Copyright 2015 The Guilford Press

Similar documents
The Resilient Revolution takes on

Biological and Experimental Psychology School of Biological and Chemical Sciences Individual Differences in Environmental Sensitivity

Section C. Intrinsic and Extrinsic Influences on Infant and Early Childhood Regulatory Development

Gene-Environment Interplay and Epigenetic Processes

Emotion and Cognition: An Intricately Bound Developmental Process

Resilience in Individuals and Communities

How social experiences influence the brain Frances A Champagne and James P Curley

Human Cognitive Developmental Neuroscience. Jan 27

COURSE DESCRIPTIONS 科目簡介

The Action Is In the Interaction

Child Mental Health: A Review of the Scientific Discourse

CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE

Recent advances in the study of biological alterations in post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD)

PSYC 335 Developmental Psychology I

Normal cognitive but disturbing behavior moral insanity. Early attributions biased: Cause of the problem within the individual

Epigenetic and Neurodevelopmental Perspectives on Variation in Parenting Behavior

Mechanisms Linking Early Experience and the Emergence of Emotions Illustrations From the Study of Maltreated Children

COMT EFFECT ON AGGRESSION IN CHILDREN WITH & WITHOUT ADHD (British Cohort) blems (t-scores) Aggressive Behaviour Prob. n =

Strategic Application of Human Development Applying Positive Psychology. The Principles of Developmental Psychology

A Developmental Perspective on the Role of Genes on Substance Use Disorder. Elisa M. Trucco, Ph.D., Florida International University

Psychology Scientific Inquiry Domain Research Methods, Measurement, and Statistics

How does adversity in childhood get under the skin

Index. Note: Page numbers of article titles are in boldface type.

SIBLINGS OF CHILDREN WITH INTELLECTUAL DISABILITY 1

Introduction: Exploring Dissociation: Setting the Course

Executive Summary. Future Research in the NSF Social, Behavioral and Economic Sciences Submitted by Population Association of America October 15, 2010

Child Psychopathology Psychology 1270 Spring Term, 2015

FARMINGDALE STATE COLLEGE

Conceptual Issues in Studying the Development of Self-Regulation

EFFECTS OF STRESS ACROSS GENERATIONS: WHY SEX MATTERS

Emotional Development

Neurobiology of Exceptionality

Epilogue. Debra H. Zand and Katherine J. Pierce

Cannabis Changes. Understanding Dynamics of Use and Dependence N. Liebregts

Toward the integration of genetics, epigenetics, and intervention

Parenting quality interacts with genetic variation in dopamine receptor D4 to influence temperament in early childhood

CHAPTER 9. Biological Foundations of Personality. The Biological Tradition. Temperament

The Adolescent Developmental Stage

PSYCHOLOGY. The Psychology Major. Preparation for the Psychology Major. The Social Science Teaching Credential

Author s Note (references for ideas mentioned in Author s Note as being from Chapter One

Emotion in Education: A Timely and Thorough Compilation

MSc Psychological Research Methods/ MPsych Advanced Psychology Module Catalogue / 2018

Dynamic Systems Theories. L. Todd Rose & Kurt W. Fischer. Diversity is the hallmark of human behavior: Regardless of age or experience, people s

Concepts for Understanding Traumatic Stress Responses in Children and Families

DIFFERENTIAL SUSCEPTIBILITY TO ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES: From Longitudinal Research to Intervention Part B. Jay Belsky

Emotion Regulation 1. Foundations of Emotion Regulation. Neuroscience and Education (HDAP 1238)

Researchers often use observational data to estimate treatment effects

Neonatal behavior of infants at familial risk for ADHD

PSYCHOLOGY (PSYC) Courses. Psychology (PSYC) 1

What can we do to improve the outcomes for all adolescents? Changes to the brain and adolescence-- Structural and functional changes in the brain

CHAPTER I INTRODUCTION

Developmental Psychopathology: Developmental and Clinical Perspectives, 15 credits

Complementarity and the Relation Between Psychological and Neurophysiological Phenomena

2 Theories and Causes

Psychology (PSYC) Psychology (PSYC) 1

A neurogenetics approach to understanding individual differences in brain, behavior, and risk for psychopathology

Behavioral genetics: The study of differences

Introduction to Special Issue: Evidence-Based Assessment in Pediatric Psychology

Introduction to Clinical Management of Tourette Syndrome. In recent years there has been an explosion of interest in Tourette CHAPTER 1

