The role of the hippocampus in organic amnesia

Similar documents
The role of the human medial temporal lobe in object recognition and object discrimination

Serial model. Amnesia. Amnesia. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. Prof. Stephan Anagnostaras. Lecture 3: HM, the medial temporal lobe, and amnesia

Morris water maze: standard test for spatial memory in rodents

Henry Molaison. Biography. From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

NST II Psychology NST II Neuroscience (Module 5)

Memory. Psychology 3910 Guest Lecture by Steve Smith

HBEV: Non-Print Items

The perceptual-mnemonic/feature conjunction model of perirhinal cortex function

Brain Imaging Applied to Memory & Learning

Memory Impairment in Monkeys Following Lesions Limited to the Hippocampus

Chapter 17 Suggested Readings. Borsutzky, S., Fujiwara, E., Brand, M., & Markowitsch, H. J. (2008). Confabulations in

A systems neuroscience approach to memory

Against memory systems

October 2, Memory II. 8 The Human Amnesic Syndrome. 9 Recent/Remote Distinction. 11 Frontal/Executive Contributions to Memory

10/24/2017. Medial Temporal Lobes. Autobiographical Memory. Episodic and Semantic Memory. Arlo Clark-Foos, Ph.D.

Arlo Clark-Foos, Ph.D.

Remembering the Past to Imagine the Future: A Cognitive Neuroscience Perspective

ORGANIC AMNESIA: A DIVERSITY IN DEFICITS Christopher R. Madan Department of Psychology, University of Alberta

CSE511 Brain & Memory Modeling Lect 22,24,25: Memory Systems

THE MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE

Intact Visual Perceptual Discrimination in Humans in the Absence of Perirhinal Cortex

Systems Neuroscience November 29, Memory

Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory

Linda Lušić Research fellow Department of neuroscience School of medicine. University of Split

CASE 49. What type of memory is available for conscious retrieval? Which part of the brain stores semantic (factual) memories?

PERCEPTUAL AND MNEMONIC MATCHING-TO-SAMPLE IN HUMANS: CONTRIBUTIONS OF THE HIPPOCAMPUS, PERIRHINAL AND OTHER MEDIAL TEMPORAL LOBE CORTICES

Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Word Memory Test Performance in Amnesic Patients With Hippocampal Damage

The hippocampus and memory: a comparative and ethological perspective Wendy A Suzuki* and Nicola S Clayton

When Amnesic Patients Perform Well on Recognition Memory Tests

C. Brock Kirwan, Ph.D.

Neuropsychologia 48 (2010) Contents lists available at ScienceDirect. Neuropsychologia

Impaired Transverse Patterning in Human Amnesia Is a Special Case of Impaired Memory for Two-Choice Discrimination Tasks

Frontal Contributions to Memory Encoding Before and After Unilateral Medial Temporal Lobectomy

Perirhinal Cortex Ablation Impairs Visual Object Identification

Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system

Cross-Modal Matching Performance in Amnesia

The Neurophysiology of Memory

The motor regulator. 2) The cerebellum

Memory and the Hippocampus: A Synthesis From Findings With Rats, Monkeys, and Humans

Detection and Explanation of Sentence Ambiguity Are Unaffected by Hippocampal Lesions but Are Impaired by Larger Temporal Lobe Lesions

Report. The Hippocampus Supports Recognition Memory for Familiar Words but Not Unfamiliar Faces

The perirhinal cortex and long-term familiarity memory

Two cortical systems for memoryguided

The motor regulator. 2) The cerebellum

Structure and function of declarative and nondeclarative memory systems

Impaired Recency Judgments and Intact Novelty Judgments after Fornix Transection in Monkeys

Behavioural Brain Research

Learning and Memory. The Case of H.M.

