The Effectiveness of Deaf-Blind Pedestrians Warning Signage on Drivers Behaviour

Similar documents
CITY OF WOODINVILLE, WA REPORT TO THE CITY COUNCIL rd Avenue NE, Woodinville, WA Q...,,

Best Practice Principles for teaching Orientation and Mobility skills to a person who is Deafblind in Australia

APPLICATION FOR PARATRANSIT ELIGIBLE SERVICE

DRIVING AT NIGHT. It s More Dangerous

SPED 510 Podcasts Episode 03: Dr. Gene Bourquin

1.0 Executive Summary Introduction Figure Monroe and North Street Analysis of Traffic Data... 4

W. 45 th St. Austin, TX 78756

Sign Management Current Practices

THE TOWN OF CINCO BAYOU SIGN ORDINANCES ORDINANCE NO

W. 45 th St. Austin, TX 78756

Identification of human factors criteria to assess the effects of level crossing safety measures

Road safety. Tool 1 COMMUNITY TOOLS

Development of a Driving Attitude Scale

Protecting Workers with Smart E-Vests

Distracted Driving. Stephanie Bonne, MD

36 th International Traffic Records Forum New Orleans, Louisiana. Ron Beck Cristian Oros Missouri State Highway Patrol

Sign Retroreflectivity A Minnesota Toolkit

SAFE COMMUNITIES SURVEY RESULTS 2000

Idaho Public Driver Education

Case description - a man lying on a road who has been run over

Identify the letter of the choice that best completes the statement or answers the question.

1. POLICY: 2. DEFINITIONS

Arts & Science of Sign Design

Americans with Disabilities Act (ADA)

This tool box talk will address the potential risks of working with vibrating tools, and what you can do to minimise those risks

lyondellbasell.com Exercise Safety

INTERSECTION SIGHT DISTANCE

MEDICAL FITNESS TO DRIVE EVALUATIONS

Wireless Emergency Communications Project

AWARENESS INTERACTION TRAINING

Driving at Night. It's More Dangerous

6. What would you do when facing the following Traffic signal: FLASHING RED?

Traffic Sign Detection and Identification

STAFF REPORT ACTION REQUIRED. Electronic and Illuminated Sign Study and Recommendations for Amendments to Chapter 694

Driving Cessation and HD

An Examination on Required Sound Levels for Acoustic Warning Devices for Quiet Vehicles

A PCT Primer. Fred Nickols 6/30/2011

CITY OF TORONTO. BY-LAW No

Effective Kerb Heights for Blind and Partially Sighted People

The Design of Driving Simulator Performance Evaluations for Driving With Vision Impairments and Visual Aids. Aaron J. Mandel

Fatigue management guidelines

USSCF ON-PREMISE SIGNS / RESEARCH CONCLUSIONS

PCT 101. A Perceptual Control Theory Primer. Fred Nickols 8/27/2012

LAKE COUNTY SHERIFF S OFFICE

Running Head: Overcoming Social and Communication Barriers 1

Older Driver at Risk: What do I do?

Do you know how to help people who are blind, deafblind or partially sighted?

John Eliot School. 135 Wellesley Avenue, Needham, MA Friday, December SPECIAL EDITION - Bulletin #9

Evaluation of a Training Program (STRAP) Designed to Decrease Young Drivers Secondary Task Engagement in High Risk Scenarios

Transportation and Healthy Aging: Issues and Ideas for an Aging Society

Watch and wait (active monitoring)

Using A Logic Model Framework For Program Planning And Evaluation

Seattle LED Adaptive Lighting Study

Changing Driver Behavior Through Unconscious Stereotype Activation

briefing notes - road safety issues Auckland Motorways

Tips for Effective Communications

Florida as a Model State for Older Drivers. Kezia D. Awadzi, PhD March 7, 2008 CMS Annual Student Conference Gainesville, Florida

Introduction. Diagnosis

DOWNLOAD OR READ : THEORY TEST HIGHWAY CODE PDF EBOOK EPUB MOBI

AN OVERVIEW OF TRAFFIC ACCIDENTS INVOLVING THE ELDERLY POPULATION IN THE PUBLIC URBAN ROADS OF SÃO PAULO CITY

Public Hearings on Planned Upgrades to the New Car Assessment Program. AGENCY: National Highway Traffic Safety Administration (NHTSA), Department of

Achievements in Public Health, Motor-Vehicle Safety: A 20th... Century Public Health Achievement

Diminished Driving Recognition and Screening of Older Drivers

TRUCKS AND CHILD SAFETY (TACS) Overview of TACS, the benefits, application process & FAQ s

Driver Behaviour Issues relevant to Temporary Traffic Management solutions

Texas Transportation Institute The Texas A&M University System College Station, Texas

Arlington Public Schools Thomas Jefferson Site Evaluation Transportation, Circulation, and Parking Review