Infants Differential Processing of Female and Male Faces Jennifer L. Ramsey-Rennels 1 and Judith H. Langlois 2

Roots of Adult Disease Traced to Early Childhood Adversity

Frontal EEG Asymmetry and Regulation during Childhood

How Do Individual Experiences Aggregate to Shape Personality Development? Elliot M. Tucker-Drob

Periods of Development. Areas of Development. What is Human Development? Why Should We Study Development? Human Development Topic 1

CUA. National Catholic School of Social Service Washington, DC Fax

Week 2: Disorders of Childhood

INTEGRATIVE PSYCHOTHERAPY

TERMINOLOGY INSECURITY? APPLICATION OF ATTACHMENT THEORY TO THE HUMAN-COMPANION ANIMAL BOND, AND REVIEW OF ITS EFFECTS

Robust cognitive neuroscience constructs and comorbidity: can they help?

The Brain is Still a Mystery. National Institutes of Health 7/11/2012

PATHWAYS. Age is one of the most consistent correlates. Is Desistance Just a Waiting Game? Research on Pathways to Desistance.

CHAPTERS 1-2. Developmental Psychology. A Topical Approach to Lifespan Development

The Development of Social Cognition

Editors Jeffrey H. D. Cornelius-White Renate Motschnig-Pitrik Michael Lux. SPRINGER Science + Business Media, New York, 2013

Temperamental Effortful Control (Self- Regulation)

Jennifer Watling Neal Michigan State University

Running head: CSP BOOK REVIEW 1


Resilience in 8 Key Questions & Answers

Effective Intentions: The Power of Conscious Will

Manuscript Version Sage holds the Copyright. Introduction. Seemingly, no two investigators agree on what intelligence means or includes, but

Genetic Factors in Temperamental Individuality

Growth of Empowerment in Career Science Teachers: Implications for Professional Development

Descending Marr s levels: Standard observers are no panacea. to appear in Behavioral & Brain Sciences

Gender Sensitive Factors in Girls Delinquency

Course Outcome Summary

EHDI 2015 Louisville, KY Patricia E. Spencer Marilyn Sass-Lehrer Gallaudet University, Washington, DC

Copyright 2018 The Guilford Press

Activity 19 Great Ideas in the History of Psychology. Purpose

Supporting Families to Build Secure Attachment Relationships : Comments on Benoit, Dozier, and Egeland

Chapter 6 Genetics. Total Assessment Guide (T.A.G.) 56, 57, 58, 59, 60, 61 22, 23, 25, 26, 27, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32, 34

THE URBAN CHILD INSTITUTE

Diathesis and Stress. Lecture 40

on Brain Development Infant brain development

Trauma: Critical, Contemporary, Culturally Competent

Book Reviews. in developmental psychology, education, social work, and social policy.

Research Approach & Design. Awatif Alam MBBS, Msc (Toronto),ABCM Professor Community Medicine Vice Provost Girls Section

What kinds of research? Research

Bill Wilson. Categorizing Cognition: Toward Conceptual Coherence in the Foundations of Psychology

Transcription:

This is a chapter excerpt from Guilford Publications. Handbook of Infant Biopsychosocial Development. Edited by Susan D. Calkins. Copyright 2015. Purchase this book now: www.guilford.com/p/calkins Chapter 1 Introduction to the Volume Seeing Infant Development through a Biopsychosocial Lens Susan D. Calkins The Biopsychosocial Lens: Conceptual Integration across Domains of Infant Functioning The development of the human infant from a seemingly passive, dependent, and immature organism at birth to within a span of 2 to 3 very short years a walking, talking, seemingly independent person, is a truly remarkable process. The effort to understand these rapid changes has been the domain of infancy researchers for more than 100 years. However, late in the 20th century, the world of infancy research entered an exciting and somewhat intellectually and methodologically challenging era. The introduction of sophisticated technology, providing access to genetic and physiological processes, allowed scientists to study developmental changes that were hard to access observationally. Ultimately, this led to a more integrated view of human development that incorporated the multiple biological and behavioral factors that conspired, in sometimes unknown ways, to produce unique developmental processes and pathways. Changes in the way scientists thought about development shifted rapidly and substantially from a purely maturational perspective (growth causes change) to an environmental perspective (family causes change) to a biological perspective (development is gene driven), and finally to variations on the interactionist theme: nature (genes and biology) and nurture (families, peers, and culture) interacted in complex ways to produce a range of developmental outcomes. Clearly, the recent interactionist perspectives hold the most promise for understanding how biology, behavior, and environment all contribute to 3