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Cellular Neurobiology BIPN140

Learning and Generalization Deficits in Patients With Memory Impairments Due to Anterior Communicating Artery Aneurysm Rupture or Hypoxic Brain Injury

The animal model of human amnesia: Long-term memory impaired and short-term memory intact

MNEMONIC ROLE OF THE FORNIX: INSIGHTS FROM THE MACAQUE MONKEY BRAIN

Confusional state. Digit Span. Mini Mental State Examination MMSE. confusional state MRI

THE USE OF ANIMAL MODELS TO STUDY THE EFFECTS OF AGING ON COGNITION

Neuroscience of Consciousness II

Perceptual deficits in amnesia: challenging the medial temporal lobe mnemonic view

Acquisition and retention of visual discrimination learning after ablation of perirhinal cortex in the rat

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Perceptual Learning, Awareness, and the Hippocampus

Recognition Memory for Single Items and for Associations Is Similarly Impaired Following Damage to the Hippocampal Region

Ch 8. Learning and Memory

Category-Specific Item Recognition and the Medial Temporal Lobe

Brain Mapping of Episodic Memory in Patients with Medial Temporal Lobe Epilepsy Using Activation Positron Emission Tomography

Contrasting Effects on Discrimination Learning after Hippocampal Lesions and Conjoint Hippocampal Caudate Lesions in Monkeys

Theories of memory. Memory & brain Cellular bases of learning & memory. Epileptic patient Temporal lobectomy Amnesia

Title: Biological Approach 1.2 Methods of the Biological Approach How?

The Hippocampus Supports both the Recollection and the Familiarity Components of Recognition Memory

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Annu Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2007 November 7.

Brain Mechanisms of Memory and Cognition 5 Neural basis of memory (2): multiple memory systems

Deficits in Attentional Orienting Following Damage to the Perirhinal or Postrhinal Cortices

Comparison of explicit and incidental learning strategies in memory-impaired patients. Results. Significance

Specialization in the Medial Temporal Lobe for Processing of Objects and Scenes

The possible contribution of the amygdala to memory

Systems Neuroscience Dan Kiper. Today: Wolfger von der Behrens

Recognition memory for faces and scenes in amnesia: Dissociable roles of medial temporal lobe structures

NIH Public Access Author Manuscript Annu Rev Neurosci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2012 January 1.

Chapter 2: Studies of Human Learning and Memory. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Chapter 2: Studies of Human Learning and Memory. From Mechanisms of Memory, second edition By J. David Sweatt, Ph.D.

Effect of Unitization on Associative Recognition in Amnesia

Contrasting hippocampal and perirhinal cortex function using immediate early gene imaging

Remember/Know Judgments Probe Degrees of Recollection

The contribution of the human medial temporal lobe to perception: Bridging the gap between animal and human studies

Dissociation in retrograde memory for object discriminations and object recognition in rats with perirhinal cortex damage

Recollection and familiarity in dense hippocampal amnesia: A case study

The Three Amnesias. Russell M. Bauer, Ph.D. Department of Clinical and Health Psychology. College of Public Health and Health Professions

More dendritic spines, changes in shapes of dendritic spines More NT released by presynaptic membrane

Neuroanatomy of Memory

Introduction to Physiological Psychology Learning and Memory II

Prof. Saeed Abuel Makarem & Dr.Sanaa Alshaarawy

NEUROANATOMY OF MEMORY

Human Amnesia and Animal Models of Amnesia: Performance of Amnesic Patients on Tests Designed for the Monkey

Object memory and perception in the medial temporal lobe: an alternative approach Timothy J Bussey and Lisa M Saksida

Neural Correlates of Temporal Context Retrieval. Fang Wang. Thesis submitted to the faculty of the

DEFINING EMOTION 11/19/2009 THE BIOLOGY OF EMOTION & STRESS. A change in physiological arousal, ranging from slight to intense.

