SHORT NOTES I or we : Family socialization values in a national probability sample in Taiwan

Similar documents

The Individual-Oriented and Social- Oriented Chinese Bicultural Self: Testing the Theory

SELF and CULTURE What type of change?

SUPPLEMENTARY INFORMATION

Culture and Social Behavior, Self and Identity. Chapter 13. Defining the Concept of Self CULTURE AND THE CONCEPT OF SELF

In search of a new paradigm for cultural psychology

Psychological Experience of Attitudinal Ambivalence as a Function of Manipulated Source of Conflict and Individual Difference in Self-Construal

Problem with Cross-Cultural Comparison of User-Generated Ratings on Mechanical Turk

Emotion regulation and well-being among Puerto Ricans and European Americans

The Self Group Distinction Scale: A new approach to measure individualism and collectivism in adolescents

Questioning the utility of self efficacy measurements for Indians

Expressions of the Self in Individualistic vs. Collective Cultures: a cross-cultural-perspective teaching module

Emergence and composition of the traditional-modern bicultural self of people in contemporary Taiwanese societies

THE STRUCTURE AND MEASUREMENT OF SELF-CONSTRUALS: A CROSS- CULTURAL STUDY OF THE SELF-CONSTRUAL SCALE LILIA GABRIELA MIRAMONTES

Culturally Construed Dual Sources of Well-being: Preliminary Psychometric Evidences

Structural Validation of the 3 X 2 Achievement Goal Model

Toward the Theoretical Constructs of East Asian Cultural Psychology

Personality and beliefs about the world revisited: Expanding the nomological network of social axioms

Personality Traits Effects on Job Satisfaction: The Role of Goal Commitment

Validation of Intercultural Sensitivity Measure Individualism Collectivism

Validation of Intercultural Sensitivity Measure Individualism Collectivism

A STUDY OF THE IMPORTANCE AND IMPACT OF AUTONOMY ON THE MOTIVATION AND SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING OF BRITISH AND ECUADORIAN UNIVERSITY STUDENTS

Individualistic and Collectivistic Values, Age and Length of Residency Among Chinese Australian Immigrants

Research on Software Continuous Usage Based on Expectation-confirmation Theory

Effects of Cultural Adjustment on Academic Achievement of International Students

INTERDEPENDENCE AS A MEDIATOR BETWEEN CULTURE AND INTERPERSONAL CLOSENESS FOR EURO-CANADIANS AND TURKS

ROLE OF SOCIAL VALUES IN ENHANCING WELLBEING OF OLDER PEOPLE

Cultural Models and Implicit Theories of Self and World: A View from (Cross-)Cultural Psychology!

Test Bank. Chapter 2: The Cultural Context. Multiple Choice

Basic Needs and Well-Being: A Self-Determination Theory View

Running Head: SUBJECTIVE WELL-BEING 1

PSYCHOLOGY. The Psychology Major. Preparation for the Psychology Major. The Social Science Teaching Credential

A Cross-cultural Analysis of the Structure of Subjective Well-Being

Self-Related and Other-Related Pathways to Subjective Well-Being in Japan and the United States

Health Behavioral Patterns Associated with Psychologic Distress Among Middle-Aged Korean Women

Chapter Two: Cross-Cultural Research Methods

RUNNING HEAD: THE SOCIAL ORIENTATION HYPOTHESIS The Origin of Cultural Differences in Cognition: The Social Orientation Hypothesis

Kuusisto, E., Gholami, K., Schutte, I.W., Wolfensberger, M.V.C., & Tirri, K. (2014).