4 PART I. SETTING THE STAGE developmental outcomes. As such, the aim of this volume is to present recent, novel, and paradigm- shifting work that has viewed infant development through a biopsychosocial lens. This lens offers a unique perspective on a period of life characterized by rapid changes across different domains of function, broadly characterized as biological, psychological, and social. And, this perspective has implications for how we think about, study, and analyze the data we collect about infants in these different domains. The conceptual integration of biological, behavioral, and social levels of analysis (Gottlieb, 2007) has been referred to in various ways, including developmental science (Cairns, Elder, & Costello, 1996) and developmental or dynamic systems theory (Lewkowicz, 2011; Lickliter, 2008). Regardless of the label, these approaches articulate clear support for a unique perspective on development, one that gives greater acknowledgment of the complex and reciprocal, rather than prescriptive, role of biological processes in transactional models of development (Calkins, 2011; Sameroff, 2010). The biopsychosocial perspective aims to account for the processes and mechanisms responsible for growth and change in structure and functioning in children, within the context of families and the broader social context, and with an appreciation of the contribution of underlying biological processes. The articulation of the basic principles of the biopsychosocial approach was stimulated by several landmark studies of genetic and biological processes in humans and animals. This work led to reformulations of developmental theory and, eventually, a movement toward empirical designs and analytical techniques crafted specifically to reveal the dynamic nature of early development. Thus, the biopsychosocial lens we rely on in this volume provides scholars of infancy with a view of development that crosses multiple levels within and between individuals. The focus of the work in this volume varies considerably from chapter to chapter; as a whole, the work presented here encompasses the genetic, neural, physiological, psychological, familial, and contextual levels of individual functioning, and together yield a richer understanding of the daunting complexity of human infants and their worlds. Emergence of the Biopsychosocial Perspective in the Study of Infancy The contemporary study of infant development has yielded remarkable growth in our understanding of the very early emerging skills and abilities of young children. Historically, efforts to study human infants viewed them as somewhat passive creatures whose development depended largely on the maturation of the brain and on the interventions of others. Soon, however, clever research methodologies evolved for studying the appearance and development of skills in humans lacking the linguistic skills to reveal their cognitive and

1. Introduction to the Volume 5 social knowledge. These methodologies capitalized on the primitive abilities of infants looking, sucking, and motor movement to infer skills and knowledge that ranged from the perceptual to the social. In this way, science began to view infants as active participants in their own development, capable of using their social environments to facilitate subsequent learning and skill development. A large body of infancy research conducted throughout the latter half of the 20th century adopted this perspective and focused on identifying patterns of abilities and developments in the broad domains of emotion and cognition, as well on studying the significance of individual differences in these domains for later functioning, adaptation, and mental health (cf. Bremner & Fogel, 2004). Two significant changes in the field of child development in the late 20th and early 21st centuries, one conceptual and one methodological, again altered our view of infancy, this time to one that acknowledged the complex interactions among the child s biology, his or her behaviors, and his or her environment (Gottlieb, 2007; Sameroff, 2010; Shonkoff, 2010). First, the field of psychology more broadly, and developmental psychology in particular, began to concede that the nature nurture debate was irrelevant, because the partitioning of genetic and environmental influences in development was likely impossible given their interdependence (Lewkowicz, 2011; Meaney, 2010). Instead, the field began to embrace a more complex view of development that considered how dimensions of both nature and nurture dynamically interact across time. The movement toward this perspective was precipitated by studies of the role of genes in human and animal behavioral and biological phenomena that illuminated the complex ways that genes and environments both participate in the developmental process. The first such investigations were done with nonhuman animals and retrospective studies of human adults. For example, several pioneering animal studies focused on the serotonin transporter gene. A repeat length polymorphism in the promoter region of this gene (5-HTTLPR) has been shown to affect the rate of serotonin uptake and may play a role in a range of problematic behavioral outcomes, including aggression. This work revealed that rhesus monkeys that were raised by peers, rather than mothers, exhibited more behavioral and physiological problems (alcohol consumption, poor stress reactivity), and deficits in selfregulation (impulsivity, inappropriate aggression, orienting problems, risk taking) if they possessed the risk allele of the serotonin transporter- linked promoter region (5-HTTLPR; s/s or s/l) instead of the nonrisk allele (l/l). These findings suggest that the risk allele of the serotonin transporter gene was only predictive of maladaptive outcomes for monkeys in poor- quality rearing environments. For those monkeys that experienced a natural and supportive mother infant relationship, there was no effect of genotype (Barr et al., 2004; Bennett et al., 2002; Champoux et al., 2002; Suomi, 2006). Similarly, in two influential studies of gene environment (G E) interactions in human adults, Caspi and colleagues (2002) found that early adverse