INVESTIGATIONS OF THE NEURAL BASIS OF SOURCE MEMORY STRENGTH BRION S WOROCH DISSERTATION

Chapter 4. Long-term memory and the medial temporal lobe

An evaluation of the concurrent discrimination task as a measure of habit learning: performance of amnesic subjects

Lag-Sensitive Repetition Suppression Effects in the Anterior Parahippocampal Gyrus

Transcription:

The role of the hippocampus in organic amnesia June 10, 2014 Abstract Organic amnesia is a neurological disorder characterised by severe impairment in episodic memory. In the past, hippocampal damage has been assumed to be both necessary and sufficient to cause organic amnesia, however more recent research refutes this, instead suggesting the contribution of an array of brain processes. The anatomically narrow research focus on the hippocampus must now be revised in order to advance our understanding of amnesia, and the workings of human memory. One structure alone does not explain the diversity of research findings in the laying down of memories. We must look beyond the hippocampus in the search for better ways to find memories that have been lost. Keywords Amnesia, memory, hippocampus, neuropsychology, cholinergic processes 1

Organic amnesia is a neurological disorder characterised by severe memory impairment. Understanding amnesia would not only allow for the development of effective rehabilitation procedures for amnesic individuals, but would vastly extend our knowledge regarding the psychological and neurological underpinnings of memory. Whilst neuropsychological research is characterised by exploration and examination of the functions of various brain regions, in the past amnesia research relied heavily on investigation of a single brain region: the hippocampus. Recent evidence suggests that the hippocampus may not play such an independently causal role in amnesia as was once thought. As such, the anatomically narrow focus of previous research may have hindered the advancement of our understanding of both the psychological and neurological framework that underlies organic amnesia. Perhaps the most well-known case study in neuropsychology is that of Henry Molaison (H.M.). Scoville and Milner (1957) reported that subsequent to surgery involving bilateral removal of the hippocampus to relieve his severe epilepsy, patient H.M. exhibited extensive anterograde amnesia. In contrast, patients with minor hippocampal damage showed fewer or no memory impairments. This research implicated the hippocampus as the determining factor of severe amnesia. Whilst this revolutionary work had extensive benefits in terms of establishing theories of modularity, and substantiating the distinction between declarative and procedural memory (Squire & Wixted, 2011), it resulted in amnesia becoming intrinsically linked with this single brain structure (Bussey, 2004; Warburton & Brown, 2010). Subsequent years saw the publication of a plethora of experiments, case studies and models of memory focussing on the hippocampus alone (Cohen & Eichenbaum, 1993; Rempel-Clower, Zola, Squire, & Amaral, 1996; Redish, 2001). The vast literature on the role of the hippocampus in organic amnesia also stems from the availability of research participants. Patients with hippocampal damage are relatively common due to its susceptibility to various pathologies including oxygen deprivation resulting from ischemia, anoxia or stroke, encephalitis, viral infection and temporal lobe surgery(spiers, Maguire, & Burgess, 2001). As a result, the effects of hippocampal lesions in vivo can be studied far more readily than lesions of other brain regions that may play a role in amnesia. Whilst it is undoubtedly true that the hippocampus plays a role in memory, the extent to which this single brain structure is responsible for amnesia has been fundamentally overstated. Despite evidence from non-human animal research that hippocampal lesions did not result in the severe amnesia observed in H.M. (Mahut, 1971; Mishkin, 1978; Murray and Mishkin, 1986), only rarely did researchers challenge the hippocampus supremacy (Horel, 1978; Vanderwolf & Cain, 1994). However, several decades after the initial report of H.M. by Scoville and Milner (1957), Corkin, Amaral, Gonzlez, Johnson, and Hyman (1997) demonstrated using magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) not only that the damage in H.M. s brain extended far beyond the hippocampus to the adjacent rhinal cortices and the surrounding white matter, but also that the caudal section of the hippocampus contrarily remained largely intact. The presence of this extensive damage was subsequently verified by a post-mortem of H.M. s brain (Annese et al., 2014). More recently, reports of patients with localised hippocampal damage have indicated less dense amnesia (Spiers, Maguire, & Burgess, 2001), suggesting that the additional extra-hippocampal damage in H.M. may have significantly contributed to the severity of his memory impairments. As such, the case of H.M. can no longer be classed as an 2