In-Vehicle Communication and Driving: An Attempt to Overcome their Interference

Fear Ladder (Example)

Catherine. I am 46 yrs old with Usher syndrome 2a. I am married with two teenage boys 15 and 13. I am

Regional Differences in Preferences for Median Crossover Signing

M A N I T O B A ) Order No. 35/06 ) THE HIGHWAYS PROTECTION ACT ) March 9, 2006

ROAD SAFETY MONITOR. ALCOHOL-IMPAIRED DRIVING IN THE UNITED STATES Results from the 2018 TIRF USA Road Safety Monitor

CORTE MADERA TOWN COUNCIL STAFF REPORT TOWN MANAGER, MAYOR AND MEMBERS OF THE TOWN COUNCIL KELLY CROWE, ASSOCIATE CIVIL ENGINEER

HIGHLANDS COMMUNITY DEVELOPMENT DISTRICT

2008AARPDriverSafety Program CourseEvaluation

Emerging Technologies and Themes in Sleep Medicine. Luke Roling American Academy of Sleep Medicine, Communications Specialist; Darien, Illinois

American Society of Highway. Beardsley Connector Project. Jacob Dean

NDSS Helpline ndss.com.au

Lake Charles Transit System (LCTS) Application for Para-Transit Service Program

OLDER PEOPLE WITH DUAL SENSORY LOSS: MEETING THEIR NEEDS. Nicola Venus-Balgobin

Accessibility. Serving Clients with Disabilities

Condensed summary lesson: Organ and tissue donation stories

UNOFFICIAL COPY OF HOUSE BILL 1457 A BILL ENTITLED

ADA PARATRANSIT ELIGIBILITY APPLICATION AND INSTRUCTIONS

Modifications to Traffic Signal Operation to Improve Safety for Alcoholaffected

School Zone vs Work Zone What s the difference?

Transportation Committee. March 2018 Committee Reports. Committee Members: Christo Brehm and Sabina Urdes. Contact:

NOVEMBER 7, 2017 STREET LIGHT MASTER PLAN EXISTING CONDITIONS REPORT

Freak the Mighty. Edition: Scholastic, New York Guided Imagery and problem solving: Instructions for Generating Discussion

Tactical Checkpoint: Hail/Warn Suppress/Stop Target Behavioral Response Laboratory

Questions for ionising radiation applications

The Evaluation of Children with Deaf-Blindness: A Parent Mini-Guide

IEP MEETING CHECKLIST FOR PARENTS OF CHILDREN WITH DEAF-BLINDNESS

We have developed this leaflet to help you understand more about cataracts.

Tips on How to Better Serve Customers with Various Disabilities

The Children s Home K Community Walk. for Children & Families. Saturday, May 6th, 9:00am to 12:00noon

Exploring the Needs of Aging Drivers in the Transportation System

Slow down and live in the moment. This is a worthwhile resolution for the New

Transcription:

The Effectiveness of Deaf-Blind Pedestrians Warning Signage on Drivers Behaviour Dona Sauerburger, Eugene Bourquin, and Jomania Sauerburger People with disabilities and their advocates have sometimes requested that special warning signage be posted in areas to help make street crossings safer. Related research has found these signs ineffective but they continue to be installed. The current research examined whether or not posted signage indicating the presence of people who are deaf-blind would cause drivers to yield more frequently for pedestrians with a mobility cane. The results can inform O&M practices and professionals working on behalf of people with disabilities. In the United States, orientation and mobility (O&M) specialists are sometimes asked to make recommendations about or support requests to make streets safer by installing signs such as BLIND CHILD AREA or SLOW-DEAF CHILD. The effectiveness of similar signs, such as Children at Play, has been shown to be questionable, and studies indicate no reduction in speed or incidence of accidents where they have been installed (Vanderbilt, 2008). Other data cast doubt on the ability of signage alone to control drivers behaviours. According to Vanderbilt (2008) in his book, Traffic: Why we drive the way we do, when drivers routinely see signs warning of deer crossings (in the United States) or elephant crossings (in Sri Lanka) or camel signs (in Tunisia)... studies have shown that most drivers do not change their speed at all (p. 186). The Manual on Uniform Traffic Control Devices (MUTCD), the American road managers professional and technical master guide, suggests that as the number of these signs increases, the effectiveness of each sign is reduced, and a 1993 NCHRP Synthesis of Highway Practice No. 186: Supplemental Advance Warning Devices indicated that these signs are not considered effective (Bourquin, 2011). For example, the Kansas DOT (2011) seems unambiguous in their opinion of child area warning signs and lists six rationales against the installations at its website: These signs do not describe where the child might be; most streets within a residential area have children who react in the same way, and each driver must be aware of all children in a neighborhood environment; these signs provide parents and children with a false sense of security that their children are safe when playing in or near the street; when the novelty of such a sign wears off, the signs no longer attract the attention of regular passersby; unique or unusual warning signs are a target for vandals and souvenir hunters and International Journal of Orientation & Mobility Volume 5, Number 1, 2012 11