6 PART I. SETTING THE STAGE experience alone did not predict adult psychopathology. As with the animal studies described previously, adults carrying the risk (s/s or s/l) allele of serotonin were more likely to be depressed when they experienced stressful life events than adults without the short allele or those with the short allele who did not experience stressful life events (Caspi et al., 2002). In a second study, they found that childhood maltreatment alone did not predict antisocial behavior in adulthood, but rather that there was a significantly higher chance of developing later antisocial behavior for those individuals who possessed the risk allele of the functional polymorphism of the monoamine oxidase A (MAOA) gene and also experienced maltreatment (Caspi et al., 2003). The study of G E interaction suggests that the environment in which genes are expressed alters behavioral outcomes. However, the characterization of this interaction is far from simple and the mechanisms through which development occurs are quite complex. For example, other animal work demonstrates quite convincingly that the effect of the environment occurs at both the biological and behavioral levels. For example, Meaney and colleagues have studied the rearing experience of rat pups and its role in the development of the hypothalamic pituitary adrenocortical (HPA) axis, which is principally involved in the behavioral response to stress (Meaney, 2010). Rat mothers naturally differ in the amount of licking and grooming (LG) caregiving behaviors they provide their pups; high amounts of these behaviors appear to influence the development of the rat pup s developing stress system. Rat pups of low-lg mothers that are cross- fostered with high-lg mothers become themselves less stress reactive, both physiologically and behaviorally, suggesting that the environmental exposure to different caregiving experiences alters the expression of genes that are implicated in the development of the stress system (Champagne et al., 2008; Meaney, 2010). These influential studies of the dynamic influences across genetic, physiological, and behavioral levels has guided recent work in the field of infant development. In one such study, researchers examined the effect of the dopamine receptor gene D2 (DRD2) on infant physiological responses to stress over the first year of life in the context of infants experiences with caregivers (Propper et al., 2008). Infants possessing the taq1 A1 polymorphism of DRD2, associated with impulse- control problems and sensation- seeking behaviors, who were also exposed to sensitive maternal caregiving over the first year of life, exhibited a more optimal and expected cardiac response to stress at 12 months of age, comparable to the cardiac reactivity of those infants possessing the nonrisk version of the gene. Infants without the risk allele displayed typical and effective cardiac response to stress regardless of whether mothers were sensitive, suggesting that the caregiving environment may, in fact, be less important for their regulatory outcomes. So, as the scientific understanding of human genetic and psychobiological processes has grown, scholars have come to abandon the notion that

1. Introduction to the Volume 7 development is gene driven and instead have adopted an appreciation of reciprocal processes at all levels of development. This perspective has led to a shift in focus in the field of developmental psychology, and in the study of infant development in particular, that has influenced our attempts to understand and study how specific behavioral developments emerge and influence infant development and functioning, as well as how those developments feedback to influence both biological and social processes. Scientists have come to view development as a fundamentally dynamic process involving transactions between the child and his or her environment that influence children s development, and the behavior of those who comprise their environment, at multiple biological and behavioral levels (e.g., Blair, 2002; Calkins, 2011; Lewkowicz, 2011). Empirical Implications of a Biopsychosocial Perspective According to the biopsychosocial perspective, the child s biology, behavior, and social environment are changing one another continually over the course of development. This view of development has emerged because the science of development, and the empirical work that investigates these processes, has become much more interdisciplinary in nature and begun to incorporate biological constructs and principles, as well as empirical measures and findings, from the fields of genetics, neuroscience, comparative psychology, psychobiology, and psychophysiology. This interdisciplinary nature of the perspective has led to new challenges in the study of infant development. Understanding development in such a comprehensive and transactional way implies that investigators adopt a multilevel perspective, and perhaps as well, to actually study development across different levels of influences, ranging from the genetic to the social (Gottlieb, 2007). This multilevel perspective led to a critical empirical shift in the way development is studied both in the laboratory and in the field. Over the last several years, multilevel empirical approaches to the study of infant development have proliferated. Investigators traditionally interested in a specific behavioral phenomenon such as temperament, memory, or attachment began to view these behaviors as embedded in a complex system of biological and social processes. And, as this volume reflects, the work that has emerged has surveyed a diversity of specific indicators of these processes, often using longitudinal designs to study children and their environments over time. One area of growth in this work has been the specification of what constitutes environment. Although the term often applies to the family environment, more recently, we have begun to broaden and deepen our understanding of this term, describing the family and its functioning at a more micro level, and going beyond the family to the larger social and cultural world in which the child and the family are embedded. Moreover, when considering G E interaction, the

