem, people think that they "knew it all along." Thus, once we know something to be true, it is extremely difficult to ignore this information and reason about our own Address correspondence to Daniel M. Bernstein, Department of Psychology, Box 351525, University of Washington, Seattle, WA, 98195-1525; e-mail: dbernstein@u.washington.edu. prior state or another person's ignorance. Despite an abundant literature on hindsight bias, this effect has not yet been demonstrated in children. The reason for this is that traditional hindsight tasks rely on verbal methods and introspection well beyond the grasp of young children (e.g., "what would you have said if you hadn't known x?"). To circumvent this problem, we used a computer-based visual identification procedure recently developed by Harley and colleagues (Harley, Carlsen & Loftus, 2001; 2003). Although Harley et al. used this technique for adults, it is appropriate for children as it relies predominantly on visual perception rather than language. In our version of the task, observers children, as well as adults identified gradually clarifying common objects. On half the trials, observers had no advance knowledge of the object's identity, while on the remaining trials, observers knew a priori what the object was going to be, and their task was to estimate when a same age peer would identify it. The task and questions were identical for both children and adults: Observers were told that the same age peer had no advance knowledge of the clarifying objects' identity. Thus, our hindsight tasks resembled conceptual perspective taking tasks in that children and adults tried to determine a naïve peer's perspective (cf., Taylor, 1988). Our aim was to determine when hindsight bias develops and whether the magnitude of this bias changes with age. EXPERIMENT 1 In Experiment 1, observers identified gradually clarifying images of degraded common objects. On half the trials, observers were naïve about the objects' identity (Baseline), whereas on other trials observers had foreknowledge, and their task was to estimate when a same age peer would identify the objects (Hindsight). PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE (2004, IN PRESS) 1

Visual Hindsight Bias Blur Pixel Crop Fig. 1. Stimulus degradations. Experiment 1 included Blur and Pixel. Experiment 2 included Blur and Crop. Method Observers Observers included 12 3-year-olds (Mean = 43.4 months, Range = 40-46; 7 female); 12 4-yearolds (Mean = 56.2 months, Range = 52-59; 7 female); 12 5-year-olds (Mean = 67 months, Range = 62-69; 8 female); and 16 University of Washington undergraduates (Adult: n = 16; 8 female). Materials Thirty-two line drawings of common objects (Snodgrass & Vanderwart, 1980) served as stimuli. Pictures of each object were scaled to fit a 245 X 245 pixel square on a G4 Macintosh laptop computer. Each object was degraded in two ways: Blur and Pixel (see Figure 1). Blurring was accomplished for each object by Fouriertransforming the object from pixel space into spatial-frequency space, multiplying the resulting frequency amplitude spectrum by a Gaussian lowpass filter and inverse-fourier transforming the result back into pixel space. Degree of blur was determined by the size of the low-pass filter and, for technical reasons, is characterized as a distance (feet). For details, see Bernstein, Loftus & Meltzoff (2003) and Loftus (2001). For Pixel, some proportion of image pixels was changed to random grayscale values. Degree of Pixel degradation was characterized as the noise-to-signal ratio: p/(1-p) where p is proportion changed pixels. For each object and each degradation type, we designed 15 increasingly degraded images such that differences between successive degraded images were roughly equal perceptually. Each object clarified from fully degraded to clear. Each of the 15 images per object remained on screen for 1000 ms., and then was replaced by the next, less degraded image 1. Counterbalancing Each trial involved observers identifying clarifying images of an object. The experimental session consisted of four blocks of eight trials per block. For a given block of trials, degradation type (i.e., Blur or Pixel), and outcome knowledge (i.e., Baseline or Hindsight) remained constant. The four experimental conditions were counterbalanced within each age group. A different object order was randomly generated for each set of four observers within each age group. The same object order was used for each of the four age groups. Design There were three independent variables: Age (3, 4, 5, Adult) X Degradation Type (Blur, Pixel) X Outcome Knowledge (Baseline, Hindsight). Degradation Type and