Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens. The following table shows the percentage of male fairy-wrens in various age and social status categories.

Similar documents
Environmental Potential for Polygamy. Polygamy Favored. Do females always benefit from being mates of polygynous males? Resource Defense Polygyny

FILM SESSIONS. Film session I - Behaviour of Communication (September 30)

Biology 352, Spring 2017 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam

Anti-predator behavior: Group defense - Many eyes - Selfish herd - Dilution effects - Tonic immobility. Dispersal:

Mating systems and parental investment. Mating systems. Resource distribution. Polygyny. Pattern of matings in a population. Antithesis = promiscuity

Male parental effort and paternity in a variable mating system

r = intrinsic rate of natural increase = the instantaneous rate of change in population size (per individual). If r > 0, then increasing

Cost/benefit approach

Adaptation and Optimality Theory

Good resources. Why are females choosy? Good resources. Resources from males can tip the scales of relative investment

Good resources. Why are females choosy? Good resources. Good resources direct benefits Good genes indirect benefits

Lekking and the Lek Paradox

Bird Mating Systems/Sexual Selection

Evolution of Mating Systems. Chapter 8

Polygyny in animals From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Finding Mr. Right Featured scientist: Carrie Branch from University of Nevada Reno

Reproduction. Chapter 7

Effect of act on fitness of recipient

Some observations. Some traits are difficult to view as adaptations, because they appear to provide a disadvantage to the organism

April 12: Reproduction III: Female choice. Female choice

University of Groningen. The illusion of monogamy Bouwman, Karen Marian

BLY 122 Lecture Notes (O Brien) Chapter 47 Behavior

EnSt/Bio 295 Exam II This test is worth 100 points; you have approximately 50 minutes. Allocate your time accordingly.

The Gene Propagation Game

Reproducti e skew in birds: models, problems and prospects

All discussion of mating strategies and sex differences begins with Darwin s theory of Sexual Selection

3/26/ Sexual dimorphism is differences between males and females of a species. 2. Anisogamy. 1. Fecundity

UNIT 9. PARENTAL CARE AND MATING SYSTEMS

Evolutionary Forces. What changes populations?

Lectures 7 & 8 Wednesday, October 12, 2011 & Friday, October 14, 2011

Sexual selection. 1) Sexual dimorphism. 2) Variation in mating success. 3) Sexual selection. 4) Female choice based on male ornaments

(A) Drive (B) Innate behavior (C) Learning, based on experience (D) A & B (E) None of the above

in cooperatively breeding superb fairy-wrens Malurus cyaneus

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 12 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2009 We want to understand the reasons

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2008 As we have seen before, the bottom line

Take a look at the three adult bears shown in these photographs:

MECHANISMS AND PATTERNS OF EVOLUTION

Sex Determination. Male = XY. Female = XX. 23 pairs of chromosomes (22 autosomes/body chromosomes, 1 sex)

Biology of Breeding: Considerations for maximizing genetic diversity of breeding groups

The behavioral ecology of animal reproduction

POST-ALLOGROOMING REDUCTIONS IN SELF-DIRECTED BEHAVIOUR ARE AFFECTED BY ROLE AND STATUS IN THE GREEN WOODHOOPOE

Testing the function of petal-carrying in the Red-backed Fairy-wren (Malurus melanocephalus)

Sexual selection and the evolution of sex differences

Empirical testing of evolutionary hypotheses has used to test many theories both directly and indirectly. Why do empirical testing?

This question is taken directly from the list of second test study questions (#6) it should not be a surprise...

Inheritance. Children inherit traits from both parents.

Eutherian Mammals Hamadryas Baboon

A n t h r o p o l o g y

Dragon Genetics. Essential Question How does Mendelian genetics explain the variation of expressed traits within a population?

Writing Assignment 1

Biology 352, Spring 2018 Exam Number KEY Second midterm exam Part 1 (short answer worth 21 % of grade)

NCEA Science 1.9 Genetic Variation AS 90948

Learning Objectives (Davies et al. 2012, Table 9.3)

POLYANDRY AND INCEST AVOIDANCE IN THE COOPERATIVE STRIPE-BACKED WREN OF VENEZUELA

Introduction to Biological Anthropology: Notes 13 Mating: Primate females and males Copyright Bruce Owen 2010 We want to understand the reasons

Introduction to Neuroscience: Behavioral Neuroscience Lecture 1: Introduction to Animal Behavior

Biology 321 QUIZ#3 W2010 Total points: 20 NAME

Types of behaviors that are elicited in response to simple stimuli

ESRM 350 Reproduction and Mating Systems

Kin recognition plays multiple roles across the animal kingdom. From an

Making Decisions in the Family: An Evolutionary Perspective

B. male gametes that may be carried by the wind

Reproduction in Plants and Animals

Behavior Genetics and Evolutionary Psychology. Module 5

12/3/2012. I. Benefits of mate choice. A. Direct benefits. Examples of Female Choice. Mechanisms of Sexual Selection. A.