The Origin of Cultural Differences in Cognition: The Social Orientation Hypothesis

Poster Presentation at the Annual Meeting of the American Psychological Association. Toronto, August Running Head: CROSS-CULTURAL PERSPECTIVE

A TEST OF SHARKEY AND SINGELIS (1995) MODEL OF SELF-CONSTRUAL AND EMBARRASSABILITY: SITUATIONAL VERSUS DISPOSITIONAL FACTORS 1

Psychological needs. Motivation & Emotion. Psychological needs & implicit motives. Reading: Reeve (2015) Ch 6

2011 American Psychological Association 2011, Vol. 11, No. 6, /11/$12.00 DOI: /a BRIEF REPORT

Learning Objectives. Outline. The Dyadic Adjustment Scale: Utility for Rural Marital Assessment

International Conference on Humanities and Social Science (HSS 2016)

A Hierarchical Comparison on Influence Paths from Cognitive & Emotional Trust to Proactive Behavior Between China and Japan

NUMBER OF CHILDREN IN THE FAMILY AS A PREDICTOR OF PARENTS' LIFE SATISFACTION 1

The Family Allocentrism Idiocentrism Scale: Further Convergent Validity Exploration

Self-Construals in Situational Context: Disaggregating Behaviours and Intentions Using Sinha et al. s (2002) Decision-Making Scenarios

Fertility Transition and The Progression to A Third Birth in Turkey Sutay YAVUZ

Cultural Accommodation: The Effect of Language on the. Responses of Bilingual Hong Kong Chinese Managers

The validity and reliability of the Turkish version of the relational-interdependent self-construal scale

Neff, K. D., & Lamb, L. M. (2009). Self-Compassion. In S. Lopez (Ed.), The. Encyclopedia of Positive Psychology (pp ). Blackwell Publishing.

Development and Psychometric Properties of the Relational Mobility Scale for the Indonesian Population

LANGUAGE AND SELF-CONSTRUAL PRIMING A Replication and Extension in a Hong Kong Sample

An International Study of the Reliability and Validity of Leadership/Impact (L/I)

Relationship of Stress Coping Strategies and Life Satisfaction among Students

Correlates of Self-Construals among Asian and Caucasian Undergraduates in Canada

Teacher satisfaction: some practical implications for teacher professional development models

The Effects of Gender Role on Perceived Job Stress

PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE. Research Article

higher education in a changing world

International Core Journal of Engineering Vol.3 No ISSN:

Achievement Motivation and Academic Performance Among Turkish Early and Young Adolescents in the Netherlands

EMOTIONAL INTELLIGENCE COLLEGE AMONG TEACHERS IN COIMBATORE

WHAT IS SELF? MODULE-IV OBJECTIVES 16.1 CONCEPT OF SELF. What is Self? Self and Personality. Notes

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN LIFE SATISFACTION AND ATTACHMENT STYLES WITH PSYCHOLOGICAL WELL-BEING OF CHILDREN ON COVERED BY BEHZISTEY IN TEHRAN

Value Differences Between Scientists and Practitioners: A Survey of SIOP Members

Rapid decline of female genital circumcision in Egypt: An exploration of pathways. Jenny X. Liu 1 RAND Corporation. Sepideh Modrek Stanford University

SURVEY THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN IDENTITY STYLE AND MARITAL SATISFACTION

ACTIVITY: Exploring Cultural Identity

Learning and Individual Differences

Social Support as a Mediator of the Relationship Between Self-esteem and Positive Health Practices: Implications for Practice

In Pursuit of Happiness: the Cultural Psychological Study of SWB

International Journal of Health Sciences and Research ISSN:

social and emotional brain

Running head: CULTURE & SWB 1. Cultural Differences in Subjective Well-Being: How and Why. William Tov & Ze Ling Serene Nai

The Reliability and Validity of the Adolescent Subjective Well-Being Scale in Japan

Cultural Intelligence: A Predictor of Ethnic Minority College Students Psychological Wellbeing

3/24/09. The Happy Personality: A Meta-Analysis of 137 Personality Traits and Subjective Well-Being. Kristina M. DeNeve & Harris Cooper 1998

Differed Self within Collectivist Culture: Introducing an Alternative Self-Construal Theory

Fear of Happiness - By Hazal Muhtar Faculty Advisor: Dr. Patricia M. Rodriguez Mosquera

Practicing Counseling in Chinese Communities: Some Reflections on Cultural Competence and lndigenization. Patrick S. Y. Lau

THE IMPACT OF STRESS LEVEL AND THEIR COPING STRATEGIES: A COMPARATIVE STUDY ON FEMALE COLLEGE TEACHERS AND FEMALE HOME MAKERS

Culture and Survey Behavior

Autonomy and Relatedness among Chinese Sojourners and Applicants: Conflictual or Independent Predictors of Well-Being and Adjustment?