Outcome Knowledge were within-subject factors whereas Age was a between-subject factor. Procedure Observers were tested, seated before a computer, either in their homes or in the laboratory. Observers received four practice trials, in which they identified images of common objects. The two Outcome Knowledge conditions (Baseline and Hindsight) involved different sets of objects. In the Baseline condition, each object was presented from most degraded to least degraded. Observers 1 As part of another project, children completed three false belief tasks, but these data are not relevant to present concerns and will not be presented here. 2 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE (2004, IN PRESS)

D. Bernstein et al. Ratio (Pixel) Blur 3.0 1.0 2.0 1.0 3 4 5 Age (Years) Pixel Adult 4.0 3.0 2.0 Ratio (Blur) Fig. 2. Experiment 1 results. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. The Pixel curve should be read against the left axis, while the Blur curve should be read against the right axis. named the object aloud as soon as they identified it while it clarified. In the Hindsight condition, observers first saw the object in full clarity, and were asked, "What do you think it is?" After the child observers responded, they were told: "Ok, now it's Ernie's turn to play." For the children, we used an Ernie puppet from Sesame Street. Adults were simply told about a peer named Ernie. Both children and adults were told that Ernie was the same age as them, but that he had not seen the object or heard its name (Ernie wore felt earphones, and was taken out of a bag before being introduced to children). Child Observers were told: "You need to let me know exactly when Ernie sees the 'mouse' (current object). Say, 'mouse' as soon as Ernie first sees the mouse." The object then clarified just as it had in the Baseline condition. The experimenter (DMB) stopped the object clarification when children indicated that Ernie saw the object (e.g., "now he sees it" or "mouse"). The Experimenter then typed children's responses. Adult observers stopped the object and typed their own responses. Results and Discussion Child Observers identified over 96% of the objects. Only correctly identified objects were analyzed. Our main question was whether all four age groups demonstrated hindsight bias. We calculated hindsight bias for each degradation type as the ratio of the identification point in the Hindsight condition divided by that in the Baseline condition. These ratios appear in Figure 2 2. Note that a ratio value greater than 1.0 indicates hindsight 2 The data from one 4-year-old were not analyzed, because her Blur and Pixel hindsight values (5.5 and 14.9, respectively) were well above the values for the rest of her group. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE (2004, IN PRESS) 3

Visual Hindsight Bias bias, i.e., the observer identified the object at a more degraded level in the Hindsight condition than in the Baseline condition. All groups show hindsight: for no group did the 95% confidence interval include 1.0. Across the four groups, the average log hindsight effects are 1.91±0.22 and 1.69±0.19 for Blur and Pixel respectively 3. Figure 2 shows that hindsight bias declined with age. We demonstrate this in two ways. First, we applied a contrast for monotonic decline to the four age groups (Weights = 6 1-1 -6) to the log ratio for each degradation type. The magnitudes of this contrast were 1.89±1.17 and 1.49±1.30 for Blur and Pixel, respectively; i.e., neither confidence interval included zero. Second, we calculated the ratio of children-to-adult hindsight scores. These ratios were 1.69 (95% confidence interval 1.30-2.20) and 1.62 (95% confidence interval 1.23-2.13) for Blur and Pixel, respectively; i.e., neither confidence interval included 1.0. These results indicate that (1) preschool children and adults exhibit hindsight bias by claiming that a naïve same age peer could identify images at a more degraded level than they themselves were able to identify similar images and (2) the magnitude of this bias declines from childhood to adulthood for both degradation types. EXPERIMENT 2 The results of Experiment 1 indicate that children and adults are both susceptible to visual hindsight bias and that this bias declines with age. It is possible, however, that the observed hindsight bias resulted from using different objects in the Baseline and Hindsight conditions. Research in adults has shown that hindsight bias occurs whether individuals answer different questions (in our case, questions about different objects) in the Baseline and Hindsight conditions (as was done in Experiment 1) or the same set of questions twice. To test the generality of the visual hindsight bias, in Experiment 2 we presented the same objects for identification in the Baseline and Hindsight conditions. We also replaced the Pixel degradation with a Cropping degradation. Method Observers Observers included 20 3-year-olds (Mean = 3 In this article, when we state y±x, x refers to the 95% confidence interval. 