Rare ectotherm biparental care. Who cares? Determining optimal parental investment per offspring Trade offs:

Animal Behaviour 82 (2011) 437e444. Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Animal Behaviour. journal homepage:

Animal Behavior 2/21/2017. What is Behavior? Understanding Behavior. Types of Behavior. Types of Behavior

Genetics Review. Alleles. The Punnett Square. Genotype and Phenotype. Codominance. Incomplete Dominance

4.2 Altruism -'1 I. 44 SECTION2 Evolutionand BehavioralEcology

Genetic Variation Junior Science

Section 8.1 Studying inheritance

Gene Combo SUMMARY KEY CONCEPTS AND PROCESS SKILLS KEY VOCABULARY ACTIVITY OVERVIEW. Teacher s Guide I O N I G AT I N V E S T D-65

PDF hosted at the Radboud Repository of the Radboud University Nijmegen

Ecological benefits of cooperation. Life Insurance. Life Insurance. Fortress defense

disadvantages of sexual reproduction Only 50% of your genome is in your offspring.

Cetacean Social & Reproductive Systems

Lecture K3- Behavioral Ecology Dr. Kopeny

Pinniped Social Systems

Supporting Online Material for

3. What law of heredity explains that traits, like texture and color, are inherited independently of each other?

Male Acadian flycatchers, Empidonax virescens, obtain extrapair fertilizations with distant females

Complex Traits Activity INSTRUCTION MANUAL. ANT 2110 Introduction to Physical Anthropology Professor Julie J. Lesnik

Patterns of Reproductive Skew in the Polygynandrous Acorn Woodpecker

MALE BEHAVIOR AND FEMALE RECRUITMENT IN THE RED-WINGED BLACKBIRD

Behavioral Animal Adaptations. Survival of organisms

The Effects of Water Temperature on Mate Choice For Convict Cichlids (A. Nigrofaciata)

Spatial patterns of extra-pair paternity: beyond paternity gains and losses

Genetic basis of inheritance and variation. Dr. Amjad Mahasneh. Jordan University of Science and Technology

Myers Psychology for AP* David G. Myers PowerPoint Presentation Slides by Kent Korek Germantown High School Worth Publishers, 2010

Title. Ornithorhynchus anatinus. Author. Lauren Bucchino

The Chromosomal Basis Of Inheritance

Who cares? Effect of coping style and social context on brood care and defense in superb fairy-wrens

biology Slide 1 of 32 End Show Copyright Pearson Prentice Hall

Time allowed: 2 hours Answer ALL questions in Section A, ALL PARTS of the question in Section B and ONE question from Section C.

Chapter 7: Pedigree Analysis B I O L O G Y

Assessment Schedule 2015 Biology: Demonstrate understanding of genetic variation and change (91157)

Extra-Pair Mating and Evolution of Cooperative Neighbourhoods

2. Was there a scientific way to predict the outcome of a cross between two parents?

Name Date Class. Main Idea. Human traits are controlled by single genes with two alleles, single genes with... a. b. c.

Transcription:

Social System of Superb Fairy Wrens Superb fairy-wrens are small (10g) insectivorous birds found in woodlands and edge habitat throughout eastern Australia. They live in cooperative social groups composed of a single breeding female, her social mate, and often one to four adult male helpers which defend a permanent territory. The social mate is behaviorally dominant over any other males (helpers) on his territory. Although females build the nest and incubate eggs without assistance, males contribute to next defense and feeding of young. All helpers assist with feeding and care of young. It is important to note that because superb fairy-wrens are long-lived and territorial, breeding vacancies are rare enough that females are generally unable to choose their social partner. There are three categories of males: paired dominant males without helpers, dominant males with one or more helpers, and helpers. Regardless of their social status, males become sexually active at age one when they develop adult breeding plumage and the ability to fertilize females. In addition to their complex social system, superb fairy-wrens are unique because they exhibit the highest known rates of extra-pair fertilization, a whopping 76%. This tutorial challenges you to identify some of the selective forces behind this social system. Male Status The following table shows the percentage of male fairy-wrens in various age and social status categories. TABLE 1. Percentage of male fairy-wrens in various age and social status categories, 1991 and 1992 seasons. Total n equals 135 male-seasons. Age (years) Dominant with helper Unassisted dominant Helper 1 0.0 (0) 6.7 20.7 2 4.4 7.4 13.3 3 7.4 8.2 3.0 4 5.2 3.7 0.0 (0) > 5 11.9 6.7 1.5 1. What determines a male s social status? Why might this be the case? Why do helpers help? (Part I) The following table maps the effect of group size on reproductive success in a territory. TABLE 2. The effect of group size on reproductive success in a territory, 1991/1992 and 1992/1993 seasons Pairs Groups Group size effect Average number of fledglings 3.2 3.2 p =.97 1. Does the presence of helpers increase reproductive success in a territory? Average number of young surviving to 4 weeks Probability of fledgling no young 2.4 2.3 p =.63.26.20 p =.91