Predicting Undergraduate Students Metacognitive Skills with Self- Construal*

Scope insensitivity in helping decisions: Is it a matter of culture and values?

Group Assignment #1: Concept Explication. For each concept, ask and answer the questions before your literature search.

DEVELOPMENT AND VERIFICATION OF VALIDITY AND RELIABILITY OF

Is self-enhancement related to modesty or to individualism-collectivism? A test with four Israeli groups

The Mediating Role of Parental Expectations in Culture and Well-Being

A Study of Life Satisfaction and Optimism in Relation to Psychological Well-Being Among Working and Non Working Women

Aggregation of psychopathology in a clinical sample of children and their parents

Family Planning Programs and Fertility Preferences in Northern Ghana. Abstract

A study of association between demographic factor income and emotional intelligence

Culture, Self-construal and Social Cognition: Evidence from Cross-Cultural and Priming Studies

Original Papers. Emotional Frankness and Friendship in Polish Culture

Knowledge as a driver of public perceptions about climate change reassessed

Effectiveness of Parent Occupation on Mental Health, Self-Esteem, and Subjective Well-Being among College Students

Transcription:

Asian Journal of Social Psychology Asian Journal of Social Psychology (2009), 12, 145 150 DOI: 10.1111/j.1467-839X.2009.01276.x SHORT NOTES I or we : Family socialization values in a national probability sample in Taiwan Luo Lu Department of Business Administration, National Taiwan University, Taipei, Taiwan The aim of the present secondary data analysis was to explore antecedents and consequences of family socialization values emphasizing independence or interdependence, using a Taiwanese national probability sample. Analysis of variance revealed that those who were male, older and less educated emphasized greater interdependence values. In contrast, those who were younger, with higher social status and urban residents emphasized greater independence values. Multiple regression analysis further revealed that valuing interdependence was related to preferring a greater number of offspring, a higher endorsement of filial piety, greater marital and life satisfaction. Finally, in this national sample, endorsement on independence and interdependence values was equivalent.ajsp_ 145..150 Key words: Chinese, family socialization values, independence, interdependence, national survey. Introduction Following Hofstede s (1980) seminal work, the topic of individualism and collectivism (I/C) has been the focus of a great deal of zealous research activities in psychology. These research efforts have provided overwhelming evidence of behavioral differences between people from collectivist societies and those from individualist ones (Kim, Triandis, Kagitcibasi, Choi, & Yoon, 1994). The main difference across cultures concerns the basic unit of survival (Hui & Triandis, 1986). For individualists, the basic unit is the self, and the well-being of self is the main criterion of adaptation. For collectivists, the basic unit is a group or collectivity, and the welfare of the group is the highest social and moral value. Oyserman, Coon, and Kemmelmeier (2002) concluded in their meta-analysis that the core component of individualist value is valuing personal independence whereas those of collectivist value are sense of duty to group and relatedness to others. However, it is now generally agreed that I/C could be viewed as two cultural syndromes that may coexist within the same geopolitical locale and the same person. For instance, Tu (1991) used multiple modernity to denote the tradition-modern coexistence in Asia. Yang (1988) proposed limited convergence of modernity when he observed traditional elements coexisting with modern elements of the Chinese culture. More recently, Lu and Yang (2006) termed the emergent hybrid as tradition-modern biculturalism in contemporary Chinese societies, and provided Correspondence: Luo Lu, Department of Business Administration, National Taiwan University, No. 1, Sec. 4, Roosevelt Road, Taipei 106, Taiwan, ROC. Email: luolu@ntu.edu.tw Received 20 September 2007; accepted 25 June 2008. extensive evidence supporting their proposal. Such coexistence of tradition/modern or I/C values was also found among Indians (Sinha & Tripathi, 1994). The same was also true for Turks. Kagitçibasi (1990) proposed three models of family change: X, the collectivist model based on total interdependence; Z, the individualistic model based on total independence; and Y, a dialectical synthesis of the two. Viewing family as an integral part of socialization for the transmission of cultural values, Kagitçibasi (1996) further analyzed the meaning-making system of family socialization practices in the context of her three models. Specifically, in Model X, the control/ obedience orientation is the key and total interdependence is valued; in Model Z, the autonomy/self-reliance orientation is the key and total independence is valued. Somewhere between these two extremes, a Turkish study found the coexistence of independent and interdependent orientations in mothers childrearing values (Kagitcibasi, Sunar, & Bekman, 1988), thus supporting the Y model. However, systematic research on family socialization values has been rare (Miller, 1988). We thus conducted a secondary data analysis exercise to explore antecedents and consequences of family socialization values in terms of emphasizing independence or interdependence in the cultural context of a Chinese society (Taiwan) using a national probability sample. Valuing and socializing children in interdependence is in accordance with traditional Chinese collectivist culture, while emphasizing independence results from the modern Western individualistic influence. Following Lu and Yang s (2006) theory of tradition-modern biculturalism, we would expect that contemporary Taiwanese adopt both independence and interdependence family socialization values.