have a high replacement cost; unique message signs have no legal meaning or established precedent for use in basic traffic engineering references; their use is discouraged because of both the lack of proven effectiveness and undesirable liability exposure (para. 1-6). There has not been an empirical study of warning area signage installed for people who are blind, deaf, or deaf-blind. There is only professional opinion and tangential evidence. Specialists who work with people who are deaf-blind have found that some citizens have requested the installation of warning signs. One individual in Maryland who is deaf-blind requested signs be installed (Figure 1) stating that they help drivers to be aware that deaf-blind people Figure 1. Signage with deaf/blind pedestrian plaque. live in the area. I walk across the street and they will stop for me (Ann Black, personal communication, January 3, 2011). We decided to see whether or not these signs had any influence on drivers yielding as they approached the crosswalk. Method Several rectangular signs saying DEAF / BLIND PEDESTRIANS (called plaques by traffic engineers) had been installed on poles at crosswalks along College Avenue in Rockville, Maryland, at the request of an individual who is deaf-blind. That individual said she has not crossed at this specific location for the last several months and had crossed there only occasionally before that. The authors talked with some of the people walking along College Avenue and they said they had never noticed people who were blind in the area. That provided reasonable assurance that this site was suitable for our study because the drivers were not likely to have been influenced by frequent experiences of seeing people with white canes crossing there regularly. The authors then identified two similar nearby streets (Fordham Ave. and Martin Ave.) which did not have the warning plaques installed. All three crossings had white painted crosswalks with the same traffic control, a diamond-shaped pedestrian crosswalk sign, with either a diagonal arrow or a DEAF / BLIND PEDESTRIANS warning plaque placed on the pole below the crosswalk sign (Figures 1 and 2). The three sites were at streets with similar widths, with one lane for moving traffic in each direction (Figure 3), and each had the same speed limit of 25 mph. According to the Table of Speed and Stopping Distance (James Madison University, 2011), cars 12 International Journal of Orientation & Mobility Volume 5, Number 1, 2012

travelling 25-30 miles per hour would need 85-109 feet to stop for pedestrians, which we averaged to 97 feet for this study. At each site, landmarks were chosen that were 97 feet to the left and to the right of the crosswalk. The drivers line of sight for the pedestrian was at least 97 feet in all trials used to collect data. In order to determine an effective method for crossing that would be sensitive to drivers responses the existing literature was reviewed related to yielding at uncontrolled crosswalks and roundabouts. Bourquin, Wall Emerson, and Sauerburger (2011) found that 90% of the drivers yielded for a pedestrian who walked out into the street displaying a cane; in other studies, depending on the type of site, more than 50% of drivers did not yield to pedestrians waiting near the curb with their white cane on the ground (Ashmead, Guth, Wall, Long, & Ponchillia, 2005; Geruschat & Hassan, 2005). To achieve a balance in drivers yielding behaviour, a collaborating pedestrian (one of the authors, called the pedestrian from here on) took a single step into the street then stopped and raised the tip of the cane higher than her head and brought Figure 2. Crosswalk with one of the authors crossing. Figure 3. View of a vehicle approach to a crosswalk. it back to the ground several times for each trial. Protocol We collected data at the three sites using the following procedure: (1) The pedestrian wore bright clothes and had a long white cane. She waited approximately 10 feet from the curb until a vehicle approached. (2) The pedestrian walked toward the curb with her cane tapping, reaching the street at about the same time that the driver reached 97 feet from the crosswalk. (3) The pedestrian stepped into the street and, looking straight ahead, she moved the cane high up and down several times and waited for the driver to stop (Figure 2). (4) If the driver stopped, the pedestrian proceeded across. If the driver did not stop, she waited until the car passed and then returned to the sidewalk. We recorded whether or not the driver yielded (stopped to allow the pedestrian to cross or slowed down enough that the driver would have been able to stop if the International Journal of Orientation & Mobility Volume 5, Number 1, 2012 13