Page 2 Who are helpers helping? This table contains information about the relatedness of helpers to nestlings. TABLE 1. Percentage of help directed at relatives and non relatives, split by helper age. Relationship of helpers to nestlings 1 year old 2+ years First-order relatives ( r = 0.5 ) 35 % 23 % Second-order relatives ( r =.25) 46 % 23 % Unrelated young ( r <.25 ) 19 % 54 % Totals 100 % 100 % 1. Do you think that the levels of relatedness between helpers and nestlings is enough to be the sole reason that helpers stay and help instead of breeding on their own? Why might relatedness decrease as the helper gets older? 2. How might helpers deal with the problem of having some related young and some unrelated young in the nest they are helping? Remember that every time a helper feeds an unrelated young this is an evolutionary waste of energy. Feeding rate of helpers in relation to their mean relatedness to the young. 4. Do you see any correlation in the graph? Are helpers adjusting their feeding rates according to their relatedness to the young? Should they? Figure 1. The feeding rate of helpers in relation to their mean relatedness to the young they provisioned. Crosses represent nests with at least one young sired by the helper, open circles denote no paternity. Deletion of the arrowed point eliminates the weak negative correlation.

Page 3 Why do dominant males prefer or tolerate the presence of helpers? 1. Why do you think dominant males would choose to allow helpers on his territory? Remember that the presence of helpers does not increase the reproductive success in a territory. Paternity by the dominant male in his own nest is affected by the presence of helpers. 2. What clear correlation do you see? How do helpers affect the dominant male s paternity? From the above graphs, should dominant males accept helpers or not? Behavior of dominant males with and without helpers. Figure 1. Number of nest visits made by dominant males during a 30 min period in relation to the number of helpers. Figure 2. Probability that a dominant male will depart on an extra-group foray during a 30 min period in relation to the number of helpers, and the reproductive status of his mate.

Page 4 3. How is the behavior of dominant males affected by helpers? How could these behaviors be advantageous for the dominant male? 4. When the dominant male is not actively attending the nest, what do you think he is doing? How can he best spend the free time he gains from the presence of helpers? The following graphs give us insight into which males gain extra-pair fertilizations. 5. Which males are getting the most extra-pair fertilizations? Refer to the table about male status on page one. Fig. 3. (A) The effect of age on the probability of obtaining extra-group fertilization. (B) The effect of social status on the proportion of males obtaining an extra-group fertilization. Numbers indicate total males in each social category. 6. What proportion of the males sired extra-group offspring? Are the numerous extra group offspring being fathered by many different males or a select few? Here is the frequency with which males attain extra-pair fertilizations. Treat the shaded bars the same as white bars for now.

Page 5 Now for the shaded bars. One particular male in the study, identified by the researchers as BRY, was quite successful in his attempts to gain extra-pair fertilizations. This male fathered six young outside his group, and two generations of his descendents (a within-group son, an extra-group son, and an extra-group grandson) sired an additional 12 extra-group young. The shaded bars in the figure above identify males belonging to this illustrious BRY patriline. 7. In considering this information, what can you say about the tendency to sire extra-group young? Is it directly related to age and status, or are there more factors? Is this tendency inherited from the father or learned from the father? This table should answer an important question. In this study, the paternity of each offspring was determined (whether it was within-group or extra-group). This allowed for the calculation of a male s total reproductive success (inside and outside the group). Male survival was also calculated. Table 1. Average reproductive success and survival from one breeding season to the next (%) of dominant males living in four social categories: (0) pairs without helpers; (1) groups with a single helper; (2+) groups with two or more helpers; and (solitary) living without a female, or with a female which did not breed. Social category of the dominant male 0 1 2+ solitary significance Fledglings 2.28 3.00 3.43 p < 0.001 Independent young 1.66 2.21 2.46 p < 0.001 Male survival 70.9 75.3 78.7 71.4 not significant 8. What benefits do males gain from the presence of helpers? In the case of reproductive success, where are these benefits taking place (inside our outside his territory)? Why do helpers help? (Part II)

Page 6 Why do young males usually stay at home and become helpers? Two hypotheses are the limited-mates hypothesis and the habitat-saturation hypothesis. In both cases, a necessary resource for successful breeding is limited to the point that males will not disperse due to a lack of this resource. In this study, the dominant male was removed from a pair (no helpers), thus presenting neighboring helper males with an opportunity to disperse to an available habitat and female. TABLE 1 Disposition of non-breeding males in superb fairy-wren groups Category Number Number of potential dispersers capable of dispersing given the opportunity 33 Number of potential dispersers above - that dispersed given the opportunity 31 - that did not disperse given the opportunity 2 1. What does this data suggest about the reason that males do not disperse and instead become helpers? This table presents data about dominant males responses to helper males who were absent from their territory. Aggression is characterized by extreme harassment by the dominant male, in the form of prolonged chases lasting up to 7 minutes. Table 1. Responses of dominant male superb fairy-wrens to the return of removed helper males in the first half-hour following the helper s return Stage of nesting N Aggression % Number of min. (out of 30) during which dominant male exhibited aggressive behavior Mean chase duration in seconds Non-breeding 11 0 0 0 0 Fertilization period 4 2 50 5.1 138 Incubation 4 2 50 9 142 2. In addition to the reasons noted so far, why do helpers help? When does the dominant male respond most aggressively to a helper s departure? Why do you think he responds this way?