example of a selective hippocampal lesion that resulted in amnesia, as was so often stated in the past, and does not imply that hippocampal damage is either necessary or sufficient to cause organic amnesia. Despite this, H.M. is still used erroneously in the literature as evidence of amnesia resulting from purely hippocampal damage (Squire & Wixted, 2011). Another line of convincing evidence suggesting damage to the hippocampus alone could cause amnesia came from patients with ischemic damage (Rempel-Clower et al., 1996). Ischemic individuals such as R.B. (Zola-Morgan, Squire, & Amaral, 1986), who developed amnesia subsequent to oxygen deficits in the brain, endured restricted damage to the CA1 subfield of the hippocampus. The resulting severe amnesia was therefore seen as hippocampal dependent. However, more recent evidence suggests hidden pathology resulting from ischemia may also contribute to the impairment (Bachevalier & Meunier, 1996; Markowitsch, Weber-Luxemburger, Ewald, Kessler, & Heiss, 1997). This can include damage to the rhinal cortices, thalamus, cerebellum, and the amygdala (Smith, Auer, & Siesj, 1984). As such, even ischemic damage that is often used in arguments defending the hippocampus, does not provide evidence of an exclusive hippocampal role in amnesia. This also emphasises the problem arising from reliance upon clinical studies, which are so often used to implicate the hippocampus in amnesia. Supporting the idea that the hippocampus is not an all-encompassing memory structure, and thus is not the sole cause of amnesia, is evidence demonstrating that memory deficits can occur independently of hippocampal damage. The first observations of amnesia based on loss of a structure other than the hippocampus occurred in patients with Korsakoff syndrome (Pergola & Suchan, 2013). Such patients suffer from thiamine deficiency, often as a result of excessive alcohol consumption, which can cause damage in the thalamus and mammillary bodies, as seen in patient P.Z. (Butters & Cermak, 1988). Amnesia can also result from thalamic stroke (Carlesimo, Lombardi, & Caltagirone, 2011), firmly implicating the thalamus in amnesia. Another example of amnesia subsequent to non-hippocampal damage comes from individuals with visual memory deficit amnesia (VMDA), resulting from damage to the visual association cortices (Rubin & Greenberg, 1998). Such patients show mild anterograde, and severe retrograde amnesia. Although these symptoms differ in severity to those found in hippocampal patients, it is evident that the hippocampus is not necessarily responsible for all cases of amnesia. Several other regions have also been implicated in amnesia such as the rhinal cortices (Meunier, Bachevalier, Mishkin, & Murray, 1993). Due to their neural connections to the hippocampus such regions have previously been thought to be of secondary importance, however it has recently been postulated that these structures provide novel information that is essential for memory function, thus reversing the conventional hierarchy topped by the hippocampus (Vann, 2010; Aggleton et al., 2013). A lack of research in the past into such regions as a result of the hesitancy regarding their importance (Vann, 2010) has prevented advancement in the understanding of structures which may contribute to amnesia. The typical concept of classic anterograde amnesia is that it involves loss of both recall and recognition memory (Brown & Aggleton, 2001; Warburton & Brown, 2010). Due to the hippocampal focus, it was inevitably assumed that both processes were hippocampal dependent. In the past, studies investigating the effects of hippocampal lesions utilised surgical aspiration lesions, which necessarily involves the removal of rhinal cortices (Baxter & Murray, 2001). A resultant deficit in recognition memory strengthened 3