146 Luo Lu Furthermore, decades of research on Chinese traditionality/modernity traits has identified some demographical correlates (Yang, 1988; Lu & Kao, 2002; Lu & Gilmour, 2004; Kao & Lu, 2006). Specifically, we would expect that gender, age, education, social economic status (SES) and rural residents may be antecedents of independence and interdependence. Finally, there might be a linkage between cultural values and various family preferences. Scholars noted that children have an instrumental value as good workers in the collectivist cultures, and an expressive value in the individualist cultures (Kagitçibasi, 1990). In traditional collectivist Chinese societies, a larger family and more children are desirable, whereas filial piety is the most important family value to observe (Yang, 1981). More recent research further noted that cultural values people endorse might have an impact on their life adjustment and personal well-being. For instance, endorsing traditional Chinese values and interdependence were related to better adjustment among Taiwanese (Lu, Gilmour, & Kao, 2001; Lu & Gilmour, 2004). However, the adjustment value of endorsing modern traits such as independence is debatable and evidence inconclusive (Lu, 2006). We thus expected that desired number of offspring, endorsement of filial piety, marital satisfaction, and life satisfaction may be consequences of independence and interdependence. As this study is not a planned hypothesis testing study, only descriptive analyses were used. Method Data and participants Data for the present paper came from the 2006 Taiwan Social Change Survey (TSCS), which is the largest nationwide social survey in Taiwan. The TSCS series is operated by the Academia Sinica Taiwan, which has conducted 37 surveys as of 2006. With more than 80 000 interviews over the past 22 years, the TSCS has become the largest survey series among all of the general social surveys in the world (Smith, Kim, Koch, & Park, 2005, p. 74). Highly reputed for its methodological rigor (e.g. nationwide three-stage stratified probability proportionate to size (PPS) sampling using household registration data, well-trained interviewers making home visits, strict supervision, post-interview verification and data checking), its high-quality database is widely used for academic research and cross-cultural comparisons. The response rate for the 2006 survey was 45.7%. The current national sample comprised 2102 respondents. The entire sample was 50.2% male and 49.8% female, with a mean age of 45.96 (SD = 17.00, range = 20 93). Mean years of formal education was 11.79 (SD = 4.02). The majority (60.9%) was married, having on average 4.34 (SD = 1.90) persons in the household. Approximately half of our participants (50.8%) lived in cities of various sizes. Measures Questionnaires were given in face-to-face home interviews by trained interviewers. The author was a member of the TSCS research group, but the present paper is essentially based on a secondary data analysis. The data analysed in the present paper mainly came from the following parts of the survey. Family socialization values To correspond to our dual conceptualization of family socialization values, four items were developed to tap participants beliefs about child rearing. As Oyserman et al. (2002, p. 9) concluded in their comprehensive review on I/C-related values, the two most important individualist values are independence (freedom, self-sufficiency and control over one s life) and goals (striving for one s own goals, desires and achievements), whereas those of collectivist value are related (considering close others an integral part of the self) and duty (the duties and sacrifices that being a group member entails). We initially rephrased eight items to reflect child-rearing beliefs from established scales measuring I/C or independence/interdependence values (e.g. Hui, 1988; Singelis, 1994; Lu & Gilmour, 2007). After pre-test (N = 150) and interviews, four items were retained as they conveyed unambiguous meanings reported by our respondents. These are: we should teach children that family is more important; one should not put oneself before the family (interdependence-duty), It is more important to teach children to get along with other people harmoniously than to strive for one s own performance (interdependencerelated), Even if we don t agree with children s ideals, we should still encourage them to pursue them (independencegoals) and We should teach children to solve their own problems, not to rely on other people (independenceself-sufficiency). Seven-point rating scales were used (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree), with high scores representing high endorsement of interdependence or independence. Cronbach s a for the two-item independence scale was 0.55, and that for the two-item interdependence scale was 0.50 in the current sample. Family preferences and well-being In the survey, participants were asked to report their: (i) desired number of offspring; (ii) marital satisfaction (rated on: Overall, how satisfied are you with your marriage? 1 = completely dissatisfied, 5 = completely satisfied); (iii) life satisfaction (rated on: Overall, how satisfied are you