43.3 months, Range = 38-47; 13 female); 12 4- year-olds (Mean = 54 months, Range = 49-59; 6 female); and 12 5-year-olds (Mean = 66.75 months, Range = 62-79; 7 female), and one group of University of Washington undergraduates (Adult: n = 12; 8 female). Materials and Counterbalancing The materials and clarification process were similar to Experiment 1, with the following exceptions. There were 16 objects, each shown twice (once in the Baseline condition and once in the Hindsight condition). Instead of 15 images of each object, we designed 30 images. In the Crop condition, we cropped the object and then presented expanding portions of it, beginning in the middle, and expanding to the borders (see Figure 1). As with the Blur condition, we measured degree of Crop in terms of distance in feet (see Bernstein et al. 2003 for details). Unlike the continuous clarification procedure used in Experiment 1, images clarified as follows. Each image remained on screen for 600 ms., and was then replaced by the next, clearer one. After the 8 th image was displayed, it remained on screen until the observer responded. Then, over the 21 remaining images, the image halted after every third image had been displayed. Thus, each trial consisted of eight stopping points in total over the 30 images of each object. Unlike Experiment 1, where the observer halted the image clarification to identify the object, this new clarification procedure permitted us to collect observers' responses at eight discrete points in time. Stimulus counterbalancing was done as in Experiment 1, except that a different object order was randomly generated for each set of two observers (instead of 4) within each age group. The same object order was used in both the Baseline and Hindsight conditions. Design The design was identical to Experiment 1. Procedure The procedure was similar to Experiment 1, except that the same object was shown in both the Baseline and Hindsight conditions. Therefore, observers first identified objects in the two Baseline conditions (Blur and Crop), and then estimated when Ernie identified the same objects as they clarified in the Hindsight conditions. Observers were not told about Ernie until they had completed the two Baseline conditions. For adults, the name of each object appeared onscreen prior to each trial 4 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE (2004, IN PRESS)

D. Bernstein et al. Ratio (Crop) Blur 2.0 1.0 1.0 3 4 5 Age (Years) Crop Adult 3.0 2.0 Ratio (Blur) Fig. 3. Experiment 2 results. Error bars are 95% confidence intervals. The Crop curve should be read against the left axis, while the Blur curve should be read against the right axis. in the Hindsight conditions. Children were told the object's name (e.g., "this is going to be the fish"). After each stopping point in the Baseline (and Hindsight) conditions, observers were asked: "what do you (does Ernie) see right now?" Children often responded to this question with answers like, "nothing," "not yet," or "show me more" before identifying the object. Results and Discussion Child Observers identified over 99% of the objects 4. Only correctly identified objects were analyzed. We calculated hindsight bias as in Ex- 4 The data from one 4-year-old and one 5-year-old were not analyzed, because their Blur and Crop hindsight values (0.72 and 0.45 for the 4-year-old and 0.21 and 0.34 for the 5-year-old) were well below the values for the rest of their respective groups. periment 1, and the ratios are shown in Figure 3. The overall hindsight effect is less evident than it was in Experiment 1; however across the four groups hindsight emerged: the average hindsight effects are 1.70±0.25 and 1.32±0.17 for Blur and Crop, respectively. To determine whether the hindsight bias declined with age, we carried out analyses analogous to those in Experiment 1. The magnitudes of the monotonic-decrease contrast were 0.57±0.90 and 0.42±0.93 for Blur and Pixel, respectively; i.e., both confidence intervals included zero. The ratio of children-to-adult hindsight effects were 1.25 (95% confidence interval 1.10-1.42) and 1.10 (95% confidence interval 0.84-1.41) for Blur and Crop, respectively; i.e., only Blur excluded the null ratio of 1.0. PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE (2004, IN PRESS) 5