the view that it was a hippocampal dependent process. Whilst this idea is still embraced by some(squire, Wixted, & Clark, 2007), double dissociations subsequent to lesions of the hippocampus and perirhinal cortices suggest this assumption was ungrounded (Easton & Parker, 2003). A meta-analysis completed by Baxter and Murray (2001) found that after perirhinal cortices damage, subsequent damage to the hippocampus was inversely correlated with recognition memory as assessed by a delayed-non-match-to-sample (DNMS) task: whilst perirhinal damage impairs recognition memory, additional hippocampal damage has a protective effect. This contradicts the view that greater damage of the medial temporal lobe memory system results in more severe amnesia (Zola-Morgan, Squire, & Ramus, 1994), instead suggesting that recall and recognition are independent processes, reliant on differential brain regions rather than the hippocampus alone. Evidence opposing the hippocampus role in recognition comes from patients with fornix damage (Easton & Parker, 2003). Whilst such patients show impaired episodic memory, recognition memory remains unimpaired (Aggleton et al., 2000). Furthermore, patient K.N. presented by Aggleton et al. (2005) was unimpaired on tests of recognition despite having bilateral damage to the hippocampus, further suggesting the hippocampus is important in recall alone, with its role in other memory domains having been overemphasised in the past. Aggleton and Brown (1999) propose two dissociable systems to account for the explicit recollective and familiarity components of recognition memory, postulating that whilst the former relies on the hippocampus, the latter relies upon a system involving the perirhinal cortices and the thalamus. According to this view, hippocampal damage does not necessarily cause impairment of recognition tasks. This duel process model has been largely supported by animal studies (Warburton & Brown, 2010), as well as clinical and neuroimaging research with human participants (Brown, Warburton, & Aggleton, 2010). This demonstrates how models incorporating extra-hippocampal regions can convincingly explain some aspects of amnesia. However, the underlying neural processes remain elusive. One problem that still plagues the literature since the advocation of the hippocampus as the essential memory structure, is the resulting emphasis on a single or set of structures, rather than processes in the majority of subsequent research on amnesia. Several decades ago a theory presented by Horel(1978) broke this trend, but was quickly discounted(zola- Morgan, Squire, & Mishkin, 1982), and subsequently ignored at the time. His research has resurfaced in the literature in more recent years (Gaffan, Parker, & Easton, 2001). Horel (1978) attributed amnesia to damage of the white matter in the temporal stem, which could prevent cholinergic medial temporal lobe input (Easton & Parker, 2003). Gaffan, Parker, and Easton (2001) proposed that the removal of cholinergic cells, which form the basis of a transmitter system between the medial temporal lobe, and frontal and inferior temporal cortices, would result in dense amnesia akin to that displayed by H.M. Indeed, this was subsequently shown to be the case in monkeys (Easton, Ridley, Baker, & Gaffan, 2002; Turchi, Saunders, & Mishkin, 2005), however research with rats yielded divergent results(easton, Fitchett, Eacott, & Baxter, 2011). Whilst it still remains unclear whether the cholinergic process plays a vital role in amnesia, such work has combined study of areas outside of the hippocampus with that of a process, thus advancing the field of amnesic research, overcoming a previous fixation on brain structure and furthering our endeavour to gain a greater understanding of organic amnesia. 4

Recent technological advances have allowed for quicker research progression, with more reliable and detailed neuroimaging available for human participants, and more precise lesioning techniques for experiments with non-human animals. For example the role of extra-hippocampal areas in amnesia have recently been supported using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fmri), which implicate both the prefrontal and retrosplenial cortices in episodic memory (Pergola & Suchan, 2013). As such, researchers are beginning to focus their research on areas beyond the hippocampus (Vann, 2010; Eichenbaum, 2013; Pergola, Ranft, Mathias, & Suchan, 2013), and are furthermore broadening the understanding of the functional relationships between structures, rather than just the function of the structures themselves (Allen & Fortin, 2013), and thus acknowledging amnesia has no independent causal structure but instead involves a number of different networks throughout the brain. Retrospectively, it now seems remarkable that researchers once attributed such a complex neurological disorder as organic amnesia to a single brain structure, and this realisation demonstrates the extensive increase in understanding we now have of the disorder. However, in order to overcome this difficultly in its entirety it is important that traditional views of amnesia are tempered (Maguire & Mullally, 2013). It is apparent that brain regions damaged in H.M. other than the hippocampus are still largely underresearched and further studies are needed to address this. By also taking into account the advice of Gaffan (2005) to not assign a specific cognitive function to a specific brain region, but instead investigate the role of cortical networks, a greater understanding of organic amnesia will be achieved. While the development of new techniques, alongside novel research perspectives have been partially successful in overcoming the historical focus on the hippocampus, further research is required in order to overcome this incessant bias in the literature. In turn, this should lead to improved treatments, such as better methods of rehabilitation for patients with amnesia following stroke or traumatic brain injury, and will ultimately lead to a greater understanding of the psychological and neurological mechanisms of amnesia. 5