Family socialization values 147 with your life? 1 = completely dissatisfied, 5 = completely satisfied). Filial piety was measured by six items based on a dual model tapping authoritarian and reciprocal aspects (Yeh & Bedford, 2003). Seven-point rating scales were used (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree), with high scores representing high endorsement of filial piety. The internal consistency of this scale was 0.69 in the current sample. In addition, information on sex (coded male = 1, female = 2), age, marital status (coded married = 1, never married = 2), education attainment, income, SES (1 = lowest, 10 = highest), family size (number of people living in the household) and rural/urban residence (coded rural = 1, urban = 2) was recorded. Finally, as main research variables were measured by self-report, the social desirability bias needed to be controlled. Social desirability was measured by three items taken from the Crowne and Marlowe (1964) scale (1 = often, 4 = never), with high scores representing high susceptibility to social desirability. The internal consistency of this scale was 0.76 in the current sample. Results We first examined the conceptual validity of our Family Socialization Values Scale using exploratory factor analysis (EFA). With oblimin rotation, the EFA of four items supported a two-factor structure. The initial eigenvalues and amount of variance accounted for by the two factors were 1.83 (45.64%) and 0.81 (20.32%), respectively, together accounting for 65.96% of the variance. All items loaded on its designated subscale (factor) with loadings between 0.56 0.90. Thus, we confirmed the two-factor conceptual structure of independence/interdependence in this large national sample. A paired t-test was conducted examining within-subject differences between independence (mean = 11.75, SD = 1.70) and interdependence (mean = 11.73, SD = 1.80) scores. The finding was not significant (t(2094) = 0.49, ns), indicating that participants endorsed equally strongly on independence and interdependence values. A series of ancova were conducted, controlling for social desirability (as covariate). Findings are summarized in Table 1. We found that male, older and less educated people valued more interdependence, whereas younger people with higher SES and urban residents valued more independence. Pearson correlations were computed among main research variables, while again controlling for social desirability. Table 2 reports the full matrix of partial correlations. As independence and interdependence were significantly correlated (r = 0.44, p < 0.001), every correlation involving independence was computed controlling for interdependence (in addition to social desirability) and vice versa. This would allow us to obtain a clearer and cleaner pattern of associations. As such, higher interdependence significantly correlated with desiring a larger number of Table 1 Antecedents of independence and interdependence: ANCOVA results with social desirability as covariate Independence Interdependence Antecedents Mean SD Mean SD Male 11.76 1.72 11.82 1.75 Female 11.75 1.68 11.65 1.85 F for Sex (df) 0.02 (1,2095) 4.96* (1,2094) Younger (<45 years) 11.82 1.64 11.53 1.82 Older ( 45 years) 11.67 1.76 11.96 1.75 F for Age (df) 8.86** (1,2095) 4.46* (1,2094) (1) Junior high school and below 11.80 1.76 11.97 1.76 (2) High school 11.79 1.73 11.86 1.75 (3) College 11.81 1.70 11.63 1.90 F for Education (df) 1.48 (2,2085) 7.10*** (2,2086) Post-hoc: 1 > 2 > 3 Lower SES (<5) 11.67 1.87 11.69 1.75 Higher SES ( 5) 11.81 1.72 11.70 1.64 F for SES (df) 4.09* (1,2094) 3.47 (1,2095) Urban 11.82 1.62 11.59 1.74 Rural 11.78 1.68 11.61 1.77 F for Residence (df) 3.95* (1,2090) 0.83 (1,2090) (1) F for Social desirability was significant in each ancova. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001. SES, socio-economic status.