8 PART I. SETTING THE STAGE environment consists of everything from the cellular to the social. Indeed, as Shanahan and Hofer (2005) noted, the E in G E might best be conceptualized as exposure, a term that highlights the range of processes that may alter the pathway initiated by the gene s cellular action at biological and behavioral levels across development. In addition to the challenges of design and choice of methods that are inherent in work addressing biopsychosocial processes, analytical challenges are substantial as well. The models that have been proposed are often depicted in ways that visually represent the various levels and their interdependence, but the translation of these models to statistical analysis has been slower in emerging. Much of the early work on infant development relied on relatively simplistic correlations and regressions to examine longitudinal developmental associations. Cutting- edge research driven by theoretical models designed to capture the multiple levels of functioning and how these levels change over time requires appropriately sophisticated analytical techniques. The analytic landscape is rapidly evolving in an effort to meet this need and new advanced statistical techniques are being developed for both person- and variable- centered analyses. Within variable- centered approaches, techniques such as repeated measures analysis of variance and regression analyses have been overshadowed by growth curve modeling, path analyses, and time series analyses, with the goal of describing the trajectories of change in variables that are central to infant functioning and identifying multiple predictors of deviation from those trajectories (Laursen & Hoff, 2006). Researchers have also begun to use more person- centered approaches including profile or class analyses to address questions regarding group or individual differences in patterns of development across time and associations among variables at multiple levels of child functioning. Measurement challenges are associated with these techniques, but nevertheless, they offer a means through which researchers can examine trajectories and complex associations among behavioral constructs of interest. A key point here is that, often, developmental pathway models, which are clearly well suited to the longitudinal study of infant development, predated the statistical techniques to test them (Curran & Willoughby, 2003). Nevertheless, the ongoing development and refinement of statistical methods to test such models is key to further advances in this area. The adoption of a biopsychosocial perspective on infant development has produced a corpus of work that is rich, though often challenging to integrate, a consequence that may be largely a function of its relative youth compared with the larger body of more traditional developmental research. Importantly, though, these studies routinely reveal a complexity in development that has changed the way we think about development in general and that have motivated efforts to identify specific pathways to optimal versus compromised outcomes in childhood. Moreover, studying development across levels of biology, behavior, and environment provides us with insight into the more proximal

1. Introduction to the Volume 9 developmental mechanisms and processes that affect both infant development and the environments in which infants develop, and that can help us to identify critical points of entry for early intervention and prevention. In short, a biopsychosocial lens on development holds the promise of advancing the theoretical, empirical, analytical, and translational agenda of the field of infancy. Overview of This Volume The goal of this volume is to provide a selective review of current, cuttingedge work that assesses infant functioning across different biological, behavioral, and/or contextual levels to inform our understanding of development. The authors selected for inclusion in this volume are each conducting research from within a biopsychosocial perspective, incorporating into their work at least two levels of analysis, from the genetic to the environmental. As is clear from the work presented in this volume, integrating this perspective into their work has challenged infancy researchers to grapple with complex conceptual, empirical, and analytical problems. Many questions remain about how best to consider how these transactional processes operate across time and context; how to measure and analyze biological, behavioral, and social processes that may be difficult to disentangle; and how to translate those findings into strategies to positively influence outcomes for young children. Nevertheless, clear from this volume is that these challenges are being met and the field of infancy is quickly evolving to reflect the rapid pace of empirical and analytical advances. In adopting a multilevel approach a biopsychosocial approach this volume will provide a conceptual, empirical, and translational road map for research on infant development in the 21st century. References Barr, C. S., Newman, T. K., Shannon, C., Parker, C., Dvoskin, R. L., Becker, M. L., et al. (2004). Rearing condition and rh5-httlpr interact to influence limbic hypothalamic pituitary adrenal axis response to stress in infant macaques. Biological Psychiatry, 55(7), 733 738. Bennett, A. J., Lesch, K. P., Heils, A., Long, J. C., Lorenz, J. G., Shoaf, S. E., et al. (2002). Early experience and serotonin transporter gene variation interact to influence primate CNS function. Molecular Psychiatry, 7(1), 118 122. Blair, C. (2002). School readiness: Integrating cognition and emotion in a neurobiological conceptualization of children s functioning at school entry. American Psychologist, 57, 111 127. Bremner, J. G., & Fogel, A. (2004). Blackwell handbook of infant development. Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell. Cairns, R. B., Elder, G. H., & Costello, E. J. (Eds.). (1996). Developmental science. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