pedestrian had started to cross), or the driver did not yield (did not stop or slow down). Results We presented the pedestrian with the white cane to 27 drivers at College Avenue with the DEAF / BLIND PEDESTRIANS warning plaque (21 from the pedestrian s left and six from her right), and nine drivers at Fordham Avenue and 33 drivers at Martin Avenue, both crosswalks having the diagonal arrow plaques (27 drivers at both crosswalks were from the pedestrian s left, 15 from her right). We identified two results, yield and no yield, in each of the two conditions, DEAF / BLIND PEDESTRIANS warning plaque (DB plaque) and no DEAF / BLIND PEDESTRIANS warning plaque (No DB plaque). Data indicating yielding (stopping and sufficiently slowing vehicles) were combined. Data from the two sites without DEAF / BLIND PEDESTRIANS warning plaque were combined. Finally, the data was conflated from approaching vehicles in near and far lanes (left and right perpendicular traffic). Because of the small sample size and results of the no yield category data, we used a 2x2 contingency table and computed results from a Fisher s exact test. The outcome (P value=1.00; 95% CI) indicated no association between the DEAF / BLIND PEDESTRIANS warning plaque and yielding behaviours (Table 1). Discussion and Conclusions The presence of the DEAF / BLIND PEDESTRIANS plaque appeared to make no difference to drivers. Drivers did not yield more often to a pedestrian with a cane when the warning message was present at the crosswalk than they did when there was no such warning message. Our results are in accord with studies of the effectiveness of area warning signs and may guide us when making recommendations for such signs for pedestrians who are blind or deaf-blind. Perhaps the false sense of security that warning signage provides to pedestrians is the most crucial factor to consider. Even if drivers are able to notice, read, and process the information, the results are not what a person who is deaf-blind might anticipate. When pedestrians who are deaf-blind and O&M specialists discuss and assess the risks for crossing streets, these study results should be considered. When and if we are approached to advocate for a DEAF / BLIND PEDESTRIANS or similar warning signage, we can respond with the facts: traffic professionals do not generally recommend these signs and there is no evidence that supports that a sign will make a pedestrian safer, even if the drivers have a good view of the sign and the pedestrian. We can also anticipate that pedestrians, advocates, and others may remain unconvinced, at least when children and people with disabilities are involved. Table 1. Yields and no yields at crosswalks. Yield No Yield Total No DB plaque 38 4 42 DB Plaque 24 3 27 Total 62 7 69 14 International Journal of Orientation & Mobility Volume 5, Number 1, 2012

As one transportation department claimed, Widespread public faith in traffic signs to provide protection and parental concern for children s safety results in frequent requests for this type of signage (Shawnee, 2011, para. 2). The best approach may be education for consumers, families, agencies, and schools. Limitations and further research The number of trials in this study was relatively small. We tested a particular (but standard and common) type and size of warning plaque. Only three sites were used for the trials. These factors may limit the scope for generalising the results. In addition, the likelihood of yielding immediately after the signage was installed is unknown, and, if there was any effect, how long it would have influenced drivers. All vehicles appeared to move toward the crossing at or near the speed limit, but the precise speeds of each vehicle s approach was also unknown. The study was conducted on a bright, sunny day with the pedestrian wearing brightly-coloured clothes. It is unknown whether or not the drivers would have responded similarly if it had been raining or dark. We suggest that further research would be useful to find out how pedestrians who are blind or deaf-blind can increase the likelihood that drivers will see them and react appropriately. References Ashmead, D. H., Guth, D., Wall, R. S., Long, R. G., & Ponchillia, P. E. (2005). Street crossing by sighted and blind pedestrians at a modern roundabout. Journal of Transportation Engineering, 131(11), 812-821. Bourquin, E. (2011). Are area warning signs effective? AER O&M Division Newsletter, Summer, 17(3), 2. Bourquin, E., Wall Emerson, R., & Sauerburger, D. (2011). Conditions that influence drivers yielding behavior while crossing at uncontrolled crossings. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 105(12), 760-769. Geruschat, D., & Hassan, S. (2005). Driver behavior in yielding to sighted and blind pedestrians at roundabouts. Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 99(5), 286-302. James Madison University. (2011). Tables of speed and stopping distances. Retrieved from http://www.jmu.edu/safetyplan/ vehicle/generaldriver/stoppingdistance. shtml Kansas DOT. (2011). Why signs are not installed. Retrieved from http://www. wichita.gov/nr/rdonlyres/1d3233a9-9aad-4f4b-850f-520d430c37d9/0/ BrochureChildrenatPlaySigns.pdf n.a. (2011). FAQ: Traffic. Retrieved from http://gsh.cityofshawnee.org/web /ShawneeCMS.nsf/b62f116aa62dde9 4862575680053ac82/fbd53e28788b6270 8625759100490ac7?OpenDocument Vanderbilt, T. (2008). Traffic: Why we drive the way we do (and what it says about us). New York, NY: Alfred A. Knopf. Dona Sauerburger MA., COMS., Freelance O&M Specialist, Maryland, USA; e-mail: <dona@ sauerburger.org>. Eugene Bourquin, DHA., COMS., CI., & CT., CLVT., Helen Keller National Center for Deaf-Blind Youths and Adults, New York, USA; e-mail: <oandmhk@msn.com>. Jomania Sauerburger, research assistant, Maryland, USA. International Journal of Orientation & Mobility Volume 5, Number 1, 2012 15