148 Luo Lu Table 2 Pearson correlations among main research variables: Controlling for social desirability, and for independence or interdependence as appropriate 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 1. Sex 1 2. Age -0.16 1 3. Education -0.07** -0.43*** 1 4. Family size -0.02-0.10*** -0.11*** 1 5. Income 0.03-0.22*** 0.30*** -0.02 1 6. SES 0.09** -0.15*** 0.29*** -0.06 0.35*** 1 7. Urban/rural -0.03-0.11*** 0.24*** -0.09*** 0.16*** 0.10** 1 8. Independence 0.04 0.08*** 0.05 0.01 0.01 0.09*** 0.08** 1 9. Interdependence -0.06** 0.10*** 0.11*** 0.05 0.01 (0.44***) 0.05 0.44*** 1 10. Desired no. offspring 0.02-0.18*** 0.14*** -0.06 0.06*** -0.01 0.06* -0.02 0.07* 1 11. Filial piety -0.11*** 0.20*** 0.24*** 0.07-0.13-0.04-0.11** 0.18*** 0.29*** 0.14*** 1 12. Marital satisfaction -0.11*** -0.06*** 0.08*** -0.00 0.11*** 0.11*** 0.01 0.06* 0.07* -0.01 0.10*** 1 13. Life satisfaction -0.13*** -0.06*** 0.10*** -0.04 0.27*** 0.22*** 0.00 0.07* 0.06* -0.01 0.06*** 0.44*** 1 *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Sex: 1, Male, 2, Female; Urban/rural: 1, Rural, 2, Urban. All correlations involving independence are partial correlations controlled for interdependence, and vice versa. Correlation between independence and interdependence (controlled for social desirability) is given in the parentheses. SES, socio-economic status. offspring, a higher endorsement of filial piety, and higher avowed marital and life satisfaction. It was interesting to note that independence produced a similar pattern of associations (with the exception of desired number of offspring), although the effect size was much smaller regarding filial piety. We further conducted hierarchical multiple regression analysis. By regressing family preferences and well-being on independence and interdependence, personal background variables (i.e. sex, age, education, income, SES, urban/rural residence) were entered into the equation first to control for their possible contributions. These demographic variables significantly correlated with family preferences and/or well-being variables (Table 2). For parsimony, every analysis was repeated taking out insignificant variables until all variables remaining in the final equation were significant predictors. Desired number of offspring, filial piety, marital satisfaction and life satisfaction were dependent variables in the four regressions reported in Table 3. As can been seen in Table 3, interdependence was associated with desiring a larger number of offspring, a higher endorsement of filial piety, higher avowed marital and life satisfaction, mirroring the pattern we found with correlation analyses. As for independence, it had a weak association with a higher endorsement of filial piety, again replicating the earlier correlation results. Discussion and conclusion Using a national probability sample, the primary aim of the present research was to explore antecedents and consequences of family socialization values in terms of emphasizing independence or interdependence in the cultural context of a Chinese society. Due cautions should be exercised regarding validity of the new Family Socialization Values Scale in this study. With tight restrictions on questionnaire length and interview time, no other existing scales of I/C or independence/interdependence were used in the survey, against which the conceptual validity of the new scale may be checked. However, our EFA results did confirm the underlying two-factor structure of independence/interdependence. Further evidence may be collected in the future. Although theorists have long been asserting that values and self-systems rooted in diverse cultural traditions can coexist within an individual (e.g. Kagitçibasi, 1990; Markus & Kitayama, 1991; Lu & Yang, 2006), the present study may be the first time that we obtained evidence showing that traditional and modern elements are now taking equal weight in the Chinese value system. This was observed in a nationwide representative sample, clearly demonstrating that Chinese people are now emphasizing both collectivist and individualistic values in socializing