References Aggleton, J., & Brown, M. (1999). Episodic memory, amnesia, and the hippocampalanterior thalamic axis. Behavioural and Brain Sciences, 22 (3), 425-444. Aggleton, J., Brown, M., Bachevalier, J., Parkinson, J., Warburton, E., Corkin, S.,... & Witztum, E. (2013). Understanding amnesia: Is it time to forget HM? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 425-466. Aggleton, J., McMackin, D., Carpenter, K., Hornak, J., Kapur, N., Halpin, S.,... & Gaffan, D. (2000). Differential cognitive effects of colloid cysts in the third ventricle that spare or compromise the fornix. Brain, 123 (4), 800-815. Aggleton, J., Vann, S., Denby, C., Dix, S., Mayes, A., Roberts, N., & Yonelinas, A. (2005). Sparing of the familiarity component of recognition memory in a patient with hippocampal pathology. Neuropsychologia, 43 (12), 1810-1823. Allen, T., & Fortin, N. (2013). The evolution of episodic memory. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110 (2), 10379-10386. Annese, J., Schenker-Ahmed, N., Bartsch, H., Maechler, P., Sheh, C., Thomas, N.,... & Corkin, S. (2014). Postmortem examination of patient HM s brain based on histological sectioning and digital 3D reconstruction. Nature communications, 5, doi:10.1038/ncomms4122 Bachevalier, J., & Meunier, M. (1996). Cerebral ischemia: Are the memory deficits associated with hippocampal cell loss? Hippocampus, 6 (5), 553-560. Baxter, M., & Murray, E.(2001). Effects of hippocampal lesions on delayed nonmatchingto-sample in monkeys: A reply to Zola and Squire (2001). Hippocampus, 11 (3), 201-203. Brown, M., & Aggleton, J. (2001). Recognition memory: What are the roles of the perirhinal cortex and hippocampus? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2 (1), 51-61. Brown, M., Warburton, E., & Aggleton, J. (2010). Recognition memory: Material, processes, and substrates. Hippocampus, 20 (11), 1228-1244. Bussey, T. (2004). Multiple memory systems: Fact or fiction? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section B. 57 (1), 89-94. Butters, N., & Cermak, L. (1988). A case study of the forgetting of autobiographical knowledge: Implications for the study of retrograde amnesia. In D. Rubin (Ed.), Autobiographical memory (pp. 253-272). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press. 6

Carlesimo, G., Lombardi, M., & Caltagirone, C. (2011). Vascular thalamic amnesia: A reappraisal. Neuropsychologia, 49 (5), 777-789. Cohen, N., & Eichenbaum, H. (1993). Memory, amnesia, and the hippocampal system. Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press. Corkin, S., Amaral, D., Gonzlez, R., Johnson, K., & Hyman, B. (1997). HM s medial temporal lobe lesion: Findings from magnetic resonance imaging. The Journal of Neuroscience, 17 (10), 3964-3979. Easton, A., Fitchett, A., Eacott, M., & Baxter, M. (2011). Medial septal cholinergic neurons are necessary for context-place memory but not episodic-like memory. Hippocampus, 21 (9), 1021-1027. Easton, A., & Parker, A. (2003). A cholinergic explanation of dense amnesia. Cortex, 39 (4), 813-826. Easton, A., Ridley, R., Baker, H., & Gaffan, D. (2002). Unilateral lesions of the cholinergic basal forebrain and fornix in one hemisphere and inferior temporal cortex in the opposite hemisphere produce severe learning impairments in rhesus monkeys. Cerebral Cortex, 12 (7), 729-736. Eichnbaum, H. (2013). What HM taught us. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 25 (1), 14-21. Gaffan, D. (2005). Widespread cortical networks underlie memory and attention. Science, 309 (5744), 2172-2173. Gaffan, D., Parker, A., & Easton, A. (2001). Dense amnesia in the monkey after transection of fornix, amygdala and anterior temporal stem. Neuropsychologia, 39 (1), 51-70. Horel, J. A. (1978). The neuroanatomy of amnesia: A critique of the hippocampal memory hypothesis. Brain, 101 (4), 403-445. Maguire, E., & Mullally, S. (2013). The hippocampus: A manifesto for change. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 142 (4), 1180. Mahut, H. (1971). Spatial and object reversal learning in monkeys with partial temporal lobe ablations. Neuropsychologia, 9 (4), 409-424. Markowitsch, H., Weber-Luxemburger, G., Ewald, K., Kessler, J., & Heiss, W. (1997). Patients with heart attacks are not valid models for medial temporal lobe amnesia. A neuropsychological and FDG -PET study with consequences for memory research. European Journal of Neurology, 4 (2), 178-184. 7