10 PART I. SETTING THE STAGE Calkins, S. D. (2011). Caregiving as coregulation: Psychobiological processes and child functioning. In A. Booth, S. McHale, & N. Lansdale (Eds.), Biosocial foundations of family processes (pp. 49 59). New York: Springer. Caspi, A., McClay, J., Moffitt, T., Mill, J., Martin, J., Craig, I. W., et al. (2002). Role of genotype in the cycle of violence in maltreated children. Science, 297(5582), 851 854. Caspi, A., Sugden, K., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Craig, I. W., Harrington, H., et al. (2003). Influence of life stress on depression: Moderation by a polymorphism in the 5-HTT gene. Science, 301(5631), 386 389. Champagne, D. L., Bagot, R. C., van Hasselt, F., Ramakers, G., Meaney, M. J., de Kloet, E., et al. (2008). Maternal care and hippocampal plasticity: Evidence for experience- dependent structural plasticity, altered synaptic functioning, and differential responsiveness to glucocorticoids and stress. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(23), 6037 6045. Champoux, M., Bennett, A., Shannon, C., Higley, J. D., Lesch, K. P., & Suomi, S. J. (2002). Serotonin transporter gene polymorphism, differential early rearing, and behavior in rhesus monkey neonates. Molecular Psychiatry, 7, 1058 1063. Curran, P. J., & Willoughby, M. T. (2003). Implications of latent trajectory models for the study of developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 15(3), 581 612. Gottlieb, G. (2007). Probabilistic epigenesis. Developmental Science, 10, 1 11. Laursen, B. & Hoff, E. (2006). Person- centered and variable- centered approaches to longitudinal data. Merrill Palmer Quarterly, 52, 377 389. Lewkowicz, D. J. (2011). The biological implausibility of the nature nurture dichotomy and what it means for the study of infancy. Infancy, 16, 331 367. Lickliter, R. (2008). Developmental dynamics: The new view from the life sciences. In A. Fogel, B. J. King, & S. G. Shanker (Eds.), Human development in the twentyfirst century: Visionary ideas from systems scientists (pp. 11 17). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. Meaney, M. (2010). Epigenetics and the biological definition of gene x environment interactions. Child Development, 81, 49 71. Propper, C., Moore, G., Mills- Koonce, R., Halpern, C., Hill, A., Calkins, S., et al. (2008). Gene environment contributions to the development of vagal tone. Child Development, 79, 1378 1395. Sameroff, A. (2010). A unified theory of development: A dialectic integration of nature and nurture. Child Development, 81, 6 22. Shanahan, M. J., & Hofer, S. (2005). Social context in gene environment interactions: Retrospect and prospect. Journal of Gerontology, 60B, 65 76. Shonkoff, J. (2010). Building a new biodevelopmental framework to guide the future of early childhood policy. Child Development, 81, 49 71, 357 367. Suomi, S. J. (2006). Risk, resilience, and gene environment interactions in rhesus monkeys. Annals of New York Academy of Sciences, 1094(1), 52 62.. All rights reserved under International Copyright Convention. No part of this text may be reproduced, transmitted, downloaded, or stored in or introduced into any information storage or retrieval system, in any form or by any means, whether electronic or mechanical, now known or hereinafter invented, without the written permission of The Guilford Press. Purchase this book now: www.guilford.com/p/calkins Guilford Publications 370 Seventh Avenue, Suite 1200 New York, NY 10001 212-431-9800 800-365-7006 www.guilford.com