Family socialization values 149 Table 3 Predicting family preferences and well-being Dependent variables Predictors b DR 2 R 2 F Desired no. offspring Age 0.25*** 0.07*** 0.07 Interdependence 0.07*** 0.01*** 0.08 55.63*** Filial piety Sex -0.10*** Age 0.18*** Education -0.16*** Income -0.06** 0.15*** 0.15 Independence 0.07*** Interdependence 0.29*** 0.10*** 0.25 97.14*** Marital satisfaction Sex -0.11*** SES 0.09** Income 0.08** 0.03*** 0.03 Interdependence 0.10*** 0.01*** 0.04 13.07*** Life satisfaction Sex -0.09*** SES 0.14*** Income 0.21*** 0.10*** 0.10 Interdependence 0.11*** 0.01*** 0.11 60.87*** **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001. Indicates a new step in hierarchical regression. b and F are taken from the final equation. SES, social economic status. their children, thus consolidating the foundation for a longlasting coexistence of traditional and modern rudiments in the society. This trend is especially pronounced among the young, well-educated, high SES, urban residents, as such is the demographic profile we identified for the prevalence of independence values. This profile also corroborates previous findings for the Chinese (Lu & Kao, 2002; Lu & Gilmour, 2004). It makes sense that for the young educated Chinese living in metropolitan cities, emphasizing independence, autonomy, active striving and personal achievement will promote adjustment to the industrial urban environment, hence such characteristics are more likely to be adopted as family socialization values to pass down the generations. Family socialization values were also found to be linked to family preferences and personal well-being. Interdependence representing core values of the Chinese cultural tradition was associated with desiring a bigger family and more willingness to adhere to filial piety, both are central features of the Chinese traditional family institution (Yang, 1981). More interestingly, interdependence also seems to hold the key for better adjustment in life. In the present study, interdependence was associated with higher marital and life satisfaction, partialling out contributions of other demographic factors such as SES and income. Although the effect size was not large (1% added explanatory power in both regressions), the finding corroborates a previously noted association between traditional Chinese values and better adjustment among Taiwanese (Lu et al., 2001; Lu & Gilmour, 2004). The value of tradition in a modern world is not to be overlooked. To conclude, in the present nationwide representative Taiwanese sample, we observed the equal strength of independence and interdependence as family socialization values. We further identified demographic profiles of favouring independence versus interdependence, and noted that independence/interdependence was linked to some indices of family preferences and personal wellbeing. Noting limits in the independence/interdependence measurement, the tentative conclusion is that bicultural identity in terms of family socialization values do exist among the Taiwanese. Documenting the existence of such bicultural identity and identifying its personal and institutional correlates are contributions of this study. Acknowledgement This 2006 TSCS research was supported by a grant from the National Science Council, Taiwan, ROC, NSC95-2420- H-001-006-B1. References Crowne, D. F. & Marlowe, D. (1964). The Approval Motive: Studies in Evaluative Dependence. New York: John Wiley.