Visual Hindsight Bias In summary, the results of Experiment 2 replicate the main finding of Experiment 1: Children ages 3 to 5 and adults showed visual hindsight bias in a computer object identification task. Unlike Experiment 1, Experiment 2 did not yield clear evidence that hindsight bias declined with age. General Discussion In two experiments, preschoolers (3-, 4-, and 5-year-olds) and adults exhibited visual hindsight bias on the same object identification task. Our task was structured such that, half the time, observers did not know the object's identity a priori as it clarified (Baseline), while other times, observers knew the object's identity before estimating when a naïve, same age peer would identify the object (Hindsight). When observers knew the object's identity, they overestimated their peer's knowledge by claiming that their peer would identify the object before they themselves could. This visual hindsight bias occurred for two different types of visual degradation in each experiment and whether observers were tested on different objects (Experiment 1) or the same objects (Experiment 2) in the Baseline and Hindsight conditions. In sum, there was strong evidence that hindsight bias develops early in life and persists into adulthood (see Jacobs & Klaczynski, 2002). Hindsight bias declined with age in Experiment 1, but not in Experiment 2. This is the first demonstration that hindsight bias exists in children. These findings may have implications for theory of mind development. Although both hindsight and theory of mind tasks, such as representational change (Gopnik & Astington, 1988), require children to reason about their own, or another's, state of ignorance, the substantial increase in performance that is welldocumented in this, and other similar tasks, around age 4, is not mirrored by a concurrent decrease in the hindsight bias (see Birch & Bloom, 2001). These findings also may have more applied significance for both adult and child eyewitness testimony, where, in retrospect, observers believe that viewing conditions at the scene of a crime were better than they were. The present approach provides a valuable inter-disciplinary tool to study perception and cognition from both a developmental and cognitive science standpoint. REFERENCES Bernstein, D.M., Loftus, G.R. & Meltzoff, A. (2003). Object identification and perceptual interference in Acknowledgments This work was supported by the University of Washington's Center for Mind, Brain & Learning, the Talaris Research Institute, the Apex Foundation (the family foundation of Bruce and Jolene McCaw), and NIMH grant MH41637 to G. Loftus. We thank Erin Harley for her insights. preschool children and adults. Manuscript submitted. Birch, S. & Bloom, P. (2001). Children's appreciation of mental states: A curse of knowledge account. Poster presented at the 2 nd meeting of the Cognitive Development Society, Virginia Beach, VA. Fischhoff, B. (1975). Hindsight Foresight: The effect of outcome knowledge on judgment under uncertainty. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 1, 288-299. Gopnik, A. & Astington, J.W. (1988). Children's understanding of representational change and its relation to the understanding of false belief and the appearance-reality distinction. Child Development, 59, 26-37. Harley, E. M., Carlsen, K. A., & Loftus, G. R. (2001, November). Is hindsight 20/20? Evidence for hindsight bias in visual perception. Poster presented at the 42 nd meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Orlando, FL. Harley, E. M., Carlsen, K. A., & Loftus, G. R. (2003). The "I saw it all along" effect: Demonstrations of visual hindsight bias. Manuscript submitted for publication. Hawkins, S. A. & Hastie, R. (1990). Hindsight: Biased judgment of past events after the outcomes are known. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 311-327. Jacobs, J.E., & Klaczynski, P.A. (2002). The development of judgment and decision making during childhood and adolescence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 145-149. Loftus, G.R. (2001). Recognizing faces at a distance. Presented at the meeting of the Psychonomic Society, Orlando, FL. Snodgrass, J.G. & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174-215. Taylor, M. (1988). The development of children's ability to distinguish what they know from what they see. Child Development, 59, 703-718. Werth, L., Strack, F. & Forster, J. (2002). Certainty and uncertainty: The two faces of the hindsight bias. Organizational behavior and human decision processes, 87, 323-341. 6 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE (2004, IN PRESS)