Meunier, M., Bachevalier, J., Mishkin, M., & Murray, E. (1993). Effects on visual recognition of combined and separate ablations of the entorhinal and perirhinal cortex in rhesus monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience, 13 (12), 5418-5432. Mishkin, M. (1978). Memory in monkeys severely impaired by combined but not by separate removal of amygdala and hippocampus. Nature, 273, 297-298. Murray, E., & Mishkin, M.(1986). Visual recognition in monkeys following rhinal cortical ablations combined with either amygdalectomy or hippocampectomy. The Journal of Neuroscience, 6 (7), 1991-2003. Pergola, G., Ranft, A., Mathias, K., & Suchan, B. (2013). The role of the thalamic nuclei in recognition memory accompanied by recall during encoding and retrieval: An fmri study. Neuroimage, 74, 195-208. Pergola, G., & Suchan, B. (2013). Associative learning beyond the medial temporal lobe: Many actors on the memory stage. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00162 Redish, A. D. (2001). The hippocampal debate: Are we asking the right questions? Behavioural Brain Research, 127 (1), 81-98. Rempel-Clower, N., Zola, S., Squire, L., & Amaral, D. (1996). Three cases of enduring memory impairment after bilateral damage limited to the hippocampal formation. The Journal of Neuroscience, 16 (16), 5233-5255. Rubin, D., & Greenberg, D. (1998). Visual memory-deficit amnesia: A distinct amnesic presentation and etiology. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95 (9), 5413-5416. Scoville, W. B., & Milner, B. (1957). Loss of recent memory after bilateral hippocampal lesions. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 20 (1), 11-21. Smith, M., Auer, R., & Siesj, B. (1984). The density and distribution of ischemic brain injury in the rat following 2-10 min of forebrain ischemia. Acta Neuropathologica, 64 (4), 319-332. Spiers, H., Maguire, E., & Burgess, N. (2001). Hippocampal amnesia. Neurocase, 7 (5), 357-382. Squire, L., & Wixted, J. (2011). The cognitive neuroscience of human memory since H.M. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 34, 259. Squire, L., Wixted, J., & Clark, R. (2007). Recognition memory and the medial temporal lobe: A new perspective. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8 (11), 872-883. 8

Turchi, J., Saunders, R., & Mishkin, M. (2005). Effects of cholinergic deafferentation of the rhinal cortex on visual recognition memory in monkeys. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102 (6), 2158-2161. Vanderwolf, C., & Cain, D. (1994). The behavioral neurobiology of learning and memory: A conceptual reorientation. Brain Research Reviews, 19 (3), 264-297. Vann, S. (2010). Re-evaluating the role of the mammillary bodies in memory. Neuropsychologia, 48 (8), 2316-2327. Warburton, E., & Brown, M.(2010). Findings from animals concerning when interactions between perirhinal cortex, hippocampus and medial prefrontal cortex are necessary for recognition memory. Neuropsychologia, 48 (8), 2262-2272. Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L., & Amaral, D. (1986). Human amnesia and the medial temporal region: Enduring memory impairment following a bilateral lesion limited to field CA1 of the hippocampus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 6 (10), 2950-2967. Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L., & Mishkin, M. (1982). The neuroanatomy of amnesia: Amygdala-hippocampus versus temporal stem. Science, 218 (4579), 1337-1339. Zola-Morgan, S., Squire, L., & Ramus, S. (1994). Severity of memory impairment in monkeys as a function of locus and extent of damage within the medial temporal lobe memory system. Hippocampus, 4 (4), 483-495. 9