150 Luo Lu Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture s Consequences: International Differences in Work-Related Values. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Hui, C. H. & Triandis, H. C. (1986). Individualism-collectivism: A study of cross-cultural researchers. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 17, 225 248. Hui, H. C. (1988). Measurement of individualism-collectivism. Journal of Research in Personality, 22, 17 36. Kagitçibasi, C. (1996). The autonomous-relational self: a new synthesis. European Psychologist, 1, 180 186. Kagitcibasi, C., Sunar, D. & Bekman, S. (1988). Comprehensive Preschool Education Project Final Report. Ottawa: IDRC. Kagitçibasi, C. (1990). Family and socialization in cross-cultural perspective: A model of change. In: J. Berman, ed. Nebraska Symposium on Motivation: Cross-Cultural Perspectives, pp. 135 200. Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Kao, S. F. & Lu, L. (2006). The relationship of conjugal congruence in psychological traditionality/modernity to marital adjustment. Indigenous Psychological Research in Chinese Societies, 25, 47 100. Kim, U., Triandis, H. C., Kagitcibasi, C., Choi, S. C. & Yoon, G. (1994). Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Application. London: Sage. Lu, L. (2006). Cultural fit: Individual and societal discrepancies in values, beliefs and SWB. Journal of Social Psychology, 146, 203 221. Lu, L. & Gilmour, R. (2004). Culture, self and ways to achieve SWB: A cross-cultural analysis. Journal of Psychology in Chinese Societies, 5, 51 79. Lu, L. & Gilmour, R. (2007). Developing a new measure of independent and interdependent views of the self. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 249 257. Lu, L. & Kao, S. F. (2002). Traditional and modern characteristics across the generations: Similarities and discrepancies. Journal of Social Psychology, 142, 45 59. Lu, L. & Yang, K. S. (2006). The emergence and composition of the traditional-modern bicultural self of people in contemporary Taiwanese societies. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 9, 167 175. Lu, L., Gilmour, R. & Kao, S. F. (2001). Culture values and happiness: An East-West dialogue. Journal of Social Psychology, 141, 477 493. Markus, H. R. & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implication for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224 253. Miller, S. (1988). Parents beliefs about children s cognitive development. Child Development, 59, 259 285. Oyserman, D., Coon, H. M. & Kemmelmeier, M. (2002). Rethinking individualism and collectivism: Evaluation of theoretical assumptions and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 3 72. Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 580 591. Sinha, D. & Tripathi, R. C. (1994). Individualism in a collectivist culture: A case of coexistence of opposites. In: U. Kim, H. C. Triandis, S. C. Choi & G. Yoon, eds. Individualism and Collectivism: Theory, Method, and Application, pp. 123 136. London: Sage. Smith, T. W., Kim, J., Koch, A. & Park, A. (2005). Social-science research and the General Social Surveys. ZUMA-Nachrichten, 56, 68 77. Tu, W. M. (1991). A Confucian perspective on global consciousness and local awareness. IHI Bulletin: A Quarterly Publication of the International House of Japan, 11, 1 5. Yang, K. S. (1981). Social orientation and individual modernity among Chinese students in Taiwan: Further empirical evidence. Journal of Social Psychology, 113, 159 170. Yang, K. S. (1988). Will society modernization eventually eliminate cross-culture psychological differences? In: M. H. Bond, ed. The Cross-Culture Challenge to Social Psychology, pp. 67 85. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. Yeh, K. H. & Bedford, O. (2003). A test of the dual filial piety model. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 6, 215 228.